Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Physiol., 10 January 2019
Sec. Exercise Physiology
This article is part of the Research Topic Fatigability and Motor Performance in Special and Clinical Populations View all 13 articles

Self-Reported Fatigue After Mild Traumatic Brain Injury Is Not Associated With Performance Fatigability During a Sustained Maximal Contraction

  • 1Department of Biomedical Sciences of Cells and Systems, University Medical Center Groningen, University of Groningen, Groningen, Netherlands
  • 2Department of Neurology, University Medical Center Groningen, University of Groningen, Groningen, Netherlands

Patients with mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI) are frequently affected by fatigue. However, hardly any data is available on the fatigability of the motor system. We evaluated fatigue using the Fatigue Severity Scale (FSS) and Modified Fatigue Impact Scale (MFIS) questionnaires in 20 participants with mTBI (>3 months post injury; 8 females) and 20 age- and sex matched controls. Furthermore, index finger abduction force and electromyography of the first dorsal interosseous muscle of the right hand were measured during brief and sustained maximal voluntary contractions (MVC). Double pulse stimulation (100 Hz) was applied to the ulnar nerve to evoke doublet-forces before and after the sustained contraction. Seven superimposed twitches were evoked during the sustained MVC to quantify voluntary muscle activation. mTBI participants reported higher FSS scores (mTBI: 5.2 ± 0.8 SD vs. control: 2.8 ± 0.8 SD; P < 0.01). During the sustained MVC, force declined to similar levels in mTBI (30.0 ± 9.9% MVC) and control participants (32.7 ± 9.8% MVC, P = 0.37). The decline in doublet-forces after the sustained MVC (mTBI: to 37.2 ± 12.1 vs. control: to 41.4 ± 14.0% reference doublet, P = 0.32) and the superimposed twitches evoked during the sustained MVC (mTBI: median 9.3, range: 2.2–32.9 vs. control: median 10.3, range: 1.9–31.0% doubletpre, P = 0.34) also did not differ between groups. Force decline was associated with decline in doublet-force (R2 = 0.50, P < 0.01) for both groups. Including a measure of voluntary muscle activation resulted in more explained variance for mTBI participants only. No associations between self-reported fatigue and force decline or voluntary muscle activation were found in mTBI participants. However, the physical subdomain of the MFIS was associated with the decline in doublet-force after the sustained MVC (R2 = 0.23, P = 0.04). These results indicate that after mTBI, increased levels of self-reported physical fatigue reflected increased fatigability due to changes in peripheral muscle properties, but not force decline or muscle activation. Additionally, muscle activation was more important to explain the decline in voluntary force (performance fatigability) after mTBI than in control participants.

Introduction

Traumatic brain injury is a serious health problem and one of the most important causes of impairment in adults; most cases sustain a mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI; Cassidy et al., 2014; Levin and Diaz-Arrastia, 2015). Despite the fact that most patients recover from mTBI, 15–20% of patients have persistent symptoms (Cassidy et al., 2014; Levin and Diaz-Arrastia, 2015) and fatigue is among the most frequently occurring complaints (van der Naalt et al., 1999; Lundin et al., 2006; Lannsjö et al., 2009; Mollayeva et al., 2014). Fatigue is a disabling symptom because it impairs physical and social functioning, and it impedes return to work (van der Naalt et al., 1999, 2017; Stulemeijer et al., 2006; de Koning et al., 2017).

Nowadays, the term fatigue is most often reserved for the self-reported symptom which can be evaluated using questionnaires. According to the taxonomy proposed by Kluger and colleagues, fatigue is mediated by two attributes (Kluger et al., 2013). The first attribute, perceived fatigability, corresponds to changes in subjective sensations occurring at rest or in the context of task performance and derives from psychological and homeostatic factors (Enoka and Duchateau, 2016). The second attribute, performance fatigability, relates to impaired task performance and is defined as the decline in an objective measure of performance over a discrete period of time (e.g., a decline in voluntary force or power during sustained contractions; Enoka and Duchateau, 2016). Although perceived and performance fatigability are distinct qualities they can interact.

So far, most studies investigating fatigue following mTBI have focussed on the relationship between self-reported fatigue and cognitive task performance (Möller et al., 2014, 2017; Nordin et al., 2016; Wylie and Flashman, 2017). Although the impact of mTBI on cognitive parameters is prominent, few groups also report (long-lasting) impairments in motor parameters including: a small reduction in voluntary drive (Powers et al., 2014), inhibitory changes in the primary motor cortex (De Beaumont et al., 2009), and reduced walking speed in a dual-task condition (Yasen et al., 2017). However, no data is available on the fatigability of the motor system in mTBI.

Another neurological patient population suffering from fatigue is multiple sclerosis (MS). Previous studies by our lab showed that self-reported fatigue in persons with MS was associated with measures of performance fatigability during a sustained maximal contraction These studies provided the rationale and experimental framework to investigate fatigue following mTBI (Steens et al., 2012; Wolkorte et al., 2015a). Although differences in the pathophysiology of MS and mTBI are evident, the conditions have aspects in common including neuroinflammation and loss of white matter integrity (Compston and Coles, 2008; Andriessen et al., 2010; Johnson et al., 2013; Tremblay, 2014; Armstrong et al., 2016). Since white matter integrity is essential for maintaining optimal activation of motoneurons during voluntary muscle contractions, we expect that mTBI will result in reduced voluntary activation of muscle fibers and affect measures of performance fatigability.

Therefore, it was the aim of the present study to assess the neuromuscular factors contributing to performance fatigability after mTBI, and to appraise these findings in the context of the increased levels of self-reported fatigue that occur post-injury. We hypothesize that mTBI may impair voluntary drive, thereby reducing muscle activation and leading to increased performance fatigability. Also, that this change might partially explain the increased levels of self-reported fatigue following mTBI. Furthermore, by investigating fatigue and fatigability using the same paradigm that was previously used in MS, we hope to increase the understanding of fatigue in neurological populations.

Materials and Methods

Study Population

Twenty-two patients with mTBI and 22 age- and sex-matched control participants were included. mTBI was defined by a Glasgow coma scale score between 13–15 on admission, with posttraumatic amnesia less than 24 h and/or loss of consciousness of less than 30 min. Inclusion criteria included persistent complaints of (self-reported) fatigue for more than 3 months post injury and adequate hand dexterity (to perform the motor tasks). Exclusion criteria included psychiatric disorder, neurologic disease (including previous TBI), and drug or alcohol abuse. Data from the control subjects were also used in an accompanying study (Sars et al., 2018). Experiments were designed in accordance with the declaration of Helsinki (World Medical Association, 2013) and approval of the experimental procedures was provided by the medical ethical board of the University Medical Center Groningen. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants before the experiment.

Questionnaires and Cognitive Testing

For the mTBI patients, self-reported fatigue was quantified using two questionnaires: the Fatigue Severity Scale (FSS; Krupp et al., 1989) and the Modified Fatigue Impact Scale (MFIS; Multiple Sclerosis Council for Clinical Practice Guidelines, 1998; Schiehser et al., 2015). Mood was evaluated using the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS; Zigmond and Snaith, 1983). Cognitive impairment and attentional processing were assessed in mTBI participants using the written form of the Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT; Smith, 1982) and the 3 s Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT’3; Gronwall, 1977; McCauley et al., 2014), respectively. Handedness was evaluated using the Edinburgh inventory (Oldfield, 1971).

Force Recording

Index finger abduction force was measured using hand-held transducers (van Duinen et al., 2007; Figure 1A). Participants held the transducers with their index fingers extended. The horizontal bar of the transducer was aligned parallel to the index finger and the finger bracket was positioned over the proximal interphalangeal joint. The thumb was taped to digits III-IV to maintain this hand position throughout the experiment. Force signals was sampled at 500 Hz using a 1401 interface and recorded on a computer with Spike 2 software (version 7.12, Cambridge Electronic Design, Cambridge, United Kingdom).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Experimental setup and an example of raw data. (A) Photograph showing the hands a participant equipped with force transducers. The finger bracket is positioned over the proximal interphalangeal joint of the index fingers as can be seen for the left hand. Placement of EMG electrodes over the FDI can be seen for the left hand and electrodes used for electrical stimulation can be seen on the right wrist. (B–D) Panels showing raw data recorded in a male mTBI participant during tasks 1–3, respectively. From top to bottom each panel shows the time-points of electrical nerve stimulation, index finger abduction force of the right hand, EMG of the right FDI, force of the left hand, and EMG of the left FDI. MVC, maximal voluntary contraction; FDI, first dorsal interosseous.

Electromyography

Surface EMG was recorded from the first dorsal interosseous (FDI) of both hands using sintered silver/silver chloride electrodes (In Vivo Metric, Healdsburg, United States). After cleaning the skin with alcohol, one electrode was applied over the muscle belly of each muscle and the second electrode was applied over the adjacent metacarpophalangeal joint. A reference electrode was applied over the right wrist. EMG signals were amplified (200 V/V) using custom-made amplifiers, band-pass filtered (10–1000 Hz), and sampled (at 2000 Hz) using the same interface as the force signals.

Electrical Nerve Stimulation

Voluntary muscle activation of the right FDI was determined using the interpolated twitch technique (Merton, 1954; Behm et al., 1996; Gandevia, 2001). The ulnar nerve was electrically stimulated using a constant-current stimulator (pulse width 200 μs; DS7A, Digitimer, Welwyn Garden City, United Kingdom) via a pair of self-adhesive electrodes applied over the ulnar nerve close to right wrist. Stimulation intensity was set to ≥130% of the intensity required to evoke a maximal M-wave in the FDI (range: 39–100 mA). During the experimental tasks, forces were evoked using double pulses (10 ms interval) to improve the signal-to-noise ratio (Gandevia and McKenzie, 1988). The forces evoked at rest are referred to as doublet-force; to comply with previous literature, we use the term superimposed twitch (SIT) for the forces evoked during voluntary contractions.

Motor Tasks

Participants performed three isometric motor tasks in which they were asked to abduct their index finger. Participants were seated behind a desk and could see their force production on a monitor in front of them during the tasks (see Figure 1).

Task 1 Maximal Voluntary Contraction (MVC)

Participants generated three maximal isometric index finger abductions (10 s) with the left hand and four with the right hand, alternating between hands. Contractions were followed by 50 s rest between subsequent contractions. A superimposed twitch was evoked during at least two maximal contractions with the right hand (∼3 s after the start of the contraction) and three doublet-forces were evoked at rest after the last contraction (2 s interval) to obtain potentiated doublet-forces.

Task 2 Submaximal Contractions

To determine differences in voluntary activation with a different method we also evoked superimposed twitches during submaximal contractions (De Serres and Enoka, 1998). Participants had to match their force level to a horizontal line indicating target force at 10, 30, 50, and 70% MVC. Three blocks were performed with each hand, alternating between the left and right hand (12 contractions per hand). Each contraction was sustained for 10 s with 40 s rest between contractions. A superimposed twitch was evoked during at least two sets of four contractions with the right hand.

Task 3 Sustained MVC

Participants generated a brief maximal index finger abduction for 6 s followed by 60 s rest. Next, participants generated a sustained MVC lasting 124 s. Superimposed twitches were evoked once during the brief MVC and at seven time points during the sustained MVC (18 s interval). Three doublet-forces (2 s interval) were evoked at rest after the brief MVC (doubletpre) and after the sustained MVC (doubletpost).

Analysis

Questionnaire and cognitive test scores were calculated. PASAT scores below the 5th percentile of the normative scores (i.e., <32/60 for ≤12 years of education and <35/60 for >12 years of education) were indicative of impairment (Rao et al., 1991). SDMT scores were expressed as a T-score of the normative data matched for age and education (Smith, 1982). FSS scores were indicative of fatigue if a participants scored greater than the cut-off of 4.5 (Flachenecker et al., 2002), and greater than 29 in the case of MFIS total scores (Schiehser et al., 2015).

Force and EMG data were processed in Spike 2; EMG signals were transformed by calculating the root mean square (rms) over a moving window of 500 ms. Maximal force and rms-EMG were determined for the MVCs. The largest of the three potentiated doublet-forces evoked at rest was taken as the reference value and the superimposed twitches evoked during the maximal and submaximal contractions in task 1 and 2 were expressed as a percentage of this force.

During the sustained MVC, force and rms-EMG at the start and the end of the sustained contraction were average over 6 s and expressed as a percentage of maximal force and rms-EMG, respectively. Furthermore, force and rms-EMG during the sustained MVC were averaged over 2 s epochs (62 epochs in total, the 7 epochs which overlapped the electrical stimulation were excluded from statistical analysis), and normalized to maximal force and rms-EMG obtained during the MVC (task 1). The coefficient of variation (CVforce) for the right index finger abduction force was calculated by dividing the standard deviation during the 2 s epoch by the mean force. The superimposed twitches during the sustained MVC were linearly corrected for the decline in muscle force (due to factors within the muscle) using the following formula (Schillings et al., 2003):

Corrected SIT at time t=SITtdoubletpre(timet/124s)×(doubletpredoubletpost)×100%

with t as the time of stimulation, and doubletpre and doubletpost as the doublet-force prior to and after the sustained contraction, respectively.

Statistical Analysis

Statistical analysis was performed in R studio (R version 3.4.3). Normality was assessed graphically using quantile–quantile plots. Group comparisons of questionnaire scores and motor task parameters were performed by ANOVA with sex included as covariate in the analysis of motor parameters in order to better explain the remaining variance. Non-normally distributed data were transformed to obtain a normal distribution of the ANOVA residuals (Prescott, 2018). If transformation could not achieve a normal distribution of the model residuals, Mann–Whitney U tests were used. Values in the text are shown as mean (SD) for normally distributed data or median (range) for non-normally distributed data.

Multilevel modeling was used to analyze the superimposed twitches during the submaximal contractions (task 2). Pre-planned statistical models were used to describe the relationship between the size of the SITs and the force level at the time of stimulation. Next, group was added as a fixed effect to assess differences between mTBI and control participants, followed by sex.

To describe the changes over time, multilevel modeling was performed for the force, rms-EMG, and CVforce during the sustained MVC. Pre-planned statistical models were used to describe these time-related changes. We started with a simple model in which the intercept was allowed to vary randomly per participant. More complex models were constructed including time and the 2nd and 3rd degree polynomials of time as fixed effects, as well as random slopes for each of these variables per participant. After each step, statistical analysis determined whether the more complex model survived; the more complex model explained the data significantly better than the previous model if the Akaike information criterion (AIC) decreased by at least 2. For all parameters (force, rms-EMG, and CVforce) the models including (2nd and 3rd) polynomials of time and random intercepts and slopes explained significantly more variance. Therefore, in the results section we only describe whether the more complex models in which group, sex, and interactions were added explained (significantly) more variance than this basic model. Next, group (mTBI) was added to the basic model as a fixed effect. Since sex and age can affect the time-related changes in force and EMG (Sars et al., 2018), we subsequently included these variables and their interactions to the model in a stepwise fashion. Model residuals were examined graphically for normality and heteroscedasticity (using quantile-quantile plots and heteroscedasticity plots) for all multilevel models. If required, the dependent variable was transformed to meet these criteria. Finally, robustness of the final models was tested by re-estimating the model on a trimmed dataset. This was done by first identifying the data points with scaled residuals greater than 2 for the corresponding model. These data points (model outliers) were then removed from the dataset and the model was re-estimated.

Associations between self-reported fatigue and the measures of performance and perceived fatigability were examined in mTBI participants. For this analysis, the MVC of the mTBI participants was expressed as a Z-score (calculated using the mean and standard deviation of MVCs in the control group, per sex). Associations were analyzed between force decline, voluntary muscle activation, and the change in doublet-force after the sustained MVC. Associations between two nominal variables were analyzed using Spearman’s rank test (e.g., FSS and HADS scores). Linear regression was used for the associations between continuous variables and for multivariate analyses. Model residuals were examined for normal distribution using quantile-quantile plots.

Results

One mTBI participant had problems performing stable MVCs and the SIT deviated more than three SD from the mean. Another mTBI participant did not perform the sustained contraction adequately and showed progressively increasing force during the first minute. These two participants were excluded from all analyses. To maintain balanced groups, the two matched control participants were also excluded. The mean age of the remaining mTBI participants was 40.1 years (range: 23–56, 8 females) and 41.1 years in controls (range: 21–59, 8 females). One mTBI participant stopped contracting during the sustained MVC after 84 s. The data up to this time point were included in the analysis, however, this participant was excluded from all analyses regarding associations of self-reported fatigue and performance fatigability (we also checked whether inclusion of this subject affected our conclusion). Demographics and injury characteristics of the mTBI participants are shown in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographics and injury characteristics of mTBI participants.

Questionnaires

Participants with mTBI reported higher levels of fatigue and depression. The FSS scores were increased in mTBI compared to controls (5.2 ± 0.8 SD vs. 2.7 ± 0.8 SD, P < 0.01), and 15 out of the 20 mTBI participants scored greater than the cut-off of 4.5 indicative of fatigue (Flachenecker et al., 2002). The median MFIS score reported by the mTBI participants was 51 (range: 25–71); all but one participant scoring above the cut-off score of 29 for fatigue (Schiehser et al., 2015). Scores on the MFIS subdomains are shown in Table 2. Furthermore, scores on the depression domain of the HADS were higher in mTBI (median: 7, range: 2–14) than in control participants (median: 0, range: 0–2, P < 0.01).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Questionnaire scores.

Cognitive Tests

The median score on the PASAT’3 was 49.5/60 correct responses (range: 31–59). One mTBI participant scored below the 5th percentile cut-off (i.e., <32/60 correct responses for an individual with <12 years of education; Rao et al., 1991). The median score on the SDMT was 51.5 (range: 36–72). Data were expressed as a T-score, corrected for age and education. The mean T-score was 41.7 (range: 30–75; Smith, 1982). Four participants had T-scores below the normal range (≤40); one of these participants fell in the category ‘very low’ (T-score ≤ 30).

Baseline Measurements

Index finger abduction MVCs of the right hand did not differ between mTBI participants (40.5 ± 8.6 N) and controls (38.0 ± 11.2 N, F1,37 = 0.97, P = 0.33; with sex included as a covariate, see Table 3). Similar outcomes were observed for the left hand (Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Motor parameters.

The potentiated doublet-forces at rest were also not different between mTBI (12.9 ± 2.4 N) and controls (11.1 ± 3.6 N, F1,37 = 3.91, P = 0.06, with sex included as a covariate, Table 3). The superimposed twitches during the brief MVC were also similar for the two groups (mTBI median: 3.9, range: 0.0–17.3 vs. control median: 2.8, range: 0.9–15.4% potentiated doublet-force, F1,38 = 0.08, P = 0.78; after logarithmic transformation. See Figure 2A).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. (A) Dot plot showing the size of the superimposed twitches during the maximal voluntary contractions in task 1, values are rounded to the nearest 0.5%. The horizontal line indicates the median for the control participants (blue dots) and mTBI participants (red dots). (B) Superimposed twitches evoked during the submaximal contractions (at 10, 30, 50, and 70% MVC) in task 2. Control data is shown in blue, mTBI in red. A linear regression line is shown for both groups, but no significant differences were observed between control and mTBI participants. SIT, superimposed twitch.

Voluntary Muscle Activation During the Submaximal Contractions

The superimposed twitches during the submaximal contractions were modeled using a multilevel model. The size of the superimposed twitches could be explained by a model including background force as fixed factor (β = -1.09, t = -42.18, P < 0.01) as well as random intercepts and slopes for force per participant. The model did not show significant improvement after including group or sex as fixed-factors. Thus, the twitch becomes on average 10.9% smaller when the voluntary background force increases with 10% MVC but we found no differences in the evoked twitches between controls and mTBI (see Figure 2B).

Voluntary Force and rms-EMG During the Sustained MVC

During the sustained contraction the force during the first 6 s (control: 81.8 ± 7.7 vs. mTBI: 80.8 ± 8.9% MVC) and last 6 s (control: 32.7 ± 9.8 vs. mTBI: 30.0 ± 9.9% MVC) did not differ between groups (Table 3). A difference was observed, however, between males and females (F1,36 = 7.57, P < 0.01) indicating a larger force decline in male participants (Table 3).

Instead of only using the data obtained at start and the end of the sustained contraction, we modeled the time course of the force. Adding group or sex to the basic model (see section “Materials and Methods”) did not result in a significant model improvement (group: Δ AIC = -1.9, sex: Δ AIC = -0.2). However, the model improved after including the interaction between sex and time (sex time: β = 4.968, t = -2.20, P = 0.03; Δ AIC = -2.7). No interaction was observed for time group. These results demonstrate that male participants showed a larger force decline over time, without a difference between mTBI and control participants (see Figures 3A,B).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Time course of the force and rms-EMG of the right hand during the sustained MVC. For all panels, control data (n = 20) is shown in blue and mTBI (n = 20) in red. (A,B) Force (averaged over 2 s epochs) during the sustained MVC in male and female participants. (C,D) Mean rms-EMG (averaged over 2 s epochs) in male and female participants. Error bars in (A–D) show the standard deviation.

The model explaining the variance of the rms-EMG of the right FDI did not improve after including group (Δ AIC = 2.0). However, the basic model improved significantly after including sex (Δ AIC = -2.6; sex: β = 2.10, t = 2.28, P = 0.03), indicating that rms-EMG was lower in male participants (see Figures 3C,D). Next, a significant interaction was found for sex group, but including this interaction did not significantly improve the model (t = -2.62, P = 0.01; Δ AIC = -1.9).

Finally, the coefficient of variation of the force of the right hand (CVforce) was examined. To obtain a normal distribution of the residuals, CVforce was transformed using a power transformation (λ = 0.1). Including group, sex, and age in the basic model did not significantly improve the model. Thus, CVforce increased over time but no differences were found between groups or sex.

During the sustained contraction the contralateral, non-target FDI also becomes (unintentionally) active (Zijdewind and Kernell, 2001). Both contralateral force and rms-EMG were log transformed to improve distribution of the residuals. Both parameters increased over time and were best fit by the basic model, but no differences were observed between groups or sex.

Because one participants stopped the sustained MVC after 84 s, the multilevel analyses were repeated without this participant. This did not affect the conclusion for any of the parameters.

Voluntary Muscle Activation During the Sustained Contraction

The time-course of the superimposed twitches before and during the sustained MVC) was modeled using a multilevel model. SITs were transformed by taking the square root. The model included time as fixed-factor (time: β = 0.99, t = 4.31, P < 0.01) and random intercept and slope for time per participant. The model indicates an increase in the size of the superimposed twitches over the course of the sustained contraction (i.e., a decrease in voluntary muscle activation), and no differences between mTBI and control participants (Figure 4). The mean of the seven superimposed twitches during the sustained MVC did not differ significantly between mTBI (median: 9.3 range: 2.2–32.9, n = 19) and control participants (median: 10.3, range: 1.9–31.0% doubletpre, F1,37 = 0.92, p = 0.34; after square root transformation of the mean SIT).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Time course of the superimposed twitches during the sustained MVC. Dots show the mean size of the superimposed twitches evoked before (t = 0 s) and during the sustained MVC (t = 7–115 s). Control data (n = 20) is shown in blue and mTBI (n = 20) in red. Error bars indicate the standard deviation. Superimposed twitches were linearly corrected for the decline in muscle force and expressed as a percentage of the doubletpre.

Doublet-Forces After the Sustained Contraction

The doublet-force evoked after the sustained contraction declined in mTBI (to 37.2 ± 12.1% reference doublet-force, n = 19) and control participants (to 41.4 ± 14.0% reference doublet-force) but did not differ between groups (F1,36 = 1.03, P = 0.32; with sex included as a covariate).

Associations Between Self-Reported Fatigue and Measures of Fatigability

Since mTBI participants reported significantly higher FSS scores than control participants, we analyzed associations between these questionnaires with different measures of fatigability for the mTBI participants only.

In mTBI participants (n = 20), no significant relationship was observed between the FSS scores and the HADS depression scores (P = 0.13). MFIS scores were, however, significantly correlated with HADS depression scores (rho = 0.73, P < 0.01); the association between HADS was also observed for the physical subdomain of the MFIS (rho = 0.70, P < 0.01), as well as the cognitive subdomain (rho = 0.52, P = 0.02). FSS or MFIS scores did not show significant associations with the raw PASAT or SDMT scores.

Linear regression of FSS or MFIS with force decline and MVC Z-scores (conform Steens et al., 2012) did not produce a significant association (FSS: P = 0.29; MFIS: P = 0.92, n = 19). However, a significant negative association was observed between the MFIS physical subscale and the decline in doublet-force (R2 = 0.23, P = 0.04, n = 19; Figure 5).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Association between scores on the physical subdomain of the MFIS and the decline in the evoked doublet-force after the sustained MVC (i.e., doubletpost). Male mTBI participants (n = 11) are shown in green, female mTBI participants (n = 8) are shown in purple.

Associations With Force Decline

Force decline (n = 39) during the sustained contraction (i.e., force during last 6 s/first 6 s) was negatively associated with the MVC (R2 = 0.17, P < 0.01) and positively associated with the decline in doublet-force (R2 = 0.50, P < 0.01; Figure 6). Force decline was not associated with voluntary muscle activation during the sustained MVC (i.e., mean SIT; P = 0.10). Analyzing force decline for the mTBI (n = 19) and control groups (n = 20) separately, the decline in doublet-force was able to explain more variance in control (R2 = 0.53, P < 0.01) than in mTBI participants (R2 = 0.45, P < 0.01). However, in mTBI participants significantly more variance (P = 0.03) could be explained by including voluntary muscle activation (mean SIT) in the model together with the decline in doublet-force (R2 = 0.59, P < 0.01; mean SIT: β = -4.03, P = 0.03, doubletpost: β = 0.73, P < 0.01). Including the voluntary muscle activation was not able to improve the model in controls.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Association between force decline during the sustained MVC and the evoked doublet-force after the sustained contraction (i.e., doubletpost) showing that participants with greater force decline also showed a greater decline in electrically evoked force. Control participants (n = 20) are shown in blue and mTBI (n = 19) in red.

Discussion

Consistent with literature (van der Naalt et al., 1999; Lundin et al., 2006; Stulemeijer et al., 2006; Lannsjö et al., 2009), participants with mTBI reported increased levels of fatigue and depression at more than 3 months post injury compared to control participants. No force or EMG differences were observed between groups during the sustained contraction reflecting similar levels of performance fatigability. Although individual differences in force decline could best be explained by changes in muscle properties (i.e., the decline in the doublet-force; see Figure 6), in mTBI participants the remaining variance could be explained by differences in voluntary muscle activation (i.e., mean SIT). Furthermore, (moderate) associations were found between scores on the physical subdomain of the MFIS questionnaire and the decline in doublet-force after the sustained contraction in mTBI participants.

Fatigue and Performance Fatigability After mTBI

As expected, participants with mTBI reported increased levels of fatigue at more than 3 months post injury (van der Naalt et al., 1999; Lundin et al., 2006; Stulemeijer et al., 2006; Lannsjö et al., 2009); indicated by both FSS and MFIS scores. However, contrary to our expectations, performance fatigability in mTBI participants did not differ from age- and sex-matched controls.

The injury mechanism of mTBI has been well described. Impact-acceleration forces cause axonal injury and white matter tracts of the anterior corona radiata and corpus callosum are among the regions which are frequently damaged (Armstrong et al., 2016). Axon demyelination, as well as neuroinflammation with microglial activation develop post-injury (Andriessen et al., 2010; Armstrong et al., 2016). A post-mortem study using tissue from moderate-severely injured patients found that inflammation can persist for several years post-injury (Johnson et al., 2013). These findings are complemented by imaging studies in mTBI patients which have shown structural white-matter changes in addition to functional changes such as disruption of networks (Inglese et al., 2005; Eierud et al., 2014).

Force decline during a sustained maximal contraction provides a measure of performance fatigability (Enoka and Duchateau, 2016). This decline in voluntary force may occur due to changes in peripheral muscle properties; i.e., factors at and distal to the neuromuscular junction (Allen et al., 2008). In addition, muscle fibers can become less optimally activated by the central nervous system (Gandevia, 2001). The interpolated twitch technique can be used to provide an estimation of the portion of the muscle that is not maximally activated (Behm et al., 1996). Since maximal activation of muscle fibers requires optimal activation of the motoneuron pool we expected that voluntary muscle activation could be compromised after mTBI as the result of damage to white matter tracts and myelin modification that evolves post-injury (Inglese et al., 2005; Armstrong et al., 2016). This is especially important for hand muscles which receive a relatively large portion of synchronized input from the corticospinal tract (Day et al., 1989; Lemon, 2008; Keen et al., 2012). In a previous study, a small decline in voluntary muscle activation of the FDI was observed in the early stage following concussion (Powers et al., 2014). However, we did not find differences in force decline nor in voluntary muscle activation between mTBI and control participants in the present study.

Besides the force decline and voluntary muscle activation of the right FDI, we analyzed two other parameters as indirect measures of performance during the sustained MVC. Firstly, the coefficient of variation of the force of the right hand (CVforce) was used as a measure of force steadiness. Secondly, the force and EMG of the contralateral (left) hand were recorded and analyzed. Both measures increase during fatiguing contractions (Lippold, 1981; Zijdewind and Kernell, 2001; Shinohara et al., 2003) and reflect increased effort. Neither parameter showed a difference between mTBI and control participants that could indicate increased effort in the mTBI group.

In both groups, individual differences in force decline during the sustained MVC were best explained by changes in peripheral muscle properties (i.e., the decline in doublet-force after the contraction; see Figure 6). However, only in the mTBI group could the variation in voluntary muscle activation (i.e., mean SIT during the sustained contraction) explain the remaining variance of the force decline. Although we were not able to objectify differences in performance between the groups, this may suggest that maintaining voluntary drive could be difficult after mTBI. This idea is further supported by the task performance of the two excluded participants and one mTBI participant who all had profound difficulties in performing the sustained contraction.

The rationale for investigating performance fatigability in the context of increased self-reported fatigue came from previous findings in individuals with relapsing remitting MS (Steens et al., 2012; Wolkorte et al., 2015a). Although the severity of fatigue reported by mTBI participants was similar to the levels of fatigue in MS (Steens et al., 2012: FSS 5.3 ± 0.9 SD), associations between self-reported fatigue and reduced voluntary muscle activation or (normalized) force decline were not found in the present study. However, a (moderate) association between the physical subdomain of the MFIS and the decline in doublet-force after the sustained MVC was observed in mTBI participants. The associations between the decline in doublet-force with self-reported (physical) fatigue as well as with force decline underline the importance of peripheral muscle properties to explain variation in fatigue and performance fatigability in persons with mTBI.

Cognitive Impairment After mTBI

Cognitive impairment is a prominent complaint after mTBI (van der Naalt et al., 1999; Lundin et al., 2006; Lannsjö et al., 2009). In the present study we screened for cognitive impairment using the PASAT and SDMT. On both tests, performance of the mTBI participants was comparable to the reference population. It is possible that the 3 s PASAT task was not sensitive enough to capture subtle differences (Tombaugh, 2006), but also the SDMT showed low scores for only 20% of mTBI participants. Most of our mTBI participants (and controls) had completed a higher education program (see Table 1). Since higher education is known to attenuate changes in cognitive task performance due to TBI (Sumowski et al., 2013), it is possible that the relatively high level of education of our mTBI participants diminished the effects of the TBI on the cognitive (and perhaps also the motor) task.

Several studies have focused on the relationship between self-reported fatigue and performance during cognitive tasks; another example of performance fatigability (Ziino and Ponsford, 2006; Möller et al., 2014, 2017). For these tasks, also no associations between measures of self-reported fatigue and performance fatigability were observed.

Fatigue and Depression

It is known that fatigue and depression are often found in the same patients and that fatigue and depression scores are mostly well correlated (Kluger et al., 2013). In the present mTBI population we found increased values for both depression and fatigue scores. We found an association between the HADS depression scores and the MFIS scores, but not the FSS scores. Though, previous studies have also reported the association between HADS depression scores and FSS scores after mTBI (Norrie et al., 2010; Möller et al., 2014).

Force Measurements

Across all groups, the MVCs of the left hand were on average 20% larger than for the right hand. This difference has been reported before in our studies (e.g., Post et al., 2007) but not by other groups. Our force transducer is equipped with a full bridge strain gauge configuration and therefore records force independent of the position of the finger bracket (i.e., the connection between the index finger and the transducer; see Figure 1A; van Duinen et al., 2007). Furthermore, the force difference between the left and right hand remains present if the same force transducer is used for both hands. In other words, differences in the length of the index finger do not affect the force data and cannot explain the difference in force. In cadavers, we dissected three pairs of human hands to study the anatomical differences between the left and right FDI. No systematic differences were found between the left and right FDI for the mass, length, or cross sectional width; though the cross sectional width of the left FDI was larger in two pairs of specimens.

Limitations of the Study

The target muscle of the present study was the FDI, a small hand muscle. We chose this muscle because we wanted to be able to compare the present data with the data obtained in persons with MS (Steens et al., 2012). Furthermore, intrinsic hand muscles are more strongly dependent on synchronized corticospinal input compared to most other muscles (Keen et al., 2012). Therefore, we expected differences in voluntary muscle activation in this muscle. It is, however, possible that measures of performance fatigability would have shown larger differences in other larger muscle (groups).

Contrary to most research related to fatigue following mTBI, we used motor tasks to investigate performance fatigability. A general limitation of using cognitive tasks to measure time-related changes (i.e., performance fatigability) is that performance on these tasks is often affected by learning processes. Since cognitive and motor tasks share higher cortical processes as illustrated by the direct interaction between cognitive and motor task performance in both controls (Lorist et al., 2002; Wolkorte et al., 2014) and clinical populations (Howell et al., 2013; Sarajuuri et al., 2013; Wolkorte et al., 2015b; Yasen et al., 2017), we expected to find differences between mTBI patients and controls in our motor tasks. An advantage of motor tasks is the ability to quantify output (force) continuously throughout the task. Furthermore, by using the interpolated twitch technique (Merton, 1954; Allen et al., 1994; Behm et al., 1996) during a sustained maximal muscle contraction it is possible to discriminate between factors residing in the muscle and factors in the central nervous system which are responsible for force decline. Nevertheless, the present study only found minor differences in motor parameters between controls and the mTBI population. It is possible that our outcomes measures were not sensitive enough and that a combination of cognitive and motor task would require more attentional resources and therefore induce larger differences.

Conclusion

Fatigue is an important symptom after mTBI, but the underlying mechanisms remain poorly understood. Despite increased levels of self-reported fatigue, no differences were observed in force decline in persons with mTBI. Nevertheless, the present data indicate that changes in peripheral muscle properties could be important for understanding the mechanisms affecting perception of fatigue in persons with mTBI. These peripheral muscle properties together with changes in voluntary muscle activation were able to explain most of the variation in force decline during the sustained maximal contraction. Besides relevance for improving our understanding of fatigue, these findings also indicate the importance of studying a combination of motor and cognitive parameters after mTBI.

Author Contributions

IZ and JvdN designed the experiments. RP and IZ performed the experiments and analyzed the data. RP, IZ, and JvdN wrote the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by Junior Scientific Masterclass (JSM) of the UMCG to RP.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Tjalling Nijboer for his technical support and Nina Oldejans for helped in performing the experiments. We also thank all our participants who volunteered to take part in this study.

References

Allen, D. G., Lamb, G. D., and Westerblad, H. (2008). Skeletal muscle fatigue: cellular mechanisms. Physiol. Rev. 88, 287–332. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00015.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allen, G. M., Gandevia, S. C., Neering, I. R., Hickie, I., Jones, R., and Middleton, J. (1994). Muscle performance, voluntary activation and perceived effort in normal subjects and patients with prior poliomyelitis. Brain 117, 661–670. doi: 10.1093/brain/117.4.661

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andriessen, T. M. J. C., Jacobs, B., and Vos, P. E. (2010). Clinical characteristics and pathophysiological mechanisms of focal and diffuse traumatic brain injury. J. Cell. Mol. Med. 14, 2381–2392. doi: 10.1111/j.1582-4934.2010.01164.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Armstrong, R. C., Mierzwa, A. J., Marion, C. M., and Sullivan, G. M. (2016). White matter involvement after TBI: clues to axon and myelin repair capacity. Exp. Neurol. 275, 328–333. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2015.02.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Behm, D. G., St-Pierre, D. M. M., and Perez, D. (1996). Muscle inactivation: assessment of interpolated twitch technique. J. Appl. Physiol. 81, 2267–2273. doi: 10.1152/jappl.1996.81.5.2267

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cassidy, J. D., Boyle, E., and Carroll, L. J. (2014). Population-based, inception cohort study of the incidence, course, and prognosis of mild traumatic brain injury after motor vehicle collisions. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 95, S278–S285. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2013.08.295

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Compston, A., and Coles, A. (2008). Multiple sclerosis. Lancet 372, 1502–1517. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(08)61620-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Day, B. L., Dressler, D., Maertens de Noordhout, A., Marsden, C. D., Nakashima, K., Rothwell, J. C., et al. (1989). Electric and magnetic stimulation of human motor cortex: surface EMG and single motor unit responses. J. Physiol. 412, 449–473. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1989.sp017626

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Beaumont, L., Thoret, H., Mongeon, D., Messier, J., Leclerc, S., Tremblay, S., et al. (2009). Brain function decline in healthy retired athletes who sustained their last sports concussion in early adulthood. Brain 132, 695–708. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn347

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Koning, M. E., Scheenen, M. E., van der Horn, H. J., Timmerman, M. E., Hageman, G., Roks, G., et al. (2017). Prediction of work resumption and sustainability up to 1 year after mild traumatic brain injury. Neurology 89, 1908–1914. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000004604

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Serres, S. J., and Enoka, R. M. (1998). Older adults can maximally activate the biceps brachii muscle by voluntary command. J. Appl. Physiol. 84, 284–291. doi: 10.1097/00005768-199705001-00069

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eierud, C., Craddock, R. C., Fletcher, S., Aulakh, M., King-Casas, B., Kuehl, D., et al. (2014). Neuroimaging after mild traumatic brain injury: review and meta-analysis. Neuroimage Clin. 4, 283–294. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2013.12.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Enoka, R. M., and Duchateau, J. (2016). Translating fatigue to human performance. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 48, 2228–2238. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000000929

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flachenecker, P., Kümpfel, T., Kallmann, B., Gottschalk, M., Grauer, O., Rieckmann, P., et al. (2002). Fatigue in multiple sclerosis: a comparison of different rating scales and correlation to clinical parameters. Mult. Scler. J. 8, 523–526. doi: 10.1191/1352458502ms839oa

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gandevia, S. C. (2001). Spinal and supraspinal factors in human muscle fatigue. Physiol. Rev. 81, 1725–1789. doi: 10.1152/physrev.2001.81.4.1725

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gandevia, S. C., and McKenzie, D. K. (1988). Activation of human muscles at short muscle lengths during maximal static efforts. J. Physiol 407, 599–613. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1988.sp017434

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gronwall, D. M. A. (1977). Paced auditory serial-addition task: a measure of recovery from concussion. Percept. Mot. Skills 44, 367–373. doi: 10.2466/pms.1977.44.2.367

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howell, D. R., Osternig, L. R., and Chou, L. S. (2013). Dual-task effect on gait balance control in adolescents with concussion. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 94, 1513–1520. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2013.04.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Inglese, M., Makani, S., Johnson, G., Cohen, B. A., Silver, J. A., Gonen, O., et al. (2005). Diffuse axonal injury in mild traumatic brain injury: a diffusion tensor imaging study. J. Neurosurg. 103, 298–303. doi: 10.3171/jns.2005.103.2.0298

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, V. E., Stewart, J. E., Begbie, F. D., Trojanowski, J. Q., Smith, D. H., and Stewart, W. (2013). Inflammation and white matter degeneration persist for years after a single traumatic brain injury. Brain 136, 28–42. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws322

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keen, D. A., Chou, L.-W., Nordstrom, M. A., and Fuglevand, A. J. (2012). Short-term synchrony in diverse motor nuclei presumed to receive different extents of direct cortical input. J. Neurophysiol. 108, 3264–3275. doi: 10.1152/jn.01154.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kluger, B. M., Krupp, L. B., and Enoka, R. M. (2013). Fatigue and fatigability in neurologic illnesses: proposal for a unified taxonomy. Neurology 80, 409–416. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31827f07be

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krupp, L. B., LaRocca, N. G., Muir-Nash, J., and Steinberg, A. D. (1989). The fatigue severity scale. Application to patients with multiple sclerosis and systemic lupus erythematosus. Arch. Neurol. 46, 1121–1123. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1989.00520460115022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lannsjö, M., Af Geijerstam, J. L., Johansson, U., Bring, J., and Borg, J. (2009). Prevalence and structure of symptoms at 3 months after mild traumatic brain injury in a national cohort. Brain Inj. 23, 213–219. doi: 10.1080/02699050902748356

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lemon, R. N. (2008). Descending pathways in motor control. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 31, 195–218. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.31.060407.125547

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levin, H. S., and Diaz-Arrastia, R. R. (2015). Diagnosis, prognosis, and clinical management of mild traumatic brain injury. Lancet Neurol. 14, 506–517. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(15)00002-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lippold, O. (1981). The tremor in fatigue. Ciba Found. Symp. 82, 234–248. doi: 10.1002/9780470715420.ch14

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lorist, M. M., Kernell, D., Meijman, T. F., and Zijdewind, I. (2002). Motor fatigue and cognitive task performance in humans. J. Physiol. 545, 313–319. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2002.027938

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lundin, A., de Boussard, C., Edman, G., and Borg, J. (2006). Symptoms and disability until 3 months after mild TBI. Brain Inj. 20, 799–806. doi: 10.1080/02699050600744327

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCauley, S. R., Wilde, E. A., Barnes, A., Hanten, G., Hunter, J. V., Levin, H. S., et al. (2014). Patterns of early emotional and neuropsychological sequelae after mild traumatic brain injury. J. Neurotrauma 31, 914–925. doi: 10.1089/neu.2012.2826

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Merton, P. A. (1954). Voluntary strength and fatigue. J. Physiol. 123, 553–564. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1954.sp005070

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mollayeva, T., Kendzerska, T., Mollayeva, S., Shapiro, C. M., Colantonio, A., and Cassidy, J. D. (2014). A systematic review of fatigue in patients with traumatic brain injury: the course, predictors and consequences. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 47, 684–716. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.10.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Möller, M. C., Nordin, L. E., Bartfai, A., Julin, P., and Li, T. Q. (2017). Fatigue and cognitive fatigability in mild traumatic brain injury are correlated with altered neural activity during vigilance test performance. Front. Neurol. 8:496. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2017.00496

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Möller, M. C., Nygren de Boussard, C., Oldenburg, C., and Bartfai, A. (2014). An investigation of attention, executive, and psychomotor aspects of cognitive fatigability. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 36, 716–729. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2014.933779

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Multiple Sclerosis Council for Clinical Practice Guidelines (1998). Fatigue and Multiple Sclerosis: Evidence Based Management Strategies for Fatigue in Multiple Sclerosis. Washington, DC: Paralyzed Veterans of America.

Google Scholar

Nordin, L. E., Möller, M. C., Julin, P., Bartfai, A., Hashim, F., and Li, T. Q. (2016). Post mTBI fatigue is associated with abnormal brain functional connectivity. Sci. Rep. 6, 1–12. doi: 10.1038/srep21183

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Norrie, J., Heitger, M., Leathem, J., Anderson, T., Jones, R., and Flett, R. (2010). Mild traumatic brain injury and fatigue: a prospective longitudinal study. Brain Inj. 24, 1528–1538. doi: 10.3109/02699052.2010.531687

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Post, M., van Duinen, H., Steens, A., Renken, R., Kuipers, B., Maurits, N., et al. (2007). Reduced cortical activity during maximal bilateral contractions of the index finger. Neuroimage 35, 16–27. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.11.050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Powers, K. C., Cinelli, M. E., and Kalmar, J. M. (2014). Cortical hypoexcitability persists beyond the symptomatic phase of a concussion. Brain Inj. 28, 465–471. doi: 10.3109/02699052.2014.888759

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prescott, R. J. (2018). Editorial: avoid being tripped up by statistics: Statistical guidance for a successful research paper. Gait Posture doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2018.06.172 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rao, S. M., Leo, G. J., Bernardin, L., and Unverzagt, F. (1991). Cognitive dysfunction in multiple sclerosis. I. Frequency, patterns, and prediction. Neurology 41, 685–691. doi: 10.1212/WNL.41.5.685

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarajuuri, J., Psych, L., Pasanen, M., Rinne, M., Vartiainen, M., Lehto, T., et al. (2013). Relationship between cognitive and motor performance in physically well-recovered men with traumatic brain injury. J. Rehabil. Med. 45, 38–46. doi: 10.2340/16501977-1060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sars, V., Prak, R. F., Hortobágyi, T., and Zijdewind, I. (2018). Age- and sex-related differences in motor performance during sustained maximal voluntary contraction of the first dorsal interosseous. Front. Physiol 9:637. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2018.00637

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schiehser, D. M., Delano-Wood, L., Jak, A. J., Matthews, S. C., Simmons, A. N., Jacobson, M. W., et al. (2015). Validation of the modified fatigue impact scale in mild to moderate traumatic brain injury. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 30, 116–121. doi: 10.1097/HTR.0000000000000019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schillings, M. L., Hoefsloot, W., Stegeman, D. F., and Zwarts, M. J. (2003). Relative contributions of central and peripheral factors to fatigue during a maximal sustained effort. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 90, 562–568. doi: 10.1007/s00421-003-0913-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shinohara, M., Keenan, K. G., and Enoka, R. M. (2003). Contralateral activity in a homologous hand muscle during voluntary contractions is greater in old adults. J. Appl. Physiol. 94, 966–974. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00836.2002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, A. (1982). Symbol Digit Modalities Test: Manual. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.

Google Scholar

Schaart, R., Bernelot, M., and Westerman, S. (2008). The Dutch Standard Classification of Education, SOI 2006. Voorburg/Heerlen: Statistics Netherlands. Available at: https://www.cbs.nl/en-gb/background/2008/24/the-dutch-standard-classification-of-education-soi-2006

Google Scholar

Steens, A., de Vries, A., Hemmen, J., Heersema, T., Heerings, M., Maurits, N., et al. (2012). Fatigue perceived by multiple sclerosis patients is associated with muscle fatigue. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 26, 48–57. doi: 10.1177/1545968311416991

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stulemeijer, M., Van Der Werf, S., Bleijenberg, G., Biert, J., Brauer, J., and vos, P. E. (2006). Recovery from mild traumatic brain injury: a focus on fatigue. J. Neurol. 253, 1041–1047. doi: 10.1007/s00415-006-0156-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sumowski, J. F., Chiaravalloti, N., Krch, D., Paxton, J., and Deluca, J. (2013). Education attenuates the negative impact of traumatic brain injury on cognitive status. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 94, 2562–2564. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2013.07.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tombaugh, T. N. (2006). A comprehensive review of the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT). Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 21, 53–76. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2005.07.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tremblay, S., Henry, L. C., Bedetti, C., Larson-Dupuis, C., Gagnon, J. F., Evans, A. C., et al. (2014). Diffuse white matter tract abnormalities in clinically normal ageing retired athletes with a history of sports-related concussions. Brain 137, 2997–3011. doi: 10.1093/brain/awu236

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Naalt, J., Timmerman, M. E., de Koning, M. E., van der Horn, H. J., Scheenen, M. E., Jacobs, B., et al. (2017). Early predictors of outcome after mild traumatic brain injury (UPFRONT): an observational cohort study. Lancet Neurol. 16, 532–540. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(17)30117-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Naalt, J., van Zomeren, A. H., Sluiter, W. J., and Minderhoud, J. M. (1999). One year outcome in mild to moderate head injury: the predictive value of acute injury characteristics related to complaints and return to work. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 66, 207–213. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.66.2.207

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Duinen, H., Post, M., Vaartjes, K., Hoogduin, H., and Zijdewind, I. (2007). MR compatible strain gauge based force transducer. J. Neurosci. Methods 164, 247–254. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolkorte, R., Heersema, D. J., and Zijdewind, I. (2015a). Muscle fatigability during a sustained index finger abduction and depression scores are associated with perceived fatigue in patients with relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 29, 796–802. doi: 10.1177/1545968314567151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolkorte, R., Heersema, D. J., and Zijdewind, I. (2015b). Reduced dual-task performance in ms patients is further decreased by muscle fatigue. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 29, 424–435. doi: 10.1177/1545968314552529

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolkorte, R., Kamphuis, J., and Zijdewind, I. (2014). Increased reaction times and reduced response preparation already starts at middle age. Front. Aging Neurosci. 6:79. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00079

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

World Medical Association. (2013). World medical association declaration of helsinki. JAMA 310:2191. doi: 10.1001/jama.2013.281053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wylie, G. R., and Flashman, L. A. (2017). Understanding the interplay between mild traumatic brain injury and cognitive fatigue: models and treatments. Concussion 2:CNC50. doi: 10.2217/cnc-2017-0003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yasen, A. L., Howell, D. R., Chou, L.-S., Pazzaglia, A. M., and Christie, A. D. (2017). Cortical and physical function after mild traumatic brain injury. Med. Sci. Sport. Exerc. 49, 1066–1071. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000001217

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zigmond, A. S., and Snaith, R. P. (1983). The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 67, 361–370. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1983.tb09716.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ziino, C., and Ponsford, J. (2006). Vigilance and fatigue following traumatic brain injury. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 12, 100–110. doi: 10.1017/S1355617706060139

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zijdewind, I., and Kernell, D. (2001). Bilateral interactions during contractions of intrinsic hand muscles. J. Neurophysiol. 85, 1907–1913. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.85.5.1907

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: mTBI, motor task, force decline, voluntary muscle activation, interpolated twitch technique, first dorsal interosseous

Citation: Prak RF, van der Naalt J and Zijdewind I (2019) Self-Reported Fatigue After Mild Traumatic Brain Injury Is Not Associated With Performance Fatigability During a Sustained Maximal Contraction. Front. Physiol. 9:1919. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2018.01919

Received: 01 October 2018; Accepted: 20 December 2018;
Published: 10 January 2019.

Edited by:

Duane C. Button, Memorial University of Newfoundland, Canada

Reviewed by:

Saied Jalal Aboodarda, University of Calgary, Canada
Brian H. Dalton, University of British Columbia Okanagan, Canada

Copyright © 2019 Prak, van der Naalt and Zijdewind. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Roeland F. Prak, r.f.prak@umcg.nl Inge Zijdewind, c.a.t.zijdewind@umcg.nl

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.