Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Physiol., 20 November 2018
Sec. Developmental Physiology

Visual Maturation at Term Equivalent Age in Very Premature Infants According to Factors Influencing Its Development

  • 1Department of Neonatology, Centre Hospitalier Régional Universitaire de Nancy, Nancy, France
  • 2EA 3450 DevAH, University of Lorraine, Nancy, France
  • 3Department of Neuroradiology, Centre Hospitalier Régional Universitaire de Nancy, Nancy, France
  • 4PARC Clinical Research Support Facility, Centre Hospitalier Régional Universitaire de Nancy, Nancy, France

Introduction: Visual impairment is a concern in premature infants as perinatal factors may alter maturation during visual development. This observational study aimed at evaluating visual maturation at term equivalent age and factors associated with impaired visual maturation.

Methods: Infants born before 32 weeks’ gestation were evaluated with routine brain MRI, visual acuity, refraction, fundus, and clinical eye examination. Environmental factors were collected from infant’s files.

Results: Fifty-four infants (29.5 ± 1.7 weeks’ gestation, birth weight 1194 ± 288 g) were studied at term equivalent age. Visual acuity was higher in premature infants at term equivalent age than in a reference publication with the same method in term newborns at birth (1.54 ± 0.67 vs. 0.99 ± 0.40 cycles/degree, p = 0.008). In multivariate analysis, abnormal brain MRI was the only factor associated with visual acuity (r2= 0.203; p = 0.026). Incomplete retinal vascularization was observed in 29/53 of infants at term equivalent age and associated with MRI abnormalities of the posterior fossa (p = 0.027) and larger refractive sphere difference between both eyes (1.2 ± 0.8 vs. 0.6 ± 0.4 diopters; p = 0.0005). Retinopathy of prematurity was associated with indices of smaller cerebral volume (p = 0.035).

Conclusion: Higher visual acuity in premature infants at term equivalent age than in term newborns at birth may be related to longer visual experience from birth. Lower visual acuity was correlated with abnormal MRI in preterm infants at term equivalent age.

Introduction

Visual impairment is observed in 3% of preterm infants born before 37 weeks GA. The main related causes are ROP, refractive ametropia, strabismus, and visual cortex involvement. The risk of visual impairment is higher when the prematurity is more important. Hellgren et al. (2016) showed 2.1% of blindness, 4.8% of visual impairment according to the World Health Organization in a cohort of infants born before 27 weeks GA at the 6.5-year follow-up. Moreover, they presented in comparison with the term control children group at age 6.5 years 17.4% vs. 0% of strabismus, 29.7% vs. 5.9% of refractive errors, 37.5% vs. 6.2% of ophthalmologic abnormalities (Hellgren et al., 2016).

Visual development occurs in three steps. The first step is the anatomical formation of the eye, which is influenced by genetics; the second step is the visual experience related to endogenous stimulation in utero; and the third step is the visual experience by exogenous stimulation after birth (Graven, 2004). Thus, VPI are at high risk of visual development vulnerabilities because the second step of development is curtailed and the third step begins in an immature system. Moreover, at 2 years of age, VPI born before 27 weeks GA with visual impairment have more psychomotor delay than those without visual impairment (Holmström et al., 2014). Therefore, visual maturation, defined as retinal, globe, vascular and non-vascular structures growth, and factors influencing this development are important in this population and have to be taken into consideration. However, the normal visual development in premature infants and the impact of the factors that influence it are poorly understood.

The aim of this study was to describe visual maturation of VPI at TEA and to investigate factors associated with impaired visual maturation.

Materials and Methods

Subjects

In our level III NICU, all preterm infants born before 32 weeks GA undergo routine MRI and an ophthalmological evaluation at TEA as part of their follow-up. Infants with a MRI scheduled between February and June 2016 were included in the present observational study. Infants with a genetic syndrome, ocular abnormality, facial malformation, or craniosynostosis were excluded.

Primary Outcome

Primary outcome was the measurement of visual acuity at TEA. Given the early age of the infants, we measured discriminative visual acuity in cycles/degree using the preferential looking method and Baby-Vision® acuity cards (Tropique, Paris, France) (Vital-Durand and Cottard, 1985). The infants sat on the lap of an adult as the examiner presented them with a series of cards. As appropriate for the age of the infants, the distance of the examination was 40 cm from the child. All examinations were carried out by the same investigator in the same quiet room with a bright and monotonous visual environment.

The examination was performed in binocular vision. The exam began with a low-frequency spatial acuity card and continued until the wandering glance. The answer was considered positive when the cards were perceived by the child. In response to a negative or doubtful answer, the examiner showed the previous cards again until the highest card was acknowledged. The visual acuity was that for which at least three of four responses were obtained.

Secondary Outcomes

Visual Maturation Evaluation

Visual maturation evaluation included clinical eye examination evaluating the ocular anatomy (appearance of the eyeballs, pupillary reflections, corneal aspect, palpebral aspect, appearance of sclera, conjunctiva, and iris). Signs of visual deficit were sought. Ocular motricity was evaluated in binocular vision.

Different reflexes were evaluated, including attraction to light, direct and consensual photomotor reflex, reflex of closing the eyelids to glare, fixation reflex, reflex of blinking to the threat, eye pursuit, reflex of convergence. Refraction was measured by an autorefractometer (Retinomax 3®, Righton, United States) under cycloplegia using 0.5% tropicamide drops. A retinal camera (RetCam Shuttle®, Clarity Medical Systems, United States) was used for FE as part of a longitudinal evaluation in the context of the routine screening and follow-up of ROP. It allowed to evaluate the maturation of the retinal vascularization, specifically with the completeness or incompleteness of the vascularization up to the ora serrata, the presence of ROP, and papillary excavation.

Neuroradiology

We also evaluated cerebral MRI data at TEA. Prior to undergoing MRI, each infant was fed, wrapped in a blanket, and placed (not sedated) in a 1.5 Tesla Vantage Titan® of Toshiba tunnel with cardiac frequency and oxygen saturation monitoring. MRI was categorized as “MRI globally normal” or “MRI globally abnormal” when lesions were present, such as supra-tentorial injury (sequelae of germinal matrix hemorrhage, subependymal hemorrhage, and diffuse or cystic periventricular leukomalacia) and sub-tentorial or fossa posterior lesions. The following indices of cerebral volume were measured: CSI and VSA (Naud et al., 2017).

Clinical and Pharmacological Data

Data related to the perinatal period were collected from the infants’ medical files in a standardized manner. The characteristics of the infants (GA, gender, Apgar score, weight and head circumference at birth and TEA), the therapies used and the major complications of prematurity PDA, intraventricular hemorrhage diagnosed by intracranial ultrasound and defined according to Papile’s classification (Papile et al., 1978), cystic periventricular leukomalacia diagnosed by MRI, BPD defined by Jobe and Bancalari (2001), ROP according to the international classification (International Committee for the Classification of Retinopathy of Prematurity, 2005), necrotizing enterocolitis defined by Bell (1978), early onset sepsis defined by sepsis ≤ 3 days of birth and late onset sepsis defined by sepsis > 3 days of birth, blood inflammation defined by C-reactive protein > 30 mg/l or Interleukin-6 > 250 μg/l, nutrition during the infants hospitalization classified by breastfeeding, artificial milk or mixed feeding, and the duration of neonatal hospitalization were recorded.

Statistical Analysis

Visual acuity and secondary outcomes were described first. Data are presented as mean ± SD, or median and interquartile range and range. Next, visual acuity was compared to published reference data for children born at term (Vital-Durand, 1992). In order to compare the individual relationships between visual maturation and the perinatal environment, parametric (Student’s T, ANOVA, chi-square) and non-parametric tests (Mann and Whitney, Kruskal–Wallis, Fisher exact) were used when appropriate. To determine which variables were associated with visual acuity, bivariate and multivariate linear regression analyses were performed. Only the factor presenting a significant association at the threshold of 0.2 in the bivariate model were included in the multivariate model with the stepwise method (SLE = 0.2; SLS = 0.05). The threshold for significance was set at 5%. These analyses were performed by the clinical research support facility of the Nancy CHRU (Unit of Methodology, Data Management, and Statistics) using SAS v9.4 software (SAS Institute, Cary, NC, United States).

Ethics Statement

This prospective observational study was approved by our institutional review board (DRI, CHRU Nancy: PSS2016/PREMAVISION) and registered with the Commission Nationale Informatique et Libertés (number: R2016-02) and ClinicalTrials.gov (number: NCT02890251). Both parents were informed that the data will be used for research purposes and their consent obtained in writing and signed. The research followed the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki.

Results

Population Characteristics

Fifty-four infants were studied. The characteristics of our population in the neonatal period and at TEA evaluation are presented in Tables 1, 2, respectively. The duration of neonatal hospitalization was 68.5 ± 21.8 days.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Neonatal characteristics of the study population (N = 54).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Characteristics of the study population at evaluation.

Visual Acuity

Visual acuity was higher in premature infants at TEA than in the reference with term newborns at birth (1.54 ± 0.67 vs. 0.99 ± 0.40 cycles/degree, p = 0.008). No relationship was found between visual acuity and actual age or corrected age in VPI. Potential associated factors with higher visual acuity in bivariate analyses and included in the multivariate analysis are presented Table 3. Of note, the refraction of the two eyes in combined analysis did not reveal an impact of the sphere, but it did reveal a significant impact of the cylinder (right eye r = 0.031, left eye r = 0.219; p = 0.004).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. Visual acuity (cycles/degree) by risk factors in bivariate analysis.

There was no significant association with the other factors studied (p > 0.2).

In multivariate analysis, abnormal brain MRI was the only factor associated with visual acuity (r2= 0.203, p = 0.026).

Other Visual Development Parameters

No infant had signs of visual impairment. Among the observed palpebral abnormalities, 6 infants had bilateral port wine stains and 1 had a left eyelid hidrocystoma. No other morphological abnormalities were found. Eye pursuit was normal in all patients. FE revealed vascular tortuosity in three cases. No infant had papillary excavation, with a cup/disk ratio ≤0.2 in all cases.

Retinal vascularization at TEA was completed in only 24 of the premature infants. Incomplete retinal vascularization was associated with MRI abnormalities of the posterior fossa (p = 0.027) and a larger refractive sphere difference between both eyes (1.2 ± 0.8 vs. 0.6 ± 0.4 diopters; p = 0.0005). The Spearman correlation test did not demonstrate a correlation between head circumference at birth and the refractive measurements, or between head circumference at TEA and refractive measurements. However, a correlation of r = 0.306 (p = 0.024) was measured between head circumference at birth and at TEA, and a correlation of r = 0.275 (p = 0.044) between head circumference at birth and the refractive sphere difference between both eyes at TEA. Palpebral abnormalities were not significantly related to the refractive data. The blink reflex to the threat appeared to be associated with an absence of ROP at TEA (p = 0.042). We did not find any significant correlation between the glare reflex and environmental factors. The convergence reflex was related to the actual age at the time of the assessment; it was present at an average age of 99.1 ± 18 days and absent at an average age of 88 ± 13.7 days (p = 0.049). Regarding postconceptional age, it was present at an average age of 43.4 ± 2.3 weeks GA and absent at an average age of 42.1 ± 1.4 weeks GA (p = 0.076). No significant relationship was found with environmental factors.

Retinopathy of Prematurity (ROP)

Thirteen of our patients had ROP in the neonatal period; they were born more premature (27.8 ± 1.6 vs. 30.0 ± 1.5 weeks GA, p = 0.0001) and with a lower birth weight (1002 ± 315 vs. 1248 ± 256 g; p = 0.007). In addition, they had longer duration of mechanical ventilation (11.4 ± 10.8 vs. 4.1 ± 5.4 days; p = 0.002), more moderate and severe BPD (80% vs. 29.4% and 20% vs. 5.9%, respectively; p = 0.001), more surgery for PDA (30.8% vs. 2.6%, p = 0.011), and more late onset infections (53.8% vs. 20.5%, p = 0.034). The maximum ROP stage, with both eyes combined, was stage 1 in 7.7% of infants, stage 2 in 61.5% of infants, and stage 3 in 30.8% of infants. The most severe zone reached with both eyes combined was zone II in 69.2% of cases and zone III in 30.8% of cases. For the five cases of ROP observed at TEA, the maximum stage was stage 1 in 1 infant and stage 2 in 4 infants. The most severe zone was zone II in 1 infant and zone III in 4 infants. Asymmetry of the ROP between both eyes was noted. No ROP treatment was necessary in our population. ROP in VPI was significantly associated with indicators of smaller cerebral volume (VSA = 6981.1 ± 606.2 vs. 7387.7 ± 579.0; p = 0.035).

Discussion

Visual acuity was higher in premature infants at TEA than in term newborns at birth as reported by the Vital-Durand study (Vital-Durand, 1992). These studies are relevant for a comparison with our population because of the comparability of the measurement of visual acuity by the preferential looking method (Vital-Durand et al., 1996). This result is consistent with Searle et al. (1989) who showed that visual acuity of preterm infants born ≤ 36 weeks GA was better than full-term infants born > 36 weeks GA. Moreover, visual acuity development in healthy preterm infants appears to be accelerated when compared with full-term infants of the same post-conceptional age (Searle et al., 1989). This result may be related to a longer exogenous visual experience from birth in the VPI, to the contrary of the study of Weinacht et al. (1999) who showed that additional visual experience of preterm infants born after 31 weeks GA did not influence development of visual acuity during the 1st months of life.

Searle et al. (1989) showed that acuity development of normal preterm infants was related to post-conceptional age rather than actual age. Accordingly, we did not find an effect of the actual age at TEA on visual parameters in our population. A critical period for the onset of visual maturation is suggested in VPI. The neurological pathways of the retina are active between 22 and 40 weeks GA and may begin to function before the organization of sleep, which appears at approximately 28–30 weeks GA. Their organization occurs during sleep in the dark (Graven, 2004). However, total permanent darkness is deleterious for visual development; visual experience in the 1st months of life is essential for the development of visual function (Maurer and Lewis, 2001). Newborns exposed to a day and night luminous rhythm open their eyes more than those exposed to continuous light stimulation (Robinson et al., 1989). The entry of light into the eye is likely not limited in premature infants before 32 weeks PMA, as the eyelids are thin and often open (Graven, 2004). In our unit, we cover the incubators with a cloth in order to reduce the ambient brightness, especially direct exposure of the retina to strong light, which is known to be toxic (Niessen, 2006). In addition, the ambient lights are softened at night to facilitate sleep/wake rhythms. Thus, when the eyelids are closed, the children may have better darkness, which could lead to better organization of the retinal neurological pathways. The eyelids are closed for 55% of the time among those under 26 weeks GA, 93% of the time at 28 weeks GA, and 60% of the time at 34 weeks GA (Robinson et al., 1989; Fielder and Moseley, 2000). Therefore, the beginning of the critical period of visual maturation is likely around 28 weeks postmenstrual age (PMA). The number of patients is insufficient to show this developmental threshold effect at an intermediate age between 24 and 32 weeks, but this point should be taken into consideration in subsequent studies.

We only found an association between a lower visual acuity and abnormal cerebral MRI, without a significant specific link with the occipital area. This may be due to the small size of our population and a lack of power. Padilla et al reported significant global reductions in the brain tissue of premature infants born before 27 weeks GA but observed specific increases in cortical gray and white matter involving vision-related brain areas (Padilla et al., 2015).

Cerebral imaging is interesting for assessing the development of vision (Chau et al., 2013; Padilla et al., 2015; Naud et al., 2017). The cerebral volume seems related to visual development and the occurrence of abnormalities. Cerebral volume indices such as VSA, which is a standardized spherical surface area calculation (Naud et al., 2017), was significantly associated with visual acuity in the multivariate analysis. Given the interest of the VSA and the link between the development of the posterior fossa and the retina, it would be helpful to develop an indicator of occipital brain volume development. Beyond the usefulness of the VSA in physiological visual maturation, this indicator seems to provide meaningful information in pathological situations, as it is significantly lower with ROP. Sveinsdóttir et al. (2018) showed a relation between ROP and lower cerebellar volume and lower unmyelinated white matter volume at TEA. Moreover, Drost et al. (2018) showed an association between severe ROP and reduced cerebellar and brainstem volumes measured by automatic segmentation method at TEA. Both of those studies concluded to neurodevelopmental deficits at 2 years (Drost et al., 2018; Sveinsdóttir et al., 2018). For medical teams that do not have these measurement techniques available, VSA is an easy to measure indicator in routine MRI. We were not able to evaluate the measurements of optical radiation, particularly because of the poor quality of the diffusion images. It would be interesting to evaluate the occipital area specifically, particularly by fractional anisotropy measurements (Chau et al., 2013) or functional brain imaging (Padilla et al., 2015).

None of our patients showed signs of visual impairment at TEA. Although strabismus is more frequent in VPI (Denis et al., 2006) the absence of strabismus in our population at TEA may be due to the too young actual age of our infants. Strabismus is an abnormality of binocular vision, which is set up between 3 and 5 months of life (Denis et al., 2006; Niessen, 2006); thus, being able to diagnose it prior to that age is unlikely.

The convergence reflex usually appears after 4 months. The timing of the appearance of visual reflexes in VPI is not documented in the literature. Our results suggest that visual reflexes appear according to the actual age and not the corrected age because the convergence reflex tends to appear when approaching 4 months of actual age and not corrected age. Weinacht et al. (1999) suggested that convergence reflex depends on postconceptional age in low preterm infants. However, this difference can be explained by the difference of population as we studied more immature infants and because only 8 infants of our cohort had a convergence reflex at TEA.

Angiogenesis of the retina and brain visual pathway seems to play an important role in visual maturation. Of note, 54.7% of our VPI’s retinal vascularization were surprisingly yet not completed at TEA. In our study, incomplete vascularization was associated with larger refractive sphere difference. So, it is possible that the vascularization has a role on the development of the eyeballs and thus on the refractive parameters of the eyes. Retinal vascularization is never strictly symmetrical, as in the asymmetry of ROP. Hellgren et al. (2016) showed more anomalies of spherical equivalents at 30 months corrected age when infants had ROP with an effect of the severity of ROP. We did not highlight this element, perhaps because of the low prevalence of ROP in our cohort. The impact of retinal angiogenesis on volume development in the eyes of VPI would be interesting to evaluate. The incomplete retinal vascularization of VPI at TEA seems to be predictive of anisometropia.

Contrary to the results of Good and Hou (2015), we did not find a link between visual maturation at TEA and jaundice. This can be explained by the fact that none of our jaundice patients exceeded the neurotoxic threshold and were all treated with phototherapy.

The visual acuity at TEA was not different when ROP was present in the neonatal period. Because the macula is the precise vision area containing the largest number of cones and located in zone I, and none of the ROP in our population was in this zone, our finding seems to be consistent with previous data. Nevertheless, it would be interesting to evaluate the visual field at a later age.

Our study has limitations. First, the number of patients was rather small. Even though we found a number of significant associations, the non-significant trends we observed may be due to a lack of statistical power. Second, cycloplegia was performed using tropicamide due to contraindications at this age and the toxicity of other cycloplegic drugs in the context of prematurity. Tropicamide may be a less reliable cycloplegic drug and does not allow the establishment of optical correction at the age of our population. Whether the refraction performed at TEA by tropicamide can be predictive of abnormalities confirmed by a more reliable cycloplegic drug in follow-up would be interesting to assess, as it would allow greater vigilance and implementation of earlier treatment for the prevention of amblyopia.

Conclusion

The critical period of onset of visual maturation of VPI remains to be defined. Evaluating the continuation of visual maturation in these VPI in order to obtain reference values for this population is important, as well as following the evolution of the visual parameters and defining any predictive elements of future amblyopia in order to improve care. A follow-up of our cohort is planned at 18 months corrected age, 4 and 7 years.

Author Contributions

MW participated in the concept and design of the study, acquisition, analysis and interpretation of data, wrote and revised the manuscript. AN and ES participated in the acquisition of data and revised the manuscript. IC-U participated in analysis and interpretation of data. J-MH participated in the concept and design of the study, interpretation of data and revised the manuscript. All authors approved the manuscript as submitted.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Dr. Caputo for his assistance with the study methodology; S. Guignon, research nurse, and S. Voirin for their assistance in data management.

Abbreviation

BPD, bronchopulmonary dysplasia; CSI, Cortex Skull Index; FE, fundus examination; GA, week’s gestation; MRI, magnetic resonance imaging; PDA, patent ductus arteriosus; ROP, retinopathy of prematurity; SD, standard deviation; TEA, term equivalent age; VPI, very preterm infants; VSA, Volume Slice Area.

References

Bell, M. J. (1978). Neonatal necrotizing enterocolitis. N. Engl. J. Med. 298, 281–282. doi: 10.1056/NEJM197802022980519

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chau, V., Taylor, M. J., and Miller, S. P. (2013). Visual function in preterm infants: visualizing the brain to improve prognosis. Doc. Ophthalmol. 37, 41–55. doi: 10.1007/s10633-013-9397-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Denis, D., Wary, P., Fogliarini, C., Bernard, C., and Benso, C. (2006). Risk factors for early congenital esotropia. J. Fr. Ophtalmol. 29, 103–109. doi: 10.1016/S0181-5512(06)73756-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drost, F. J., Keunen, K., Moeskops, P., Claessens, N. H. P., Išgum, I., et al. (2018). Severe retinopathy of prematurity is associated with reduced cerebellar and brainstem volumes at term and neurodevelopmental deficits at 2 years. Pediatr. Res. 83, 818–824. doi: 10.1038/pr.2018.2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fielder, A. R., and Moseley, M. J. (2000). Environmental light and the preterm infant. Semin. Perinatol. 24, 291–298. doi: 10.1053/sper.2000.8597

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Good, W. V., and Hou, C. (2015). Visuocortical bilirubin-induced neurological dysfunction. Semin. Fetal Neonatal Med. 20, 37–41. doi: 10.1016/j.siny.2014.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graven, S. N. (2004). Early neurosensory visual development of the fetus and newborn. Clin. Perinatol. 31, 199–216. doi: 10.1016/j.clp.2004.04.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hellgren, K. M., Tornqvist, K., Jakobsson, P. G., Lundgren, P., Carlsson, B., Källén, K., et al. (2016). Ophthalmologic outcome of extremely preterm infants at 6.5 years of age: extremely preterm infants in Sweden study (express). JAMA Ophthalmol. doi: 10.1001/jamaophthalmol.2016.0391[Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holmström, G. E., Källen, K., Hellström, A., Jakobsson, P. G., Serenius, F., Stjernqvist, K., et al. (2014). Ophthalmologic outcome at 30 months’ corrected age of a prospective swedish cohort of children born before 27 weeks of gestationthe extremely preterm infants in sweden study. JAMA Ophthalmol. 132, 182–189. doi: 10.1001/jamaophthalmol.2013.5812

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

International Committee for the Classification of Retinopathy of Prematurity. (2005). The international classification of retinopathy of prematurity revisited. Arch. Ophthalmol. 123, 991–999. doi: 10.1001/archopht.123.7.991

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jobe, A. H., and Bancalari, E. (2001). Bronchopulmonary dysplasia. Am. J. Respir. Crit. Care Med. 163, 1723–1729. doi: 10.1164/ajrccm.163.7.2011060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maurer, D., and Lewis, T. L. (2001). Visual acuity: the role of visual input in inducing postnatal change. Clin. Neurosci. Res. 1, 239–247. doi: 10.1016/S1566-2772(01)00010-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Naud, A., Schmitt, E., Wirth, M., and Hascoet, J. M. (2017). Determinants of indices of cerebral volume in former very premature infants at term equivalent age. PLoS One 12:e0170797. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0170797

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niessen, F. (2006). Neurosensory visual development of the foetus and newborn and neonatal intensive care units. Arch. Pediatr. 13, 1178–1184. doi: 10.1016/j.arcped.2006.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Padilla, N., Alexandrou, G., Blennow, M., Lagercrantz, H., and Ådén, U. (2015). Brain growth gains and losses in extremely preterm infants at term. Cereb. Cortex 25, 1897–1905. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht431

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Papile, L. A., Burstein, J., Burstein, R., and Koffler, H. (1978). Incidence and evolution of subependymal and intraventricular hemorrhage: a study of infants with birth weights less than 1,500 gm. J. Pediatr. 92, 529–534. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3476(78)80282-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, J., Moseley, M. J., Thompson, J. R., and Fielder, A. R. (1989). Eyelid opening in preterm neonates. Arch. Dis. Child. 64, 943–948. doi: 10.1136/adc.64.7_Spec_No.943

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Searle, C. M., Horne, S. M., and Bourne, K. M. (1989). Visual acuity development: a study of preterm and full-term infants. Aust. N. Z. J. Ophthalmol. 17, 23–26. doi: 10.1111/j.1442-9071.1989.tb00485.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sveinsdóttir, K., Ley, D., Hövel, H., Fellman, V., Hüppi, P. S., Smith, L. E. H., et al. (2018). Relation of retinopathy of prematurity to brain volumes at term equivalent age and developmental outcome at 2 years of corrected age in very preterm infants. Neonatology 114, 46–52. doi: 10.1159/000487847

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vital-Durand, F. (1992). Acuity card procedures and the linearity of grating resolution development during the first year of human infants. Behav. Brain Res. 49, 99–106. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(05)80199-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vital-Durand, F., Benson, P. J., and Blakemore, C. (1996). Teller and bébé vision tropique acuity cards yield similar acuities in infants. Invest Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 37:4901.

Vital-Durand, F., and Cottard, M. (1985). [The preferential looking technic: material and procedure for testing baby vision]. J. Fr. Ophtalmol. 8, 267–272.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Weinacht, S., Kind, C., Monting, J. S., and Gottlob, I. (1999). Visual development in preterm and full-term infants: a prospective masked study. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 40, 346–353.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Keywords: visual maturation, preterm infants, cerebral MRI, cerebral volume, retinal vascularization, retinopathy of prematurity, visual acuity

Citation: Wirth M, Naud A, Schmitt E, Clerc-Urmès I and Hascoët J-M (2018) Visual Maturation at Term Equivalent Age in Very Premature Infants According to Factors Influencing Its Development. Front. Physiol. 9:1649. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2018.01649

Received: 04 July 2018; Accepted: 31 October 2018;
Published: 20 November 2018.

Edited by:

J. Jane Pillow, The University of Western Australia, Australia

Reviewed by:

Sam Ebenezer Athikarisamy, The University of Western Australia, Australia
Pia Lundgren, University of Gothenburg, Sweden

Copyright © 2018 Wirth, Naud, Schmitt, Clerc-Urmès and Hascoët. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Maëlle Wirth, m.wirth@chru-nancy.fr

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.