- 1School of Psychology, The University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia
- 2School of Psychology, The University of New South Wales, Sydney, NSW, Australia
- 3Department of Psychology, The University of Toledo, Toledo, OH, United States
- 4Department of Psychology, Adrian College, Adrian, MI, United States
- 5Department of Pain and Translational Symptom Science, University of Maryland School of Nursing, Baltimore, MD, United States
- 6Departments of Anesthesiology and Psychiatry, University of Maryland School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, United States
- 7Center to Advance Chronic Pain Research, University of Maryland, Baltimore, MD, United States
Although critical for informed consent, side effect warnings can contribute directly to poorer patient outcomes because they often induce negative expectations that trigger nocebo side effects. Communication strategies that reduce the development of nocebo side effects whilst maintaining informed consent are therefore of considerable interest. We reviewed theoretical and empirical evidence for the use of framing strategies to achieve this. Framing refers to the way in which information about the likelihood or significance of side effects is presented (e.g., negative frame: 30% will experience headache vs. positive frame: 70% will not experience headache), with the rationale that positively framing such information could diminish nocebo side effects. Relatively few empirical studies (k = 6) have tested whether framing strategies can reduce nocebo side effects. Of these, four used attribute framing and two message framing. All but one of the studies found a significant framing effect on at least one aspect of side effects (e.g., experience, attribution, threat), suggesting that framing is a promising strategy for reducing nocebo effects. However, our review also revealed some important open questions regarding these types of framing effects, including, the best method of communicating side effects (written, oral, pictorial), optimal statistical presentation (e.g., frequencies vs. percentages), whether framing affects perceived absolute risk of side effects, and what psychological mechanisms underlie framing effects. Future research that addresses these open questions will be vital for understanding the circumstances in which framing are most likely to be effective.
Overview
As one participant in a recent study aptly remarked, “If I see all the side effects of the drug I am already ill” (Herber et al., 2014, p. 4). Numerous studies indicate that negative health information can generate negative expectancies that lead to adverse outcomes – labeled the nocebo effect (Colagiuri and Zachariae, 2010; Colloca and Miller, 2011; Faasse and Petrie, 2013). This creates an ethical paradox: informed consent requires that patients are warned about potential side effects (Wells and Kaptchuk, 2012; Colloca, 2015, 2017), but these warnings themselves may produce poorer health outcomes via the nocebo effect (e.g., Myers et al., 1987; Mondaini et al., 2007; Neukirch and Colagiuri, 2015).
The burden of nocebo effects on the healthcare system is not trivial. Nocebo effects account for between 40 and 100% of drug side effects (Mahr et al., 2017). Nocebo-induced side effects can result in treatment termination, protracted treatment, and psychological distress (Barsky et al., 2002). Communication strategies that reduce negative expectancies associated with the nocebo effect, but preserve informed consent, are therefore critical. One strategy that is gaining increasing theoretical attention is framing (e.g., Williams et al., 2013; Colloca and Nestoriuc, 2016; Planès et al., 2016; Glare et al., 2018; Webster et al., 2018a; Petrie and Rief, 2019).
Interest in side effect framing stems from the work of Kahneman and Tversky (Kahneman and Tversky, 1979; Tversky and Kahneman, 1981), who demonstrated that individuals do not appraise information purely rationally and objectively, but are influenced by how that information is presented. In their classic examples (Tversky and Kahneman, 1981), shifts in preference for statistically comparable outcomes were observed when framed in terms of lives saved (positively framed) as opposed to lives lost (negatively framed). Similarly, framing of side effect information to focus on positive outcomes (e.g., likelihood of not experiencing side effects) rather than the negative (e.g., likelihood of experiencing side effects) may reduce maladaptive expectancies about side effects (Glare et al., 2018), thereby reducing the burden of nocebo side effects (Colloca and Benedetti, 2007; Faasse and Petrie, 2013). A particularly appealing aspect of using framing to reduce nocebo side effects is that because statistical information regarding side effects is equivalent, informed consent is maintained.
The effects of framing on health outcomes unrelated to the nocebo effect are well-documented (O’Keefe and Jensen, 2007, 2009; Gallagher and Updegraff, 2012). However, there appears to be surprisingly little empirical research examining whether framing can reduce nocebo-induced side effects and no attempts to synthesize existing studies. To address this, we systematically reviewed studies regarding framing and side effects, in order to identify promising framing strategies for reducing nocebo side effects and make suggestions for future research. Since only a small number of studies were identified, we present the results in narrative fashion. Full details of the search (Figure 1) and methods (Supplementary Material) are provided.
Figure 1. Search terms and PRISMA flow-chart (from: Moher et al., 2009) outlining the procedure used to identify studies included in review.
Evidence to Date
What Constitutes Side Effect Framing?
Levin et al. (1998) distinguished between three sub-types of framed information: attribute framing (valence framing of a characteristic), risky choice framing (framing of risk information), and goal framing (framing the goal of an action). Due to differentially framing a single characteristic, attribute framing is likely of greatest theoretical relevance to the nocebo effect. The single characteristic is the likelihood of experiencing the symptom, framed positively (will not) or negatively (will). However, Levin et al. (1998) taxonomy precludes framing types with qualitative differences in the information presented (e.g., “message framing” below). Therefore, we opted for a broader definition and considered any manipulation in which framing was used to accentuate side effects positively, which we refer to as “positive valence framing.” In all, we identified six empirical studies comparing a positive valence frame with another type of frame. Table 1 summarizes these studies.
Attribute Framing
Four of the six studies identified investigated attribute framing. As above, attribute framing involves identical statistical information being presented either positively (will not experience) or negatively (will experience). O’Connor et al. (1996) was the first and the only one involving a clinical sample. They compared positive and negative attribute framing regarding influenza vaccines in patients with respiratory and cardiac disease. Patients were provided with verbal, written, and pictorial information regarding side effect risk, presented in a positive (60% will not get a sore arm) or negative (40% will get a sore arm) attribute frame. Three days post intervention, the positive frame led to fewer reported side effects and less absence from work than the negative frame, indicating a significant framing effect.
Webster et al. (2018b) administered placebo tablets to healthy volunteers described as a well-known medication. Information about multiple possible side effects (e.g., headache, nausea) was framed positively (will not be affected) or negatively (will be affected) as part of a Patient Information Leaflet (PIL). They implemented a hurdle model (see: Hu et al., 2011) on side effects attributed to the treatment and found that fewer participants reported such side effects following the positive frame. However, number and severity of side effects in those reporting at least one did not differ between frames, suggesting the positive frame primarily affected any occurrence of side effects. Worth also noting, their study does not meet strict criteria for attribute framing (Levin et al., 1998). While side effect risk was framed positively or negatively, statistical presentation type and written descriptors differed by frame (e.g., headache as an “uncommon side effect, 80% will not be affected” vs. as a “very common side effect, more than 1 in 10 will be affected;” Webster, 2018), which may have influenced results.
Faasse et al. (2018a) administered placebo tablets under the guise of a benzodiazepine and were the only researchers to include a no treatment control. Information about four side effects presented verbally, in writing, and pictorially was either framed positively (will not experience) or negatively (will experience). Both frames produced nocebo side effects relative to control. The positive frame reduced side effect reporting relative to the negative frame 15 min after treatment, but not 24 h later, suggesting temporary success of the framing manipulation.
Caplandies et al. (2017) were the only researchers to include a no-framing instruction control. They delivered sham brain stimulation to healthy volunteers to induce headaches in a 2 (effect type framing: primary or side effect) × 2 (attribute framing: positive vs. negative) + 1 (no-frame control) design. Participants received a verbal warning about headaches framed either positively (30% unlikely to get headache) or negatively (70% likely to get a headache), unless assigned to the control, where they received no information regarding side effects. There was an overall nocebo effect relative to control and these nocebo headaches were more likely in primary effect condition compared to side effect condition. However, unlike the above studies, there was no significant attribute framing effect on any measure.
Positive Message Framing
Two studies employed positive message framing, involving information that side effects were indicative of the drug working (Wilhelm et al., 2018; Fernandez et al., 2019).
Wilhelm et al. (2018) administered the beta-blocker metoprolol to healthy volunteers and verbally framed dizziness as a positive (indicative of the drug working) or negative occurrence (common unpleasant side effect). Framing had no effect on the frequency or intensity of side effects, but trended toward reducing perceived threat of side effects (Pearson’s r = 0.18). Exploratory moderation analysis revealed a reduction in frequency and threat of side effects among a subset of participants (those with high harm beliefs) – but we focus here on the overall sample to ensure consistency across studies.
Fernandez et al. (2019) administered the analgesic diclofenac with a side-effect-inducing agent (atropine) in healthy volunteers. Video instructions stated that experiencing side effects were an indication that the medication was active in the body and would help reduce pain (positive frame) or simply to inform staff if side effects were experienced (control frame). In this case, the positive frame led to more treatment-attributed side effects, but lower overall side effect intensity. Further, in the positive frame, side effect frequency predicted increased analgesia suggesting the positive frame may bolster treatment efficacy via the placebo effect for those who experience side effects.
Summary
Taken together, three of four studies suggest that positive attribute framing produces small reductions in side effects (effect size range: r = 0.11–0.24). In terms of message framing, the results were less consistent, with one study showing mixed effects (increased attribution, but decreased intensity) and the other only showing trends toward reduced side effect threat (i.e., not attribution or intensity).
Methodological Considerations and Future Directions
While the handful of existing studies suggest that framing is a promising technique for reducing nocebo side effects, they also highlight several unanswered questions that need to be addressed before the widespread use of framing can be recommended.
What Is the Best Mode of Communication?
The studies reviewed varied in terms of the mode of communication of side effect warnings. Two studies employed written, verbal, and pictorial methods simultaneously (O’Connor et al., 1996; Faasse et al., 2018a), two used verbal (Caplandies et al., 2017; Wilhelm et al., 2018), one video (Fernandez et al., 2019), and one written only (Webster et al., 2018b). The data in Table 1 suggest that the studies using multiple modes (including video, which comprises visual and verbal presentation methods) elicited numerically larger framing effect sizes, suggesting that multi-modal presentation may more successfully elicit the framing effect. Of course, it is impossible to know whether pictorial methods alone or interactions between multiple methods are fundamental to driving the framing effect. Regardless, traditional methods of delivering side effect information, such as in PILs, may be limited for inducing framing effects because they involve a single, written communication mode. Emerging health technologies (Suggs, 2006; Colledge et al., 2008), however, make the multi-media delivery of health information increasingly easy to implement. Thus, future research could capitalize on such techniques and systematically examine the effect of framing in different communication modes. This would help determine the optimal mode of delivery for inducing framing effects and to overcome potentially less efficacious methods (e.g., written PILs).
What Is the Best Method of Presenting Statistical Information About Side Effects?
Framed information regarding side effect prevalence can be presented in a number of formats, ranging from verbal descriptors (e.g., “common”), natural frequencies (1 in 10) and percentages (10%). Each influences the perception of absolute perceived risk of side effects to varying degrees (e.g., Hoffrage and Gigerenzer, 1998; Yamagishi, 1999; Berry et al., 2003), potentially impacting the nocebo effect. Of the studies reviewed, one employed verbal descriptors (Fernandez et al., 2019), one percentages (Caplandies et al., 2017), two natural frequencies (Faasse et al., 2018a; Wilhelm et al., 2018), and two both percentages and natural frequencies (O’Connor et al., 1996; Webster et al., 2018b). Interpretation of the effects of statistical presentation was difficult in two studies. One employed percentages and natural frequencies separately across frames, potentially confounding the framing effect with altered perceptions of risk (Webster et al., 2018b). The other framed dizziness as either a positive or neutral consequence of treatment, but employed negative attribute framing (symptoms occur in 10 out of 100 people) in both cases (Wilhelm et al., 2018). Of the remaining, the largest framing effect was associated with verbal descriptors, followed by natural frequencies (Faasse et al., 2018a). As outlined in the subsequent section, inclusion of statistical information is advised to reduce the perception of side effect risk (Knapp et al., 2009). If the available data holds, then natural frequencies may be the optimal method of eliciting a framing effect.
An additional related factor concerns the number of side effects that patients are warned about. Evidence suggests that side effect warnings are better remembered, and nocebo side effects stronger, when fewer potential symptoms are listed (Colagiuri et al., 2012). Due to limitations on memory capacity, framing may therefore only be effective when warnings contain few side effects. This is difficult to deduce from the studies reviewed. Among those employing an attribute frame and reporting an effect, the number of listed side effects decreased as effect size increased. It is possible therefore, that limited memory for specific side effects weakens the framing effect, although more research is needed. One possible strategy to address this, would be to positively frame the overall likelihood of experiencing any side effects (as in, Fernandez et al., 2019), which may increase the salience of the positive frame, even with long lists of side effects.
How Does Framing Influence the Perceived Absolute Risk of Side Effects?
An important ethical issue regarding the use of framing strategies to reduce the nocebo effect, concerns whether framing influences the perceived absolute likelihood of side effects. That is, if positive framing led to an underestimation of the absolute risk of side effects, then one could argue that informed consent was not actually being maintained because participants are not understanding the objective risk of side effects.
O’Connor et al. (1996) was the only study identified that examined perceived absolute risk of side effects. In their study, side effects were framed either negatively or positively according to their prevalence rates in the general population and participants were required to rate their perception of risk prior to treatment. Interestingly, both types of frames were associated with an increased perception of absolute risk relative to that outlined in the informed consent process. Importantly, estimates following the positive frame were closest to the objective statistical information – a finding consistent with studies risk perception following framing manipulations outside the nocebo effect (Jasper et al., 2001).
O’Connor et al. (1996) findings are, therefore, encouraging in terms of suggesting that positive framing does not compromise informed consent via a perceived underestimation of the absolute risk of side effects. However, given that it was the only study to test this in the context of nocebo side effects, we recommend that future studies investigating the effect of framing on side effects also incorporate an assessment of absolute risk perception as a matter of course so that researchers and clinicians can be confident that framing is not undermining informed consent.
What Mechanisms Underlie the Framing Effect?
Expectancy is believed to play a key role in the development of the nocebo effect (e.g., Faasse and Petrie, 2013; Kirsch, 2018). Only two studies identified examined expectancy for side effects. One found that positive framing reduced side effect expectancy (O’Connor et al., 1996) and one found no such effect (Faasse et al., 2018a). However, despite being identified as a robust predictor of the nocebo effect (Webster et al., 2016), the precise psychological mechanisms through which expectancy gives rise to adverse side effects remains unresolved.
One line of evidence suggests that negative expectancies may elicit the nocebo effect by generating anticipatory anxiety (Benedetti et al., 2006; Colloca and Benedetti, 2007; Colagiuri and Quinn, 2018). This anticipatory anxiety has been proposed to generate increased attention toward internal bodily states and therefore adverse symptoms (Colloca and Benedetti, 2007). This is consistent with attentional theories outside of the placebo effect, such as chronic pain (e.g., Todd et al., 2015), where anxiety regarding the threat of pain leads to interpretation of pain as harmful, increasing the attentional focus on pain. Meta-analyses of attentional processes confirm biases toward sensory pain-related stimuli in patients with pain (Todd et al., 2018). Since, risk information can be processed by affective and cognitive systems (Slovic et al., 2004), we posit that positive framing attenuates anticipatory anxiety via the affective path, which in turn inhibits the nocebo effect by reducing the attention directed toward the aversive symptoms in question.
In terms of the studies identified, two provided data relevant to a possible role of anticipatory anxiety. Webster et al. (2018b) found that increased levels of anxiety were associated with elevated symptoms in the negative frame and Fernandez et al. (2019) reported a positive association between increased anxiety and side effects across groups. Faasse et al. (2018a) also measured anxiety, but this was the primary outcome for their placebo manipulation, not a possible mediator of framing. Webster et al.’s (2018b) results are consistent with evidence that side effect information delivered in PILs (typically negatively attribute framed) induce anxiety and fear in patients, particularly when many side effects are presented (Herber et al., 2014). Thus, as with assessing perceived absolute risk, we recommend that future studies examining framing and nocebo side effects explore expectancy, anxiety, and attentional biases in order to determine whether these processes underlie the framing effect.
General Issues to Do With Research on the Nocebo Effect
In addition to the specific issues regarding framing and side effects described above, there are three general issues regarding nocebo research that should be considered when evaluating framing effects. First, only two studies reviewed included control groups to assess natural history (Faasse et al. (2018a) and no-frame instruction (Caplandies et al., 2017) conditions. Such control groups are important for determining the extent to which framing strategies influence side effects above and beyond processes such as the Hawthorne effect, including whether the framing effect generalizes to other symptoms. Second, only one study assessed how long the framing effect lasts (Faasse et al., 2018a). This is important, as nocebo effects are known to be easily instated, but resistant to extinction (Colagiuri et al., 2015; Colagiuri and Quinn, 2018; Faasse et al., 2018b). Third, inconsistent techniques were used for measuring side effects, including modified versions of the Generic Assessment of Side Effects Scale (GASE; Rief et al., 2011) and study-specific items (see Table 1). This lack of consistency makes cross-study comparisons difficult because the outcomes are not necessarily equivalent. Thus, as with nocebo effect research in general, we recommend that where possible future studies on framing and side effects incorporate appropriate control groups, evaluate the duration of any framing effects, develop and implement standardized measures to assess side effects, and consider potential methodological limitations when designing experiments (e.g., blinding, randomization and power: see Supplementary Material). Finally, preliminary evidence suggests that attribute and positive message framing may differentially impact side effect reporting, with the former decreasing, and the latter potentially increasing, side effect frequency. Further research is needed to disentangle these differences. Greater attention should also be paid to comparing the ability of these frame-types to uphold consent, ensuring that any intervention implemented is ethical.
Conclusion
The handful of available studies suggests that positive valence framing of side effect warnings is a promising technique for reducing nocebo side effects, whilst maintaining informed consent. While the mechanisms are currently unknown, we propose that positive framing reduces anticipatory anxiety and subsequent attention to aversive symptoms, which then attenuates nocebo effects. Future research is, however, required to determine the optimal method of delivering such interventions, including the mode of delivery and statistical presentation of the side effects. Given that positive framing is relatively simple and cheap to implement, it has the potential to be a highly cost-effective technique for reducing the huge burden caused by nocebo effects.
Author Contributions
All authors have contributed to the conception, review strategy, and interpretation of the results. KB drafted the initial version of the manuscript. KF, AG, SH, LC, LS, and BC contributed to refining the manuscript.
Funding
This research was supported by Australian Research Council Discovery Project Grant (DP180102061) awarded to BC and LS and Australian Research Council Discovery Early Career Research Awards awarded to BC (DE160100864) and KF (DE180100471).
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank Mr. Jonathan Davies, The University of Sydney, for his help with the study coding and risk of bias assessment (details of which are included in the Supplementary Material).
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fphar.2019.00167/full#supplementary-material
References
Baguley, T. (2009). Standardized or simple effect size: what should be reported? Br. J. Psychol. 100, 603–617. doi: 10.1348/000712608x377117
Barsky, A. J., Saintfort, R., Rogers, M. P., and Borus, J. F. (2002). Nonspecific medication side effects and the nocebo phenomenon. JAMA 287, 622–627. doi: 10.1001/jama.287.5.622
Benedetti, F., Amanzio, M., Vighetti, S., and Asteggiano, G. (2006). The biochemical and neuroendocrine bases of the hyperalgesic nocebo effect. J. Neurosci. 26, 12014–12022. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.2947-06.2006
Berry, D. C., Raynor, D. K., and Knapp, P. (2003). Communicating risk of medication side effects: an empirical evaluation of EU recommended terminology. Psychol. Health Med. 8, 251–263. doi: 10.1080/1354850031000135704
Caplandies, F. C., Colagiuri, B., Helfer, S. G., and Geers, A. L. (2017). Effect type but not attribute framing alters nocebo headaches in an experimental paradigm. Psychol. Conscious. 4, 259–273. doi: 10.1037/cns0000130
Colagiuri, B., McGuinness, K., Boakes, R. A., and Butow, P. N. (2012). Warning about side effects can increase their occurrence: an experimental model using placebo treatment for sleep difficulty. J. Psychopharmacol. 26, 1540–1547. doi: 10.1177/0269881112458730
Colagiuri, B., and Quinn, V. F. (2018). Autonomic arousal as a mechanism of the persistence of nocebo hyperalgesia. J. Pain 19, 476–486. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2017.12.006
Colagiuri, B., Quinn, V. F., and Colloca, L. (2015). Nocebo hyperalgesia, partial reinforcement, and extinction. J. Pain 16, 995–1004. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2015.06.012
Colagiuri, B., and Zachariae, R. (2010). Patient expectancy and post-chemotherapy nausea: a meta-analysis. Ann. Behav. Med. 40, 3–14. doi: 10.1007/s12160-010-9186-4
Colledge, A., Car, J., Donnelly, A., and Majeed, A. (2008). Health information for patients: time to look beyond patient information leaflets. J. R. Soc. Med. 101, 447–453. doi: 10.1258/jrsm.2008.080149
Colloca, L. (2015). Informed consent: hints from placebo and nocebo research. Am. J. Bioeth. 15, 17–19. doi: 10.1080/15265161.2015.1074314
Colloca, L. (2017). Tell me the truth and i will not be harmed: informed consents and nocebo effects. Am. J. Bioeth. 17, 46–48. doi: 10.1080/15265161.2017.1314057
Colloca, L., and Benedetti, F. (2007). Nocebo hyperalgesia: how anxiety is turned into pain. Curr. Opin. Anesthesiol. 20, 435–439. doi: 10.1097/ACO.0b013e3282b972fb
Colloca, L., and Miller, F. G. (2011). The nocebo effect and its relevance for clinical practice. Psychosom. Med. 73, 598–603. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3182294a50
Colloca, L., and Nestoriuc, Y. (2016). “Nocebo and the Patient–Physician Communication,” in Ethical Counselling and Medical Decision-Making in the Era of Personalised Medicine: A Practice-Oriented Guide, eds G. Boniolo and V. Sanchini (Cham: Springer International Publishing), 29–37. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-27690-8_4
Faasse, K., Huynh, A., Pearson, S., Geers, A. L., Helfer, S. G., and Colagiuri, B. (2018a). The influence of side effect information framing on nocebo effects. Ann. Behav. Med. doi: 10.1093/abm/kay071 [Epub ahead of print].
Faasse, K., Parkes, B., Kearney, J., and Petrie, K. J. (2018b). The influence of social modeling, gender, and empathy on treatment side effects. Ann. Behav. Med. 52, 560–570. doi: 10.1093/abm/kax025
Faasse, K., and Petrie, K. J. (2013). The nocebo effect: patient expectations and medication side effects. Postgrad. Med. J. 89, 540–546. doi: 10.1136/postgradmedj-2012-131730
Fernandez, A., Kirsch, I., Noël, L., Rodondi, P. Y., Kaptchuk, T. J., Suter, M. R., et al. (2019). A test of positive suggestions about side effects as a way of enhancing the analgesic response to NSAIDs. PLoS One 14:e0209851. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0209851
Gallagher, K. M., and Updegraff, J. A. (2012). Health message framing effects on attitudes, intentions, and behavior: a meta-analytic review. Ann. Behav. Med. 43, 101–116. doi: 10.1007/s12160-011-9308-7
Glare, P., Fridman, I., and Ashton-James, C. E. (2018). “Chapter seven - choose your words wisely: the impact of message framing on patients’ responses to treatment advice,” in International Review of Neurobiology, ed. L. Colloca (Cambridge, MA: Academic Press), 159–190.
Herber, O. R., Gies, V., Schwappach, D., Thürmann, P., and Wilm, S. (2014). Patient information leaflets: informing or frightening? A focus group study exploring patients’ emotional reactions and subsequent behavior towards package leaflets of commonly prescribed medications in family practices. BMC Fam. Pract. 15:163. doi: 10.1186/1471-2296-15-163
Hoffrage, U., and Gigerenzer, G. (1998). Using natural frequencies to improve diagnostic inferences. Acad. Med. 73, 538–540. doi: 10.1097/00001888-199805000-00024
Hu, M.-C., Pavlicova, M., and Nunes, E. V. (2011). Zero-inflated and hurdle models of count data with extra zeros: examples from an HIV-risk reduction intervention trial. Am. J. Drug Alcohol Abuse 37, 367–375. doi: 10.3109/00952990.2011.597280
Jasper, J. D., Goel, R., Einarson, A., Gallo, M., and Koren, G. (2001). Effects of framing on teratogenic risk perception in pregnant women. Lancet 358, 1237–1238. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(01)06353-X
Kahneman, D., and Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica 47, 263–291. doi: 10.2307/1914185
Kirsch, I. (2018). “Chapter five - response expectancy and the placebo effect,” in International Review of Neurobiology, ed. L. Colloca (Cambridge, MA: Academic Press), 81–93.
Knapp, P., Raynor, D. K., Woolf, E., Gardner, P. H., Carrigan, N., and McMillan, B. (2009). Communicating the risk of side effects to patients. Drug Saf. 32, 837–849. doi: 10.2165/11316570-000000000-00000
Levin, I. P., Schneider, S. L., and Gaeth, G. J. (1998). All frames are not created equal: a typology and critical analysis of framing effects. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 76, 149–188. doi: 10.1006/obhd.1998.2804
Mahr, A., Golmard, C., Pham, E., Iordache, L., Deville, L., and Faure, P. (2017). Types, frequencies, and burden of nonspecific adverse events of drugs: analysis of randomized placebo-controlled clinical trials. Pharmacoepidemiol. Drug Saf. 26, 731–741. doi: 10.1002/pds.4169
Marteau, T. M., and Bekker, H. (1992). The development of a six-item short-form of the state scale of the Spielberger State–Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI). Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 31, 301–306. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1992.tb00997.x
Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., Altman, D. G., and The PRISMA Group (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Med. 6:e1000097. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097
Mondaini, N., Gontero, P., Giubilei, G., Lombardi, G., Cai, T., Gavazzi, A., et al. (2007). Finasteride 5mg and sexual side effects: How many of these are related to a nocebo phenomenon? J. Sex. Med. 4, 1708–1712. doi: 10.1111/j.1743-6109.2007.00563.x
Myers, M. G., Cairns, J. A., and Singer, J. (1987). The consent form as a possible cause of side effects. Clin. Pharmacol. Ther. 42, 250–253. doi: 10.1038/clpt.1987.142
Neukirch, N., and Colagiuri, B. (2015). The placebo effect, sleep difficulty, and side effects: a balanced placebo model. J. Behav. Med. 38, 273–283. doi: 10.1007/s10865-014-9590-5
O’Connor, A. M., Pennie, R. A., and Dales, R. E. (1996). Framing effects on expectations, decisions, and side effects experienced: The case of influenza immunization. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 49, 1271–1276. doi: 10.1016/S0895-4356(96)00177-1
O’Keefe, D. J., and Jensen, J. D. (2007). The relative persuasiveness of gain-framed loss-framed messages for encouraging disease prevention behaviors: a meta-analytic review. J. Health Commun. 12, 623–644. doi: 10.1080/10810730701615198
O’Keefe, D. J., and Jensen, J. D. (2009). The relative persuasiveness of gain-framed and loss-framed messages for encouraging disease detection behaviors: a meta-analytic review. J. Commun. 59, 296–316. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-2466.2009.01417.x
Petrie, K. J., and Rief, W. (2019). Psychobiological mechanisms of placebo and nocebo effects: pathways to improve treatments and reduce side effects. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 70, 599–625. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010418-102907
Planès, S., Villier, C., and Mallaret, M. (2016). The nocebo effect of drugs. Pharmacol. Res. Perspect. 4:e00208. doi: 10.1002/prp2.208
Reiss, S., Peterson, R. A., Gursky, D. M., and McNally, R. J. (1986). Anxiety sensitivity, anxiety frequency and the prediction of fearfulness. Behav. Res. Ther. 24, 1–8. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(86)90143-9
Rief, W., Barsky, A. J., Glombiewski, J. A., Nestoriuc, Y., Glaesmer, H., and Braehler, E. (2011). Assessing general side effects in clinical trials: reference data from the general population. Pharmacoepidemiol. Drug Saf. 20, 405–415. doi: 10.1002/pds.2067
Scheifele, D. W., Bjornson, G., and Johnston, J. (1990). Evaluation of adverse events after influenza vaccination in hospital personnel. Can. Med. Assoc. J. 142, 127–130.
Slovic, P., Finucane, M. L., Peters, E., and MacGregor, D. G. (2004). Risk as analysis and risk as feelings: some thoughts about affect. Reason, Risk, and Rationality. Risk Anal. 24, 311–322. doi: 10.1111/j.0272-4332.2004.00433.x
Spielberger, C. D. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory STAI (Form Y). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Suggs, L. S. (2006). A 10-year retrospective of research in new technologies for health communication. J. Health Commun. 11, 61–74. doi: 10.1080/10810730500461083
Symon, A., Williams, B., Adelasoye, Q. A., and Cheyne, H. (2015). Nocebo and the potential harm of ‘high risk’ labelling: a scoping review. J. Adv. Nurs. 71, 1518–1529. doi: 10.1111/jan.12637
Todd, J., Sharpe, L., Johnson, A., Nicholson Perry, K., Colagiuri, B., and Dear, B. F. (2015). Towards a new model of attentional biases in the development, maintenance, and management of pain. Pain 156, 1589–1600. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000214
Todd, J., van Ryckeghem, D. M. L., Sharpe, L., and Crombez, G. (2018). Attentional bias to pain-related information: a meta-analysis of dot-probe studies. Health Psychol. Rev. 12, 419–436. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2018.1521729
Tversky, A., and Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science 211:453. doi: 10.1126/science.7455683
Webster, R. K. (2018). Understanding and Preventing Nocebo Responses to Medicines. Unpublished Doctoral dissertation, King’s College London, London.
Webster, R. K., Weinman, J., and Rubin, G. J. (2016). A systematic review of factors that contribute to nocebo effects. Health Psychol. 35, 1334–1355. doi: 10.1037/hea0000416
Webster, R. K., Weinman, J., and Rubin, G. J. (2018a). Explaining all without causing unnecessary harm: Is there scope for positively framing medical risk information? Patient Educ. Couns. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2018.09.014 [Epub ahead of print].
Webster, R. K., Weinman, J., and Rubin, G. J. (2018b). Positively framed risk information in patient information leaflets reduces side effect reporting: a double-blind randomized controlled trial. Ann. Behav. Med. 52:kax064. doi: 10.1093/abm/kax064
Wells, R. E., and Kaptchuk, T. J. (2012). To tell the truth, the whole truth, may do patients harm: the problem of the nocebo effect for informed consent. Am. J. Bioeth. 12, 22–29. doi: 10.1080/15265161.2011.652798
Wilhelm, M., Rief, W., and Doering, B. K. (2018). Decreasing the burden of side effects through positive message framing: an experimental proof-of-concept study. Int. J. Behav. Med. 25, 381–389. doi: 10.1007/s12529-018-9726-z
Williams, J. B., Sade, R. M., and D’Amico, T. A. (2013). Framing for success: nocebo effects in thoracic surgery. Ann. Thorac. Surg. 95, 9–11. doi: 10.1016/j.athoracsur.2012.10.048
Keywords: nocebo, placebo, framing, attribute framing, side effects, expectancies, adverse health outcomes, verbal suggestion
Citation: Barnes K, Faasse K, Geers AL, Helfer SG, Sharpe L, Colloca L and Colagiuri B (2019) Can Positive Framing Reduce Nocebo Side Effects? Current Evidence and Recommendation for Future Research. Front. Pharmacol. 10:167. doi: 10.3389/fphar.2019.00167
Received: 27 November 2018; Accepted: 11 February 2019;
Published: 06 March 2019.
Edited by:
Martina Amanzio, University of Turin, ItalyReviewed by:
Irving Kirsch, Harvard Medical School, United StatesRebecca Webster, King’s College London, United Kingdom
Copyright © 2019 Barnes, Faasse, Geers, Helfer, Sharpe, Colloca and Colagiuri. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Kirsten Barnes, a2lyc3Rlbi5iYXJuZXNAc3lkbmV5LmVkdS5hdQ==