- 1Department of Pediatrics, Centro Hospitalar Universitário Cova da Beira, Covilha, Portugal
- 2Escola Nacional de Saúde Pública, Universidade NOVA de Lisboa, Lisbon, Portugal
- 3Centro de Investigação em Saúde Pública, Escola Nacional de Saúde Pública, Universidade NOVA de Lisboa, Lisbon, Portugal
- 4Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Centro Hospitalar e Universitário de Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal
Background: Early infancy and childhood are critical periods in the establishment of lifelong weight trajectories. Parents and early family environment have a strong effect on children's health behaviors that track into adolescence, influencing lifelong risk of obesity.
Objective: We aimed to identify developmental trajectories of body mass index (BMI) from early childhood to adolescence and to assess their early individual and family predictors.
Methods: This was a secondary analysis of the Millennium Cohort Study and included 17,165 children. Weight trajectories were estimated using growth mixture modeling based on age- and gender-specific BMI Z-scores, followed by a bias-adjusted regression analysis.
Results: We found four BMI trajectories: Weight Loss (69%), Early Weight Gain (24%), Early Obesity (3.7%), and Late Weight Gain (3.3%). Weight trajectories were mainly settled by early adolescence. Lack of sleep and eating routines, low emotional self-regulation, child-parent conflict, and low child-parent closeness in early childhood were significantly associated with unhealthy weight trajectories, alongside poverty, low maternal education, maternal obesity, and prematurity.
Conclusions: Unhealthy BMI trajectories were defined in early and middle-childhood, and disproportionally affected children from disadvantaged families. This study further points out that household routines, self-regulation, and child-parent relationship are possible areas for family-based obesity prevention interventions.
Introduction
Obesity is a major Public Health issue worldwide. Over the last decades, obesity has been increasing at an alarming rate and appearing at progressively younger ages (1). In fact, between 1975 and 2016, among children and adolescents aged 5–19 years, the global prevalence of overweight has increased from 4 to 18% in girls and 19% in boys, and the global prevalence of obesity has risen from 0.7 to 5.6% in girls and from 0.9 to 7.8% in boys, summing up to 340 million children and adolescents (1). Besides, 41 million children under 5 years of age were estimated to suffer from overweight or obesity in 2016 (2).
In the short term, children with overweight and obesity not only have a higher risk of hypertension, diabetes, and sleep problems, they also have a higher risk of psychological distress, such as negative body image, low self-esteem, depression, and peer problems (3, 4). Furthermore, unhealthy weight tends to persist into adolescence and adulthood, increasing the lifelong risk of these non-communicable diseases (5).
Early infancy and childhood appear to be critical periods in the establishment of lifelong weight trajectories (6–8). Therefore, it is important to understand these trajectories and their early predictors in order to inform effective public health interventions.
From an ecological perspective, family plays a major role in every aspect of a child's health and development, especially during infancy and early childhood (9). Both observational and experimental studies support the persistent effect of early family environment on health behaviors and weight status, highlighting the central role of parents in childhood obesity (10). Several studies suggest that general parenting, parenting styles and practices, and parent-child relationships can shape early eating, exercise, sleep, and screen use habits that track into adolescence (11, 12). However, its influence on weight status is still debated.
An empirical way to investigate body mass index (BMI) trajectories over time is by using Growth Mixture Modeling (GMM), an extension of Growth Curve Modeling (13). GMM is a person-oriented approach that assumes that individuals do not belong to a single homogenous population but rather to distinct unobserved subpopulations with different developmental trajectories (14). GMM focuses on longitudinal change within each subpopulation and allows the classification of individuals into latent classes based on their growth trajectories.
Investigating BMI trajectories over time requires large samples and accurate anthropometric measurements. Although several studies explored weight trajectories in childhood, some studies used categorical measures of overweight and obesity, which leads to classification bias, and others used “crude” BMI values, which do not account for growth (13). Since normal growth results in an expected increase in BMI, age- and gender-specific BMI standard deviation scores (Z-scores) are often considered the gold standard for the analysis of anthropometric data at an epidemiological level (15). In fact, the Z-score has a linear scale that allows comparison between age groups and gender. Furthermore, it can be analyzed by summary statistics: the mean Z-score reflects the nutritional status of the entire population, and the standard deviation (SD) of the Z-score reflects the quality and accuracy of the data (15). On the other hand, most of these studies explored a small subset of covariates, such as gender, ethnicity, and socioeconomic status, leaving out important factors like birthweight and breastfeeding duration that are well-known to influence weight status, and even fewer studies have included early family environment, general parenting, and parenting practices.
Thus, the main objectives of our study were (1) to identify distinct subpopulations with different developmental trajectories of BMI from early childhood to adolescence in a general prospective cohort of British children, the Millennium Cohort Study; and (2) to examine the association between early individual and family factors and subsequent BMI trajectories.
In the literature, the most commonly found trajectory is the stable normative trajectory comprising the larger portion of the sample (13). While most studies report a decreasing BMI trajectory and an increasing BMI trajectory, other studies indicate an additional stable high trajectory (13).
Therefore, we hypothesize (1) the existence of different weight trajectories, including a stable normative trajectory and a persistent high trajectory; (2) that individual and family factors influence the likelihood of belonging to a group trajectory; and (3) that an adverse early family context increases the likelihood of following an unhealthy weight trajectory.
Materials and Methods
Participants
Data were drawn from the Millennium Cohort Study (MCS), a cohort study that follows children born between September 2000 and January 2002, and living and growing up in England, Scotland, Wales, and Northern Ireland. It also provides information about family circumstances and the broader socioeconomic context. MCS was designed to overrepresent specific subgroups of the population, namely children living in disadvantaged areas and those who are ethnic minorities. The sample is clustered by electoral wards stratified by country, ethnicity, and Child Poverty Index (16).
The study began with an original sample of 18,552 families, and at Sweep 2 (2003–04), it recruited 691 “new families” who were eligible but were missed at Sweep 1. Therefore, the total number of families ever interviewed comprises 19,243 families (19,517 children). Children were around 9 months at Sweep 1 and about 3, 5, 7, 11, and 14 years old at the subsequent sweeps. The study protocol meets the ethical requirements of the Helsinki Declaration, and it was approved by Medical Research Ethics Committee. Further information about the study design can be found in Connelly and Platt (16).
To the present study, singletons and the first-born child of the families with twins and triplets with valid data on BMI in at least one of Sweeps 2–6 were included, comprising 17,165 children.
Measures
Anthropometric Measures
Weight and height were measured by trained interviewers using standardized instruments (Tanita HD-305 scales, Tanita UK Ltd.; and portable stadiometers, Leicester Height Measure, Seca UK), with children wearing neither shoes nor outdoor clothes. Weight and height were used to calculate BMI (kg/m2).
We calculated age- and gender-specific BMI z-scores (BMIz) using World Health Organization (WHO) Anthro (17) and Anthro Plus (18) software, having as reference the WHO Multicenter Growth Reference Study population. Observations with extreme values (below −5 SD or above +5 SD) were considered outliers and excluded (15). Four individuals presented BMIz values only in the first two sweeps, with borderline BMIz at age 3 (around −4 SD) corresponding to extreme thinness, and at age 5 in the overweight range (around +1.5 SD). Since there were no subsequent values to validate these observations, they were considered highly implausible and excluded.
Covariates
Predictors of nutritional status were selected based on previous research, according to Davidson and Birch's ecological model of overweight (9). A conceptual framework of early life predictors of Overweight and Obesity is presented in Figure 1. Further details on how covariates were categorized and coded are presented in Supplementary Table 1.
Figure 1. Early life predictors of overweight and obesity. Child nutritional status is influenced by individual characteristics and behaviors (inner yellow circle) and by contextual factors at family (pink circle) and community (green circle) levels. These factors interact with each other to influence how a child grows and develops. This conceptual framework is based on Davidson and Birch's Ecological model of childhood overweight and Bronfenbrenner Ecological Systems Theory. (9) Covariates included in the analysis are presented in colored circles.
Sociodemographic and economic covariates were gender, ethnicity, family structure, family poverty, maternal age at birth of cohort member, maternal nutritional status, and maternal education measured at 9 months. Ethnicity was collected as a self-perceived 6-option variable including “white,” “mixed,” “indian,” “pakistani and bangladeshi,” “black or black british,” and “other” (19, 20).
Perinatal and early infancy covariates
Gestational age (GA) was categorized as “extreme to moderate prematurity,” “late prematurity,” and “term,” according to the WHO/UNICEF definition (21).
Birthweight was categorized as “low birthweight,” “normal birthweight,” and “high birthweight,” according to the WHO definition (22).
Birthweight centiles for age and gender were calculated using Intergrowth21 software, and individuals were further classified in “Appropriate for GA,” “Small for GA,” and “Largefor GA” (23).
Breastfeeding duration was assessed in Sweeps 1 and 2; it was re-categorized as in Carling et al. (24), with two more categories (“6 to 12 months” and “more than 12 months”), according to its distribution.
Introduction to solid food was classified as “early introduction” if it occurred before 4 months, and “late introduction” if it occurred after 6 months, according to European Society for Pediatric Gastroenterology Hepatology and Nutrition recommendations (25).
Child temperament and self-regulation
Child temperament was assessed at 9 months by 14 items from the Carey Infant Temperament Scale, capturing three dimensions: mood, adaptability/approach-withdrawal, and regularity. We created total scores for each dimension by summing the individual responses, as in Hernández-Alava and Popli (26). High scores on the first two dimensions indicated distress and withdrawal (Cronbach's α = 0.546 and 0.677, respectively), and high scores on the last dimension indicated regularity (α = 0.713).
Child self-regulation was assessed at 9 months by 10 items from the Child Social Behavior Questionnaire, with two dimensions: cognitive (α = 0.573) and emotional self-regulation (α = 0.632). Total scores for each dimension were calculated as the average of valid responses (27), so that higher scores in the first dimension indicated higher cognitive self-regulation, and higher scores in the second indicated emotional dysregulation.
Household routines, parenting beliefs, and parenting activities
Household routines were assessed at age 3, comprising sleep and feeding routines. A total sum score was created, with higher scores indicating consistent routines (α = 0.541).
Parenting beliefs were assessed at 9 months comprising three items derived from the European Longitudinal Study of Pregnancy and Childhood about the importance of stimulating, talking, and cuddling to a baby's development. We summed the responses (28), so that higher scores indicated more positive parenting beliefs (α = 0.730).
Parenting activities were assessed at age 3 and comprised five items (reading, teaching the alphabet, teaching counting, teaching songs/rhymes, drawing). A total sum score was generated, so that higher scores indicated higher involvement in these activities (α = 0.610).
Discipline practices and child-parent relationship
Discipline practices were assessed at age 3 by six items from Straus's Conflict Tactics Scale, measuring how often the mother used punishing (smack, shout, “tell off”) or withdrawal tactics (ignore, take away treats, send to bedroom/naughty chair) when the child misbehaved. We created two scores: a harsh parenting score (α = 0.657) by summing the responses in the punishing items, and a positive parenting score (α = 0.556) by summing the responses in the withdrawal items (29).
Parent-child relationship was assessed at age 3 by Pianta Short Form, a 15-item self-administered scale based on attachment theory. This scale comprised two scores: closeness (α = 0.657) and conflict (α = 0.787).
Statistical Analysis
Statistical analysis was performed using IBM Statistical Package for the Social Sciences, version 24.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL) and Mplus, version 8.3 (30). We also used the packages MplusAutomation (31) and dotwhisker (32) for R version 3.5.1. (33) Statistical significance was set to p < 0.05.
Estimation of BMI Trajectories
Longitudinal BMI trajectories were analyzed with GMM. GMM classifies children into latent classes based on their longitudinal change, so that individuals with similar BMI trajectories are assigned to the same class, and individuals in different classes follow significantly different BMI trajectories (14).
We tested multiple GMM models with different specifications before choosing the final model. First, we performed single-group models to identify the pattern (intercept only, linear, quadratic, cubic) that represented change over time the best. We also performed a latent basis model, where the pattern of change is not predefined but driven by the data, and loadings for the slope factor are estimated to represent the proportion of the total amount of growth that has occurred up to that point. Next, we performed GMM with intercept and slope variances fixed at zero for each class, a subtype of GMM called Latent Class Growth Analysis (LCGA), which assumes that all individual growth trajectories within a class are homogeneous (14). Then, we performed several GMMs with different growth factor variance specifications: first, with equal variances (homoscedastic model), and then freely estimated (heteroscedastic model). To determine the optimal number of classes, we started with a one-class solution and progressively increased the number of classes.
Model Selection
The best model was chosen considering information criteria, theoretical justification, and interpretability (34, 35). First, we looked at two model fit indices: Schwarz's Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC) and the Bootstrapped Likelihood Ratio Test (BLRT) (34). BIC considers the likelihood of a model as well as the number of estimated parameters, with lower values of BIC indicating better fitting. BLRT compares a model with k classes to a model with k-1 classes, providing a p-value. We then looked at entropy, a measure of classification quality and separation between classes. Higher values of entropy (near 1) indicate more confidence in the classification (36). Although values above 0.8 are considered good, there is no consensual definition of what constitutes low entropy (36). Finally, we considered the interpretability of the models and their practical meaningfulness, rejecting models with clinically uninterpretable classes and with classes representing <1% of the total sample.
Association Between BMI Trajectories and Early Individual and Family Factors
The association between covariates and class membership was calculated based on crude and adjusted odds ratio (OR) using a bias-adjusted three-step approach, which takes into account the classification error in class assignment (37).
Missing Values
Missingness resulted from item non-response and attrition. Attrition is mainly related to sociodemographic characteristics, so the MCS study was designed to account for this bias and still provide representative information.
Missing values on BMI variables were handled with full information maximum likelihood (FIML) estimation under missing data theory. Considered the gold standard for handling missing data in latent variable indicators, FIML uses all available data points and is robust to non-normal distribution (38).
Missing values on covariates were handled using multiple imputations carried out in Mplus. We used Bayesian estimation to create 25 imputed datasets, using the Markov chain Monte Carlo algorithm, and convergence criterion was set to 0.05. We included all covariates and also BMIz variables under the missing at random assumption, accounting for the complex sample design (38). Imputed values compare reasonably to those observed.
Results
Body Mass Index
Table 1 shows the summary statistics of BMIz across all sweeps and the nutritional status according to WHO BMIz cut-offs.
Comparing our results to those of the WHO Multicenter Growth Reference Study, at age 3 our study sample showed a slightly higher mean BMIz, implying an upward shift of our sample distribution, with a progressive decrease in subsequent sweeps. The BMIz SD superior to 1 indicated a slightly more dispersed distribution, but in all sweeps, it was below 1.3, suggesting good quality of the data. The overall prevalence of overweight and obesity decreased from 44.2% at age 3 to 29.9% at age 14.
Covariates
Sociodemographic characteristics of the analytical sample are presented in Table 2, and psychosocial covariates are presented in Supplementary Table 2. Compared with children in the analytic sample, excluded individuals (who had no BMIzs in any of the sweeps) were more likely to be male, to be from ethnic minorities, to come from economically disadvantaged families and from single-parent families. Mothers of excluded children were younger and less educated. Complete bias analysis is available in Supplementary Table 3.
BMI Trajectory Estimation
The single-group models that served as a basis for subsequent GMM are presented in Supplementary Figure 1, showing that the quadratic and latent basis models appeared to better explain change in BMIz over time. Although the Quadratic GMM showed lower BICs, they provided uninterpretable trajectories; therefore, we decided to further analyze the latent basis GMM, presented in Table 3.
The LCGA showed much higher BICs than the other models, and since LCGA is based on the assumption that there is no within-class variability, we did not explore these models further.
Regarding the homoscedastic GMM, the 2-class model showed a big heterogeneous class (95.7%) and a small well-defined class (4.3%), corresponding to an Early Obesity trajectory class.
The 3-class model provided classes that were very disproportional (one big class comprising 91.2% and the two remaining classes comprising 4.5 and 4.3%). Furthermore, the two smaller classes were interpretable and meaningful, corresponding to an Early Obesity trajectory (4.5%) and a Late Weight Gain trajectory (4.3%); however, the bigger class (91.9%) was still heterogeneous, showing 20% of children with overweight and obesity across all sweeps, not corresponding to a normative class that we could use as a reference.
The 4-class model showed more proportional and meaningful classes; it provided an Early Obesity trajectory (3.7%) and a Late Weight Gain trajectory (3.3%) similar to the 3-class model, and Weight Loss (69.0%) and Early Weight Gain (24%) trajectories. The 5-class model showed an additional class that represented only <1% of the sample.
Regarding the heteroscedastic GMM models, they showed lower BIC, with BLRT favoring the 2-class model. This model provided a decreasing trajectory (77.7%) and an increasing trajectory (22.3%). However, this model showed low entropy (0.49) and provided poorly defined classes, while the decreasing trajectory still comprised around 20% of children with overweight and obesity at Sweep 6.
Therefore, we considered the 4-Class Latent Basis Homoscedastic GMM the best model to explain the change in BMIz (presented in bold in Table 3).
BMI Trajectory Characterization
Figure 2 shows the BMIz trajectories from early childhood to adolescence. Latent growth factor means and variances are available in Supplementary Table 4 for each class. The intercept and slope are inversely correlated (r = −0.257, p < 0.001), meaning that the higher the initial BMIz, the lower the growth. The time scores are 0, 0.326 (p < 0.001), 0.633 (p < 0.001), 0.924 (p < 0.001), and 1, meaning that individuals have reached 32.6% of the total change in BMIz at age 5, 63.3% at age 7, 92.4% at age 11, and 100% at age 14. Thus, the greatest amount of change occurs during early and middle-childhood, and there is little further change (7.6%) in BMIz in early adolescence.
Figure 2. Average BMI Z-score trajectory for each class, from age 3 to 14 years. Overweight and obesity ranges were based on WHO Multicenter Growth Reference Study age- and gender-specific cutoffs.
Observing the BMI trajectories, two clearly distinct classes are seen: the Early Obesity and the Late Weight Gain. The Early Obesity class follows a clearly distinct trajectory across all sweeps, with a significantly higher mean BMIz at the starting point (mean intercept = 3.102, p < 0.001) than the other classes, that then decreases until the end of the study period (mean slope = −0.935, p < 0.001) but is still above the obesity cut-off. Although the Late Weight Gain class shows a lower starting point than the other classes (mean intercept = −0.716, p < 0.001), it then shows the greatest increase (mean slope = 1.889, p < 0.001) throughout early and middle-childhood, reaching the cut-off of overweight by age 11.
The other two bigger classes (Weight Loss and Early Weight Gain) have close starting points at age 3 (normal-high mean intercepts of 0.723 and 0.931, respectively; Wald test p = 0.06), but from then forward, they follow significantly opposite trajectories during childhood (Wald test p < 0.001): the Weight Loss Class steadily decreases (mean slope = −0.781), remaining in the normative BMIz range, while the Early Weight Gain increases (mean slope = 0.428) and plateaus in the overweight range.
The proportions of children with overweight and obesity in each class is represented in Figure 3.
Figure 3. BMI Z-score trajectory class characterization, showing the proportions of children with overweight and obesity in each class according to WHO Multicenter Growth Reference Study age- and gender-specific cutoffs.
Association Between BMI Trajectories and Individual and Family Covariates
The association between BMI trajectories and individual and family covariates are shown in Figure 4, and unadjusted and adjusted OR are available in Supplementary Table 5.
Figure 4. Early life predictors of weight trajectories. Association between early life individual and contextual factors and BMIz trajectory classes, using the Weight Loss class as the reference class. (A) Unadjusted Odds Ratio; (B) Adjusted Odds Ratio. CSBQ, Child Social Behavioral Questionaire; GA, Gestational Age; PT, prematurity.
Sociodemographics varied across classes. Males were more likely to follow the Early Obesity trajectory.
Children from single-parent families, living in poverty and with less educated mothers were more likely to follow unhealthy weight trajectories. Non-white ethnicity, in general, was associated with unhealthy weight trajectories. Indian, Pakistani, and Bangladeshi ethnicity showed a strong association with Late Weight Gain, while Black and Black British ethnicity was strongly associated with Early Obesity. Maternal overweight and obesity strongly increased the odds of Early Weight Gain and Early Obesity.
Regarding perinatal and early infancy factors, the Early Weight Gain trajectory was associated with late prematurity, high birthweight, being large for GA and with early solid introduction. Breastfeeding for more than 2 months lowered the odds of Early Weight Gain, but this association disappeared after adjusting to other covariates. The Early Obesity trajectory was associated with high birthweight and being large for GA. The Late Weight Gain trajectory was associated with extreme to moderate prematurity, low birthweight and being small for GA.
Regarding psychosocial family factors, consistent sleep and eating routines lowered the likelihood of following unhealthy weight trajectories even after adjusting for other factors. The Early Weight Gain trajectory was associated with distressed temperament and with emotional dysregulation, although these associations were attenuated after adjusting to other covariates. The Early Obesity and the Late Weight Gain trajectories were associated with emotional dysregulation and child-parent conflict, but these associations disappeared after adjusting to other covariates.
Discussion
We used GMM to capture the developmental change in BMI from early childhood to adolescence. We found four weight trajectories: Weight Loss (69%), Early Weight Gain (24%), Early Obesity (3.7%), and Late Weight Gain (3.3%). Using data from the MCS, a recent study applying LCGA to raw BMI data from 3 to 11 years also found four trajectories: Stable (83.8%), Moderate Increasing (13.1%), High Increasing (2.5%), and Decreasing (0.6%) (39). In addition, a similar study using the same period and that applied GMM instead of LCGA to raw BMI values found four trajectories, but with a significantly different interpretation: Low normal (boys, 49%; girls, 42%), Mid normal (boys, 36%; girls, 38%), Overweight (boys, 12%; girls, 16%), and Obesity (boys, 2%; girls, 3%) (19). In the latter study, there were no increasing or decreasing trajectories, meaning that children did not change to different weight categories during the study period (no mobility from normal to overweight/obesity categories or vice-versa) (19). Based on a methodologically different approach, our study further builds on the contrasting results of these previous studies, applying a homoscedastic latent basis GMM on age- and gender-specific BMIz to better capture BMI variation with growth and expanding the analysis from age 3 to age 14.
BMI Trajectories Are Mainly Settled by Early Adolescence
The greatest amount of change in BMIz occurred during early and middle-childhood, and there was little further change after age 11. In our study sample, by the time children enter school, they have reached one third of the total amount of change in their BMIz; further, 59.8% of the total amount of change occurs between ages 5 and 11, suggesting that early and middle-childhood are two different critical periods to intervene in weight trajectories. The importance of preschool years in weight gain has been established in other studies (6). In contrast, in one study using data from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (7) and in another in the USA (8), BMIzs steadily increased during childhood, with the greatest change occurring after school entry, suggesting that excess weight gain and obesity also develop in middle-childhood. In both studies, there was little further change in early adolescence.
BMI Trajectories Are Influenced by Different Early Childhood Factors
The Early Obesity and Late Weight Gain classes follow clearly distinct and extreme trajectories across all sweeps and represent smaller groups of individuals (3.7 and 3.3%, respectively). The other two classes, Weight Loss and Early Weight Gain, have close starting points at age 3 and then follow less extreme but opposite trajectories during childhood, which represent the majority of individuals (69 and 24%, respectively). Although poverty, ethnic minority, single-parenthood, and low maternal education formed a common core of predictors of unhealthy weight trajectories, we found different associations with early biological, psychological, and family factors.
Early Socioeconomic Context
In our study, children living in economically disadvantaged, ethnic, and single-parent families at 9 months of age were at greater odds of following unhealthy weight trajectories compared to their peers living in white advantaged families. Poverty has been consistently associated with obesity in both children and adults. Indeed, several studies support that early childhood poverty has an enduring association with obesity (40) and that its effect persists despite subsequent improvement in socioeconomic status (41).
In our study, we found a strong association between ethnicity and unhealthy weight trajectories, that persisted even after adjusting for other factors. Several studies support that ethnic minority children have a significantly higher risk of obesity (42, 43). There may be multiple mechanisms underlying the relationship between ethnicity and obesity, including genetic and metabolic differences, alongside differences in lifestyle, eating and exercise habits (44). In a recent study in the USA, although African American and Hispanic children showed higher rates of overweight and obesity, this relationship disappeared after controlling for family income (42). Also, children from ethnic minorities tend to live in deprived neighborhoods, with lower access to healthy food options and to parks and playgrounds, and are at greater risk of poverty and segregation (43, 45), suggesting there is a complex interplay between ethnicity, socioeconomic status and obesity. In our study, the association between socioeconomic status and unhealthy weight trajectories was attenuated by other factors like maternal nutritional status and maternal education. In fact, maternal education appears to play a major role in childhood obesity (46). A large prospective study including data from 11 European cohorts concluded that low maternal education substantially increased the risk of early childhood adiposity in all countries (46). Therefore, maternal education might moderate the effect of poverty on obesity.
Child Factors
Gestational age, birthweight, breastfeeding, and complementary feeding
In our study, children with high birthweight and who were large for GA were at greater odds of Early Weight Gain and Early Obesity. Interestingly, late prematurity was associated with the Early Weight Gain trajectory, but extreme to moderate prematurity and being small for GA increased the likelihood of Late Weight Gain.
Early life context, including pregnancy, has a strong effect on later risk of obesity. Two cross-sectional studies of young adults found that those who had been born before 33 weeks GA had higher adiposity and cardiometabolic risk than those born at term (47, 48). Moreover, it has been proposed that both nutrient overabundance and scarcity during pregnancy and infancy lead to a metabolic programming that results in an increased obesity risk throughout the lifespan (49). In fact, in a longitudinal cohort in Rotterdam, individuals with fetal growth restriction followed by infant weight acceleration had higher visceral and liver adiposity than those with normal fetal and infant growth (50).
In our study, being breastfed for more than 2 months lowered the likelihood of Early Weight Gain and being breastfed for more than 4 months lowered the likelihood of Early Obesity, but not of Late Weight Gain. Early solid food introduction had an opposite association. These associations were moderated by socioeconomic and biological factors. Breastfeeding has many demonstrated benefits for both mother and child (51). The mechanisms underlying the relationship between breastfeeding and obesity are still debated and include human milk polysaccharides, modulation of gut microbiota, and promotion of sensitive feeding and self-regulation (52, 53). In the literature, the protective effect of exclusive breastfeeding on rapid weight gain is seen mainly in early childhood, but its long-lasting effect has been debated (54). Other authors even argue that this relationship may stem from the more favorable socioeconomic and educational background of breastfeeding mothers (55).
Child temperament and self-regulation
In our study, emotional dysregulation in infancy was associated with Early Obesity, but this association disappeared after adjusting for other factors.
The association between infant self-regulation and adolescent obesity has already been demonstrated in a study using the same MCS cohort (27). A growing body of evidence indicates that self-regulation has an important role in eating behavior, with children who show a lower capacity to self-sooth and to self-regulate being at greater risk of using food as a consolation tool and of obesity (56). The ability to regulate emotions begins in infancy and develops in the context of early mother-child reciprocal interactions. Breastfeeding has been shown to promote a mother's sensitivity and capability of attributing mental states to their babies, and to predict more positive and sensitive behaviors during feeding at 12 months (53).
Family Factors
Household routines, parenting beliefs, and parenting activities
In our study, children with consistent sleep and eating routines in early childhood were less likely to follow unhealthy weight trajectories, even after adjusting for socioeconomic and biological factors. The effect of household routines on obesity has already been consistently demonstrated in a study using the MCS and in the USA (27, 57).
In our study, more positive parenting beliefs at age 9 months decreased the odds of Late Weight Gain, but this association disappeared after adjusting to other covariates. Although parental engagement in children's daily activities has been previously associated with lower obesity risk (58), we found no association between parenting activities and weight trajectories.
Discipline practices and child-parent relationship
Children who had a close relationship with their mothers in early childhood were less likely to follow the Late Weight Gain trajectory, and child-parent conflict in early childhood was associated with Early Obesity and Late Weight Gain. Previous research shows a strong association between parent-child relationship and unhealthy eating and sedentary behaviors, factors well-known to promote weight gain. A recent review highlights that a secure parent-child bond and high parental connectedness are associated with better eating and general health behaviors, while an insecure attachment and difficult parent-child relationship were associated with disordered eating and sedentary behaviors, mediated by temperament and self-regulation (59). Regarding weight status, in a longitudinal study, individuals with poor mother-child relationships in early infancy, assessed by maternal sensitivity and attachment, were at greater odds of obesity during adolescence (60).
In our study, positive parenting showed a small protective association with Late Weight Gain that disappeared after adjusting to other factors. Harsh parenting has been demonstrated to increase the risk of obesity, while the appropriate use of family rules has been associated with a deceleration of obesity risk in adolescence (58, 61).
Strengths and Limitations
This study provided new evidence about the relationship between nutritional status trajectories and child and family factors. Its prospective longitudinal design allowed the exploration of BMI trajectories from early childhood to adolescence, and its large sample size allowed the exploration of multiple important individual and family covariates. To date, this is one of few studies that comprehensively included psychological factors like child temperament, self-regulation, parenting, and child-parent relationship alongside socioeconomic and biological factors.
Also, this study further explored the methodological aspects of investigating BMI trajectories. Anthropometric measures were directly collected according to the best standards, providing reliable and accurate data for subsequent analysis. Furthermore, trajectories were based on a continuous measure of BMI rather than on categorical measures of overweight and obesity. Especially, trajectories were based on BMI Z-scores, considered the gold standard for evaluating anthropometric measures. Moreover, it included 5 repeated measures of BMI over time and explored different GMM specifications.
Nevertheless, this study is not without limitations. We found it challenging to choose the best model. Although there is a common understanding on how model selection should be guided, there is some debate about the best indicators and their cut-offs. Furthermore, we opted for a homoscedastic GMM because the heteroscedastic GMM provided less substantive and poorly defined classes. Nevertheless, we must acknowledge that homoscedasticity might influence our results. Also, non-invariant models, which are more flexible and which have more parameters to estimate, tend to have superior information criteria at the expense of decreased interpretability and entropy, and one might question what would be the use of a better fitting model if it had lost its classification accuracy. Therefore, meaningfulness has a significant role in model selection. Although the entropy of our final model is not ideal, the subsequent regression analysis was adjusted to this classification bias.
Attrition is a major problem in longitudinal studies. GMM uses FIML to estimate the BMI trajectories, including all available data in the analysis, and therefore minimizes the bias effect of attrition.
Child and family covariates showed different percentages of missing values, and a complete case analysis would have substantially reduced the available sample; thus, we needed to impute missing values, which might influence our associations. Multiple imputation was performed according to best practice, and analysis in imputed and non-imputed data yielded concordant results. Although we recategorized covariates according to their distributions, previous research, and, whenever possible, international standards and recommendations, this recategorization might affect the association between BMI trajectories and covariates.
Conclusion
This study focused on BMI trajectories and looked at their early childhood predictors. In our study, 31% of the children followed an unhealthy weight trajectory that was mainly set by the time they reached early adolescence. Therefore, obesity prevention interventions should target children in early and middle-childhood, particularly those living in disadvantaged families. This study further points out that the lack of routines, low emotional self-regulation, low child-parent closeness, and child-parent conflict are significantly associated with unhealthy BMI trajectories; therefore, these are possible areas for family-based, health-promotion interventions. Further studies should focus on how different family and parenting factors interplay and influence weight trajectories, and on possible short- and long-term consequences on health status and well-being, following a developmental perspective.
Data Availability Statement
The data that support the findings of this study are available through the UK Data Service [https://beta.ukdataservice.ac.uk/datacatalogue/series/series?id=2000031], but restrictions apply to the availability of these data, which were used under Special License for the current study. Data are available through the UK Data Service after approval by the CLS Data Access Committee.
Ethics Statement
The Millennium Cohort Study protocol meets the ethical requirements of the Helsinki Declaration and ethical approval has been sought for all sweeps: Sweep 1 - South West MREC (MREC/01/6/19), Sweep 2 - London MREC (MREC/03/2/022), Sweep 3 - London MREC (05/MRE02/46), Sweep 4 - Yorkshire MREC (07/MRE03/32), Sweep 5 - Yorkshire and the Humber - Leeds East MREC (11/YH/0203), and Sweep 6 - London Central MREC (12/LO/1786). The present analyses did not require additional ethical approval. Written informed consent to participate in each sweep was provided by the participants' parents/ legal guardian.
Author Contributions
Data analysis was performed by CS. The first draft of the manuscript was written by CS and all authors commented on previous versions of the manuscript and revised it critically for important intellectual content. All authors contributed to the study conception and design, read and approved the final manuscript, and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work in ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work are appropriately investigated and resolved.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
The Millennium Cohort Study (MCS), which began in 2000, was conducted by the Centre for Longitudinal Studies (CLS). It aims to chart the conditions of social, economic, and health advantages and disadvantages facing children born at the start of the twenty-first century. The Principal Investigator and Director of the MCS is Prof. Emla Fitzsimons, UCL Institute of Education, University College London. For details, see https://cls.ucl.ac.uk/cls-studies/millennium-cohort-study/. MCS was commissioned by the Economic and Social Research Council (ESRC - http://www.esrc.ac.uk/), whose funding has been supplemented by a consortium of Government departments and the Wellcome Trust (http://www.wellcome.ac.uk/).
The authors were grateful to the Centre for Longitudinal Studies (CLS), UCL Institute of Education, for the use of these data and to the UK Data Service for making them available, as well as to all the families who have participated in the MCS. However, neither CLS nor the UK Data Service bears any responsibility for the analysis or interpretation of these data. A preliminary version of this work is available as a Preprint in (62).
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fped.2020.00417/full#supplementary-material
References
1. NCD Risk Factor Collaboration (NCD-RisC). Worldwide trends in body-mass index, underweight, overweight, obesity from 1975 to 2016: a pooled analysis of 2416 population-based measurement studies in 128.9 million children, adolescents, adults. Lancet. (2017). 390:2627–42. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(17)32129-3
2. de Onis M, Blössner M, Borghi E. Global prevalence and trends of overweight and obesity among preschool children. Am J Clin Nutr. (2010) 92:1257–64. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.2010.29786
3. Rodriguez-Ayllon M, Cadenas-Sanchez C, Esteban-Cornejo I, Migueles JH, Mora-Gonzalez J, Henriksson P, et al. Physical fitness and psychological health in overweight/obese children: a cross-sectional study from the active brains project. J Sci Med Sport. (2017) 21:179–84. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2017.09.019
4. Bacchini D, Licenziati MR, Garrasi A, Corciulo N, Driul D, Tanas R, et al. Bullying and victimization in overweight and obese outpatient children and adolescents: an Italian multicentric study. PLoS ONE. (2015) 10:e0142715. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0142715
5. Patton GC, Coffey C, Carlin JB, Sawyer SM, Williams J, Olsson CA, et al. Overweight and obesity between adolescence and young adulthood: a 10-year prospective cohort study. J Adolesc Health. (2011) 48:275–80. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2010.06.019
6. Gardner DS, Hosking J, Metcalf BS, Jeffery AN, Voss LD, Wilkin TJ. Contribution of early weight gain to childhood overweight and metabolic health: a longitudinal study (EarlyBird 36). Pediatrics. (2009) 123:e67–73. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-1292
7. Hughes AR, Sherriff A, Lawlor DA, Ness AR, Reilly JJ. Timing of excess weight gain in the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC). Pediatrics. (2011) 127:e730–6. doi: 10.1542/peds.2010-0959
8. Datar A, Shier V, Sturm R. Changes in body mass during elementary and middle school in a national cohort of kindergarteners. Pediatrics. (2011) 128:e1411–7. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-0114
9. Birch LL, Davison KK. Family environmental factors influencing the developing behavioral controls of food intake and childhood overweight. Pediatr Clin North Am. (2001) 48:893–907. doi: 10.1016/S0031-395570347-3
10. Vollmer RL, Mobley AR. Parenting styles, feeding styles, and their influence on child obesogenic behaviors and body weight. A review. Appetite. (2013) 71:232–41. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2013.08.015
11. Sleddens EFC, Gerards SMPL, Thijs C, de Vries NK, Kremers SPJ. General parenting, childhood overweight and obesity-inducing behaviors: a review. Int J Pediatr Obes. (2011) 6:e12–27. doi: 10.3109/17477166.2011.566339
12. Halliday JA, Palma CL, Mellor D, Green J, Renzaho AMN. The relationship between family functioning and child and adolescent overweight and obesity: a systematic review. Int J Obes. (2014) 38:480–93. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2013.213
13. Mattsson M, Maher GM, Boland F, Fitzgerald AP, Murray DM, Biesma R. Group-based trajectory modelling for BMI trajectories in childhood: a systematic review. Obes Rev. (2019) 20:998–1015. doi: 10.1111/obr.12842
14. Muthén B, Muthén LK. Integrating person-centered and variable-centered analyses: growth mixture modeling with latent trajectory classes. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. (2000) 24:882–91. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2000.tb02070.x
15. Mei Z, Grummer-Strawn LM. Standard deviation of anthropometric Z-scores as a data quality assessment tool using the 2006. WHO growth standards: a cross country analysis. Bull World Health Organ. (2007) 85:441–8. doi: 10.2471/BLT.06.034421
16. Connelly R, Platt L. Cohort profile: UK Millennium Cohort Study (MCS). Int J Epidemiol. (2014) 43:1719–25. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyu001
17. Department of Nutrition for Health and Development at the World Health Organization (WHO). WHO Anthro Survey Analyser: Software for Analysing Survey Anthropometric Data for Children Under 5 years of Age. Geneva: World Health Organization (2018). Available online at: https://whonutrition.shinyapps.io/anthro/ (accessed October 25, 2019).
18. WHO. WHO AnthroPlus for Personal Computers Manual: Software for Assessing Growth of the World's Children and Adolescents. Geneva: WHO (2019). Available online at: https://www.who.int/growthref/tools/en/ (accessed October 25, 2019).
19. Stuart B, Panico L. Early-childhood BMI trajectories: evidence from a prospective, nationally representative British cohort study. Nutr Diabetes. (2016) 6:e198. doi: 10.1038/nutd.2016.6
20. Flouri E, Ioakeimidi S, Midouhas E, Ploubidis GB. Maternal psychological distress and child decision-making. J Affect Disord. (2017) 218:35–40. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.04.034
21. March of Dimes PMNCH Save the Children WHO. Born too soon: the global action report on preterm birth. In: Howson CP, Kinney MV, Jelawn, editors. Maternal, Newborn, Child and Adolescent Health. Geneva: World Health Organization (2012). Available online at: https://www.who.int/maternal_child_adolescent/documents/born_too_soon/en/ (accessed October 25, 2019).
22. World Health Organization. ICD-10: International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems: Tenth Revision. Geneva: World Health Organization (2004).
23. Villar J, Cheikh Ismail L, Victora CG, Ohuma EO, Bertino E, Altman DG, et al. International standards for newborn weight, length, and head circumference by gestational age and sex: the newborn cross-sectional study of the INTERGROWTH-21st Project. Lancet. (2014) 384:857–68. doi: 10.1016/S0140-673660932-6
24. Carling SJ, Demment MM, Kjolhede CL, Olson CM. Breastfeeding duration and weight gain trajectory in infancy. Pediatrics. (2015) 135:111–9. doi: 10.1542/peds.2014-1392
25. Fewtrell M, Bronsky J, Campoy C, Domellöf M, Embleton N, Fidler Mis N, et al. Complementary feeding: a position paper by the European Society for Paediatric Gastroenterology, Hepatology, and Nutrition (ESPGHAN) committee on nutrition. J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr. (2017) 64:119–32. doi: 10.1097/MPG.0000000000001454
26. Hernández-Alava M, Popli G. Children's development and parental input: evidence from the UK millennium cohort study. Demography. (2017) 54:485–511. doi: 10.1007/s13524-017-0554-6
27. Anderson SE, Sacker A, Whitaker RC, Kelly Y. Self-regulation and household routines at age three and obesity at age eleven: longitudinal analysis of the UK millennium cohort study. Int J Obes. (2017) 41:1459–66. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2017.94
28. Kroll ME, Carson C, Redshaw M, Quigley MA. Early father involvement and subsequent child behaviour at ages 3, 5 and 7 years: prospective analysis of the UK millennium cohort study. PLoS ONE. (2016) 11:e0162339. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0162339
29. Rajyaguru P, Moran P, Cordero M, Pearson R. Disciplinary parenting practice and child mental health: evidence from the UK millennium cohort study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2019) 58:108–16.e2. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2018.06.033
31. Hallquist MN, Wiley JF. MplusAutomation: an R package for facilitating large-scale latent variable analyses in mplus. Struct Equat Model. (2018) 25:621–38. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2017.1402334
32. Solt F, Hu Y. Dotwhisker: Dot-and-Whisker Plots of Regression Results. The Comprehensive R Archive Network (CRAN) (2015). Available online at: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/dotwhisker/index.html (accessed December 5, 2019).
33. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing (2019).
34. Nylund KL, Asparouhov T, Muthén BO. Deciding on the number of classes in latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling: a Monte Carlo simulation study. Struct Equat Model. (2007) 14:535–69. doi: 10.1080/10705510701575396
35. Todo N, Usami S. Fitting unstructured finite mixture models in longitudinal design: a recommendation for model selection and estimation of the number of classes. Struct Equat Model. (2016) 23:695–712. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2016.1205444
36. Celeux G, Soromenho G. An entropy criterion for assessing the number of clusters in a mixture model. J Classif. (1996) 13:195–212. doi: 10.1007/BF01246098
37. Vermunt JK. Latent class modeling with covariates: two improved three-step approaches. Pol Anal. (2010) 18:450–69. doi: 10.1093/pan/mpq025
38. Lang KM, Little TD. Principled missing data treatments. Prev Sci. (2016) 19:1–11. doi: 10.1007/s11121-016-0644-5
39. Kelly Y, Patalay P, Montgomery S, Sacker A. BMI development and early adolescent psychosocial well-being: UK millennium cohort study. Pediatrics. (2016) 138:e20160967. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-0967
40. Lee H, Andrew M, Gebremariam A, Lumeng JC, Lee JM. Longitudinal associations between poverty and obesity from birth through adolescence. Am J Public Health. (2014) 104:e70–6. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2013.301806
41. Li M, Mustillo S, Anderson J. Childhood poverty dynamics and adulthood overweight/obesity: Unpacking the black box of childhood. Soc Sci Res. (2018) 76:92–104. doi: 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2018.05.009
42. Rogers R, Eagle TF, Sheetz A, Woodward A, Leibowitz R, Song M, et al. The relationship between childhood obesity, low socioeconomic status, and race/ethnicity: lessons from Massachusetts. Child Obes. (2015) 11:691–5. doi: 10.1089/chi.2015.0029
43. Wong MS, Chan KS, Jones-Smith JC, Colantuoni E, Thorpe RJ, Bleich SN. The neighborhood environment and obesity: understanding variation by race/ethnicity. Prev Med. (2018) 111:371–7. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2017.11.029
44. Stryjecki C, Alyass A, Meyre D. Ethnic and population differences in the genetic predisposition to human obesity. Obes Rev. (2018) 19:62–80. doi: 10.1111/obr.12604
45. Morgan Hughey S, Kaczynski AT, Child S, Moore JB, Porter D, Hibbert J. Green and lean: is neighborhood park and playground availability associated with youth obesity? Variations by gender, socioeconomic status, and race/ethnicity. Prev Med. (2017) 95:S101–8. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2016.11.024
46. Ruiz M, Goldblatt P, Morrison J, Porta D, Forastiere F, Hryhorczuk D, et al. Impact of low maternal education on early childhood overweight and obesity in Europe. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. (2016) 30:274–84. doi: 10.1111/ppe.12285
47. Thomas EL, Parkinson JR, Hyde MJ, Yap IKS, Holmes E, Doré CJ, et al. Aberrant adiposity and ectopic lipid deposition characterize the adult phenotype of the preterm infant. Pediatr Res. (2011) 70:507–12. doi: 10.1203/PDR.0b013e31822d7860
48. Sipola-Leppänen M, Vääräsmäki M, Tikanmäki M, Matinolli HM, Miettola S, Hovi P, et al. Cardiometabolic risk factors in young adults who were born preterm. Am J Epidemiol. (2015) 181:861–73. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwu443
49. Koletzko B, Brands B, Grote V, Kirchberg FF, Prell C, Rzehak P, et al. Long-term health impact of early nutrition: the power of programming. Ann Nutr Metab. (2017) 70:161–9. doi: 10.1159/000477781
50. Vogelezang S, Santos S, Toemen L, Oei EHG, Felix JF, Jaddoe VWV. Associations of fetal and infant weight change with general, visceral, and organ adiposity at school age. JAMA Netw Open. (2019) 2:e192843. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2019.2843
51. Victora CG, Bahl R, Barros AJD, França GVA, Horton S, Krasevec J, et al. Breastfeeding in the 21st century: epidemiology, mechanisms, and lifelong effect. Lancet. (2016) 387:475–90. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(15)01024-7
52. Alderete TL, Autran C, Brekke BE, Knight R, Bode L, Goran MI, et al. Associations between human milk oligosaccharides and infant body composition in the first 6 mo of life. Am J Clin Nutr. (2015) 102:1381–8. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.115.115451
53. Farrow C, Blissett J. Maternal mind-mindedness during infancy, general parenting sensitivity and observed child feeding behavior: a longitudinal study. Attach Hum Dev. (2014) 16:230–41. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2014.898158
54. Liu JX, Xu X, Liu JH, Hardin JW, Li R. Association of maternal gestational weight gain with their offspring's anthropometric outcomes at late infancy and 6 years old: mediating roles of birth weight and breastfeeding duration. Int J Obes. (2017) 42:8–14. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2017.183
55. Coduti N, Gregoire M, Sowa D, Diakakis GM, Chen Y. Characteristics of exclusively breastfeeding mothers. Top Clin Nutr. (2015) 30:174–83. doi: 10.1097/TIN.0000000000000033
56. Graziano PA, Kelleher R, Calkins SD, Keane SP, Brien MO. Predicting weight outcomes in preadolescence: the role of toddlers' self-regulation skills and the temperament dimension of pleasure. Int J Obes. (2013) 37:937–42. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2012.165
57. Anderson SE, Whitaker RC. Household routines and obesity in US preschool-aged children. Pediatrics. (2010) 125:420–8. doi: 10.1542/peds.2009-0417
58. Huang DYC, Lanza HI, Anglin MD. Trajectory of adolescent obesity: exploring the impact of prenatal to childhood experiences. J Child Fam Stud. (2014) 23:1090–101. doi: 10.1007/s10826-013-9766-6
59. Blewitt C, Bergmeier H, Macdonald JA, Olsson CA, Skouteris H. Associations between parent-child relationship quality and obesogenic risk in adolescence: a systematic review of recent literature. Obes Rev. (2016) 17:612–22. doi: 10.1111/obr.12413
60. Anderson SE, Gooze RA, Lemeshow S, Whitaker RC. Quality of early maternal-child relationship and risk of adolescent obesity. Pediatrics. (2012) 129:132–40. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-0972
61. Lohman BJ, Gillette MT, Neppl TK. Harsh parenting and food insecurity in adolescence: the association with emerging adult obesity. J Adolesc Health. (2016) 59:123–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2016.03.024
62. dos Santos CS, Picoito J, Nunes C, Loureiro I. Early individual and family predictors of weight trajectories from early childhood to adolescence: results from the Millennium Cohort Study. medRxiv [Preprint]. (2020). doi: 10.1101/2020.02.26.20027409
Data References
1. University of London, Institute of Education, Centre for Longitudinal Studies. (2017). Millennium Cohort Study: Longitudinal Family File, 2001-2015. [data collection]. 2nd Edition. UK Data Service. SN:8172, doi: 10.5255/UKDA-SN-8172-2
2. University of London, Institute of Education, Centre for Longitudinal Studies. (2017). Millennium Cohort Study: First Survey, 2001-2003. [data collection]. 12th Edition. UK Data Service. SN: 4683, doi: 10.5255/UKDA-SN-4683-4
3. University of London, Institute of Education, Centre for Longitudinal Studies. (2017). Millennium Cohort Study: Second Survey, 2003-2005. [data collection]. 9th Edition. UK Data Service. SN: 5350, doi: 10.5255/UKDA-SN-5350-4
4. University of London, Institute of Education, Centre for Longitudinal Studies. (2017). Millennium Cohort Study: Third Survey, 2006. [data collection]. 7th Edition. UK Data Service. SN: 5795, doi: 10.5255/UKDA-SN-5795-4
5. University of London, Institute of Education, Centre for Longitudinal Studies. (2017). Millennium Cohort Study: Fourth Survey, 2008. [data collection]. 7th Edition. UK Data Service. SN: 6411, doi: 10.5255/UKDA-SN-6411-7
6. University of London, Institute of Education, Centre for Longitudinal Studies. (2017). Millennium Cohort Study: Fifth Survey, 2012. [data collection]. 4th Edition. UK Data Service. SN: 7464, doi: 10.5255/UKDA-SN-7464-4
7. University of London, Institute of Education, Centre for Longitudinal Studies. (2019). Millennium Cohort Study: Sixth Survey, 2015. [data collection]. 4th Edition. UK Data Service. SN: 8156, doi: 10.5255/UKDA-SN-8156-4
Keywords: weight trajectories, early childhood, family context, Millennium Cohort Study, growth mixture modeling
Citation: dos Santos CS, Picoito J, Nunes C and Loureiro I (2020) Early Individual and Family Predictors of Weight Trajectories From Early Childhood to Adolescence: Results From the Millennium Cohort Study. Front. Pediatr. 8:417. doi: 10.3389/fped.2020.00417
Received: 23 February 2020; Accepted: 17 June 2020;
Published: 11 August 2020.
Edited by:
Mauro Fisberg, Federal University of São Paulo, BrazilReviewed by:
Duška Tješić-Drinković, University of Zagreb, CroatiaYen-Hsuan Ni, National Taiwan University, Taiwan
Copyright © 2020 dos Santos, Picoito, Nunes and Loureiro. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Constança Soares dos Santos, Y3NkLnNhbnRvcyYjeDAwMDQwO2Vuc3AudW5sLnB0