Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Pediatr., 15 January 2019
Sec. Pediatric Neurology

Executive Function in Relation to White Matter in Preterm and Full Term Children

  • 1Department of Pediatrics, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, United States
  • 2Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, CA, United States

Background: Executive function (EF) refers to cognitive abilities used to guide goal-directed behavior. Diffusion Tensor Imaging (DTI) provides quantitative characterization of white matter tracts in the brain. Children with preterm birth often have EF impairments and white matter injury.

Aim: To examine the degree of association between EF scores and white matter fractional anisotropy (FA) as measured by DTI in children born preterm and term

Study design: Cross-sectional study

Subjects: Participants, 9–16 years of age, born preterm (n = 25; mean gestational age 28.6 weeks; mean birth weight 1,191 grams), and full term (n = 20)

Outcome measures: White matter FA analyzed with Tract-Based Spatial Statistics, a technique that generates a skeleton representing the core of white matter tracts throughout the brain. Behavioral scores from EF tasks examining working memory, spatial memory capacity, and multiple skills from the Stockings of Cambridge.

Results: The groups performed comparably on all tasks. In both groups, unfavorable working memory strategy scores were associated with lower FA. Other measures of EF were not associated with whole skeleton FA in either group in either direction.

Conclusions: Strategy score on a spatial working memory task was associated with FA in preterm and full term children, suggesting common underlying neurobiology in both groups. Associations were found in frontal-parietal connections and other major tracts. Lack of associations between other EF tasks and FA may be due to variation in how children accomplish these EF tasks. Future research is required to fully understand the neurobiology of EF in children born preterm.

Introduction

Executive Function (EF) refers to interrelated cognitive abilities (i.e., working memory, response inhibition, cognitive flexibility, and organization/planning) used to guide goal-directed behavior (1). EF skills are associated with academic achievement and social competence (24). Functional neuroimaging studies link EF skills to prefrontal, parietal, and temporal cortices (57). EF performance has also been associated with white matter connections among brain regions (8, 9).

EF impairments are common in preterm children (10, 11). They are attributed, in part, to white matter injury (8, 9), which is common after preterm birth (1215). Associations have been reported between EF skills in childhood and newborn white matter abnormalities on conventional magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) (1619), which provides a qualitative impression of white matter properties.

Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) generates quantitative measures to characterize white matter (20). Fractional anisotropy (FA) measures the degree to which diffusion is anisotropic, reflecting directional coherence of axons within a white matter tract (21, 22). Reduced FA in preterm children compared to full term children has been found in multiple brain regions in the absence of cystic lesions (12, 14, 15) and has been used to infer subtle injury, though not all studies find differences in all tracts (2325). Very few DTI studies of preterm children have focused on EF skills and white matter properties. One DTI study of very low birth weight adolescents found that low FA in the left cingulum and bilateral inferior fronto-occipital fasciculi (IFOF) was significantly correlated with low EF (9). Another DTI study found that diffusivity measures other than FA at term-equivalent age in the inferior occipital and cerebellar regions were associated with impaired EF at age 7 years (26). In contrast, a DTI study of 8-year-old very preterm children found no association between DTI measures and parent-rated EF (27).

Techniques such as resting state functional MRI and structural connectivity may also be combined to better understand the relationship between white matter maturation and the development of functional connectivity among brain networks (28, 29). A study of typical development between ages 2 and 18 years found positive correlations between structural and functional connectivity and that the relationship strengthened with age (28). A study utilizing diffusion MRI with whole-brain mapping of connectivity in 11- to 31-month-old preterm infants found altered connection strength associated with age and preterm birth (30). Older children had stronger connections in tracts involving frontal lobe structures, whereas increased prematurity at birth was related to widespread reductions in connection strength in tracts involving all cortical lobes and several subcortical structures (30).

This exploratory study used DTI in preterm children and adolescents to examine associations between EF skills and FA using Tract-Based Spatial Statistics (TBSS), a whole-brain, mean white matter FA skeleton approach. We focused on FA rather than other DTI metrics, such as axial diffusivity (AD) or radial diffusivity (RD), since both AD and RD are reflected in FA (i.e., FA = AD/RD2). In this sample, we previously reported group differences in FA (25), and significant associations were found between white matter FA in multiple regions and macroscopic injury (25), IQ (25), language (31), and parent-reported behavior symptoms (32). Therefore, we hypothesized that we would find significant associations between measures of EF skills and white matter FA. We suspected significant correlations in tracts that connect frontal and parietal regions, such as the superior longitudinal fasciculus and the arcuate fasciculus, given that other studies have found associations in these tracts or activations in frontal and parietal regions (3336).

Methods

Participants

Participants were 9–16 year olds, born between 1992 and 1999, and participating in a larger multi-site study (37). The study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the Belmont Report, released by the National Commission for the Protection of Human Subjects in Biomedical and Behavioral Research. The protocol (#6985) was approved by the Stanford University IRB. All parents or guardians gave written informed consent and child and adolescent participants gave assent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Preterm subjects (n = 25) were born at ≤34 weeks gestation with birth weight <2,500 grams. Controls were born ≥37 weeks (n = 20). Participants included in the current study are those who had complete EF data and concurrent DTI data. Due to protocol changes in the larger study, not all subjects completed EF testing, resulting in a smaller number of controls. Exclusions included seizures; hydrocephalus; receptive vocabulary score <70; hearing loss; and non-English speaker. Controls were also excluded for language, learning, or psychiatric disorders. Three preterm subjects were excluded due to extremely enlarged ventricles (38). Preterm subjects were recruited by letters to families previously evaluated at the neonatal follow-up clinic. Controls were recruited through ads and group-matched to preterm children for age, sex, and race. There were no differences in maternal education, IQ scores, or handedness. See Table 1. Medical complications are in Supplemental Material.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Participant demographics and behavior scores (N = 45).

Executive Function Measures

We utilized the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (Cambridge Cognition, Ltd, Cambridge, UK), a computerized EF battery used in studies of preterm children (39). Spatial Working Memory (SWM), Spatial Span (SS), and Stockings of Cambridge (SOC) subtests captured different EF constructs. SWM measures the ability to retain and manipulate spatial information. Two outcomes include Strategy score [a low value indicates a systematic approach (40)] and Forgetting errors. SS measures spatial capacity and is a visual-spatial analog of the digit span task. Span length is the longest sequence successfully recalled. SOC measures response inhibition, planning, and organization skills. A high value on Problems solved indicates good planning/organization skills. Additional details are in Supplemental Material.

Statistical Analysis

Chi-square, independent t-tests, and Mann-Whitney U tests were used to evaluate between-group differences on demographic variables and outcome measures.

Imaging Protocols

Details for MRI and DTI protocols/processing were previously reported (32) and are in Supplemental Material. MRI data were acquired on a 3T Signa Excite (GE Medical Systems, Milwaukee, WI). For DTI, a diffusion-weighted, single-shot, spin-echo, echo-planar imaging sequence was used to acquire 60 slices, 2 mm thick, in 30 different diffusion directions (b = 900).

DTI Image Analysis and Post-processing

We used the voxel-wise “Tract Based Spatial Statistics” (TBSS) method from the Oxford Center for Functional MRI of the Brain (FMRIB) Diffusion Toolbox (41). TBSS identifies a core white matter “skeleton” that is anatomically equivalent across subjects. Advantages of TBSS include avoiding spatial smoothing (averaging voxels) and minimizing partial volume effects that can occur when more than one tract goes through a voxel, thereby leading to decreases in anatomic specificity (41). We analyzed FA, a ratio from 0 to 1.

A tensor model was fit to each voxel (42), and FA images were calculated. FA images were registered to the most representative subject in the study (target) using the FMRIB Software Library's (FSL) non-linear registration tool (43). Each subject was aligned to the target; aligned FA images were averaged to create a mean FA map. An FA threshold ≥0.2 was designated to include major white matter pathways and to exclude peripheral tracts. The mean FA image was generated and thinned to create the mean FA-skeleton, a representation of the core of all major white matter tracts common to the group. For each subject, TBSS projected the highest FA on to the FA-skeleton, corresponding to the local tract core.

Statistical Analysis of DTI Data

Analysis of the FA values at the voxel level was conducted using a permutation-based tool for non-parametric statistical thresholding (“Randomize” function in FSL) to assess group related differences (44). This method generates non-parametric, two-sample, unpaired t-tests of FA in subjects compared with controls. We assessed the degree of association between EF scores and FA of the mean skeleton within the preterm group and the control group separately in both the positive and negative directions as well as group by task interactions, covarying by age. We set significance at p < 0.05 after correction for multiple comparisons using Threshold-Free Cluster Enhancement, a method that avoids using an arbitrary threshold for initial cluster-formation or identification of signal (45). Significant clusters of voxels on the FA-skeleton were inspected with reference to John Hopkins University DTI-based white matter atlases (46) to determine tract assignments.

Results

No significant behavioral differences were found between groups on raw scores on any EF measure (Table 1). On voxel-wise analysis, for both preterm and full term groups, SWM strategy scores were significantly negatively associated with the mean FA-skeleton; as strategy scores increased (indicating poor performance), FA decreased. There were no significant positive correlations. There were no significant associations in either direction for the other measures in either group. We also examined interaction effects across both groups; there were no significant group by task interactions.

In both groups, tracts associated with strategy score included the superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLF), cingulum, inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus (IFOF), inferior longitudinal fasciculus (ILF), forceps major, and corticospinal tracts. The preterm group also had associations with the corpus callosum and forceps minor, whereas full term children had additional associations with the anterior thalamic radiations. Figure 1 shows the multiple regions of significant association on axial views of the FA-skeleton for the full term group (upper row) and the preterm group (lower row).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. White matter regions on the fractional anisotropy (FA) skeleton (green) of statistically significant associations between FA and spatial working memory strategy score in full term (upper row, red) and preterm (bottom row, red) groups (p < 0.05, non-parametric permutation test, corrected for multiple comparisons) overlaid on axial slices of the mean FA image (grayscale). Tracts in which significant associations were found include: Both groups: a. cingulum; b. forceps major; c. inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus; d. inferior longitudinal fasciculus; e. superior longitudinal fasciculus; f. corticospinal tract. Preterm group only: g. corpus callosum/forceps minor. Full term group only: h. anterior thalamic radiation. L, left; R, right; A, anterior; P, posterior.

Discussion

In summary, we found that FA in multiple tracts was associated with a single measure of EF—strategy score. Lower FA was associated with poorer strategy in both preterm and full term groups. Similar tracts were identified for both groups, although unique tracts were also associated with each group.

Our results are similar to another report of associations between lower FA and EF measures of problem solving and cognitive flexibility in a preterm sample within the cingulum and bilateral IFOF (9). The cingulum bundles course from the medial temporal lobe to the frontal lobes, and frontal regions have long been implicated in EF studies of typical adults (47, 48) and clinical populations using DTI (4951). The IFOF courses from the occipital lobes through the external capsules to the inferior frontal cortex. A study of very preterm children at age 7 years found that diffusivity measures other than FA at term-equivalent age in similar inferior occipital regions were associated with poor planning ability (26). The associations with these tracts may reflect the importance of visual input to frontal regions during processing of the task.

The association between FA and strategy score in both preterm and full term groups suggests a common neurobiological basis for memory strategy. In both groups, FA in the SLF was also associated with strategy score. These findings are similar to those in a study of typical children age 7–13 years that used the same CANTAB Spatial Working Memory task (34). Another study of typical children, ages 5–17 years, found that higher FA in the SLF was correlated with better set shifting/cognitive flexibility and inhibition (52).

Associations of FA and strategy score were also found within the ILF, forceps major, and corticospinal tracts for both groups. The ILF courses from the occipital to temporal lobes. Adult DTI studies showed associations between the ILF and a memory composite score in healthy adults (48) and working memory in adults with diffuse axonal injury (49). The forceps major extends from the corpus callosum to connect homologous regions of the occipital lobe in each hemisphere. A tractography study of healthy aging in adults found associations between the splenium of the corpus callosum and memory and EF (53). The corticospinal tracts are associated with motor domains; associations may reflect motor planning and actions required to perform the task.

In the preterm group, the corpus callosum and forceps minor were also significantly associated with SWM strategy score. The corpus callosum provides bi-hemispheric connections, and the forceps minor extend from the corpus callosum to connect homologous regions of the anterior frontal lobe in each hemisphere. A study of healthy 8–18 year olds found correlations between FA in frontal-parietal connections and neural activation on functional MRI in the superior frontal sulcus and inferior parietal lobe (54). In the full term group, the anterior thalamic radiations were identified. The ATR connects subcortical regions to the frontal lobe. A DTI study of patients with first-episode psychosis found associations of decreased FA in the ATR, among other regions, and impairments on a measure of cognitive flexibility (55). Subcortical-frontal connections are also important for EF in other DTI studies of children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) (56, 57).

We did not find associations between FA and SS or SOC in either the preterm or full term groups. These findings are in sharp contrast with other studies using the same group of participants and methods (25, 31, 32). These studies identified many positive associations between FA and other domains of function in PT children, including intelligence, language (i.e., linguistic processing speed), reading (i.e., syntactic comprehension and decoding), and behavior problems (i.e., inattention and internalizing symptoms) (25, 31, 32). The tracts associated with SWM strategy show some overlap with those associated with IQ, language, reading, and behavior symptoms, although the areas of correlation with EF are less widespread than those seen with language, reading and behavior outcomes. The reasons for the lack of findings with EF are unclear. Children may use a variety of different strategies for these tasks that differ across groups and/or represent higher individual variation within groups. These strategies may also vary with age and may have further increased the heterogeneity in this sample with wide age range. If these possibilities are true, the associations of EF skills and white matter of any specific tracts may be limited. Children may approach working memory strategy more consistently than they approach other EF skills. The lack of findings with other EF skills may also reflect methodological details. SWM strategy has a greater range than SS total span length or problems solved on the SOC. It is easier to detect correlations when the range of scores is high. Another possibility relates to limitations of TBSS, which focuses on the centers of white matter tracts and may not detect peripheral damage.

Limitations

The study utilized a small, heterogeneous convenience sample of preterm children who may not be representative of all preterm children. The EF tasks did not differentiate the groups behaviorally. The wide age range may have limited the ability to find brain-behavior relationships and group differences, as the groups may have different developmental trajectories for EF skills in middle childhood and early adolescence.

Conclusion

Use of a systematic strategy on a spatial working memory task was correlated with white matter FA in preterm and full term children, suggesting common underlying neurobiology in both groups. Better performance was associated with higher FA. Associations were found in frontal-parietal connections and other major tracts. Our study adds to the relatively small body of literature on EF and white matter in preterm children. Replication with a larger, more homogeneous sample may provide insight into the neurobiology underlying EF in children in general. Unexpectedly, no other associations between EF scores and FA were found in either group. Future work should focus on smaller age groups and adjust tasks and DTI methodology, including advanced imaging and analysis methods such as combined functional MRI and structural connectivity, to better understand the neurobiology of EF.

Author Contributions

IL conceptualized the study, analyzed and interpreted data, drafted the initial manuscript and critically reviewed, and revised the manuscript. JA contributed to conceptualization and interpretation of data, literature review, and critically reviewed and revised the manuscript. HF conceptualized the study, analyzed and interpreted data, and critically reviewed and revised the manuscript. All authors are responsible for the reported work and have approved the final manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by a grant from the National Institutes of Health, Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, RO1 HD046500 to HF, and a Mentored Patient-oriented Research Career Development Award Grant K23HD071971 to IL; NIH Pediatric Research Loan Repayment Program Award to IL; and the Stanford Clinical and Translational Science Award (CTSA) to Spectrum (UL1 TR0001085), sponsored by the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences (NCATS) at the NIH. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIH. The sponsors had no involvement in the study design; or in the collection, analysis and interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to submit the manuscript for publication. We thank the children and families who participated in our study.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fped.2018.00418/full#supplementary-material

Abbreviations

AD, axial diffusivity; ADHD, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; DTI, diffusion tensor imaging; EF, executive function; FA, fractional anisotropy; FMRIB, functional magnetic resonance imaging of the brain; FSL, FMRIB Software Library; IFOF, inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus; ILF, inferior longitudinal fasciculus; MRI, magnetic resonance imaging; RD, radial diffusivity; SLF, superior longitudinal fasciculus; SOC, Stockings of Cambridge; SS, Spatial Span; SWM, spatial working memory; TBSS, tract based spatial statistics.

References

1. Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn Psychol. (2000) 41:49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Alduncin N, Huffman LC, Feldman HM, Loe IM. Executive function is associated with social competence in preschool-aged children born preterm or full term. Early Hum Dev. (2014) 90:299–306. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2014.02.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Loe IM, Feldman HM, Huffman LC. Executive function mediates effects of gestational age on functional outcomes and behavior in preschoolers. J Dev Behav Pediatr. (2014) 35:323–33. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Loe IM, Luna B, Bledsoe IO, Yeom KW, Fritz BL, Feldman HM. Oculomotor assessments of executive function in preterm children. J Pediatr. (2012) 161:427–33.e1. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2012.02.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Luna B, Padmanabhan A, O'Hearn K. What has fMRI told us about the development of cognitive control through adolescence? Brain Cogn. (2010) 72:101–13. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Tau GZ, Peterson BS. Normal development of brain circuits. Neuropsychopharmacology (2010) 35:147–68. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Esposito M. From cognitive to neural models of working memory. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. (2007) 362:761–72. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2007.2086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Woodward LJ, Clark CAC, Pritchard VE, Anderson PJ, Inder TE. Neonatal white matter abnormalities predict global executive function impairment in children born very preterm. Dev Neuropsychol. (2011) 36:22–41. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2011.540530

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Skranes J, Lohaugen GC, Martinussen M, Indredavik MS, Dale AM, Haraldseth O, et al. White matter abnormalities and executive function in children with very low birth weight. Neuroreport (2009) 20:263–6. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e32832027fe

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Anderson PJ. Neuropsychological outcomes of children born very preterm. Semin Fetal Neonatal Med. (2014) 19:90–6. doi: 10.1016/j.siny.2013.11.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Aarnoudse-Moens CSH, Weisglas-Kuperus N, van Goudoever JB, Oosterlaan J. Meta-Analysis of neurobehavioral outcomes in very preterm and/or very low birth weight children. Pediatrics (2009) 124:717–28. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-2816

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Vangberg TR, Skranes J, Dale AM, Martinussen M, Brubakk AM, Haraldseth O. Changes in white matter diffusion anisotropy in adolescents born prematurely. Neuroimage (2006) 32:1538–48. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.04.230

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Yung A, Poon G, Qiu D-Q, Chu J, Lam B, Leung C, et al. White matter volume and anisotropy in preterm children: a pilot study of neurocognitive correlates. Pediatr Res. (2007) 61:732–6. doi: 10.1203/pdr.0b013e31805365db

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Volpe JJ. Brain injury in premature infants: a complex amalgam of destructive and developmental disturbances. Lancet Neurol. (2009) 8:110–24. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(08)70294-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Back SA, Riddle A, McClure MM. Maturation-dependent vulnerability of perinatal white matter in premature birth. Stroke (2007) 38(2 Suppl.):724–30. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000254729.27386.05

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

16. Woodward LJ, Edgin JO, Thompson D, Inder TE. Object working memory deficits predicted by early brain injury and development in the preterm infant. Brain (2005) 128:2578–87. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh618

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Edgin JO, Inder TE, Anderson PJ, Hood KM, Clark CA, Woodward LJ. Executive functioning in preschool children born very preterm: relationship with early white matter pathology. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2008) 14:90–101. doi: 10.1017/S1355617708080053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Luu TM, Ment LR, Allan W, Schneider K, Vohr BR. Executive and memory function in adolescents born very preterm. Pediatrics (2011) 127:e639–46. doi: 10.1542/peds.2010-1421

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Murray AL, Scratch SE, Thompson DK, Inder TE, Doyle LW, Anderson JF, et al. Neonatal brain pathology predicts adverse attention and processing speed outcomes in very preterm and/or very low birth weight children. Neuropsychology (2014) 28:552–62. doi: 10.1037/neu0000071

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Feldman HM, Yeatman JD, Lee ES, Barde LH, Gaman-Bean S. Diffusion tensor imaging: a review for pediatric researchers and clinicians. J Dev Behav Pediatr. (2010) 31:346–56. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e3181dcaa8b

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Asato M, Terwilliger R, Woo J, Luna B. white matter development in adolescence: a DTI study. Cereb Cortex (2010) 20:2122–31. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp282

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Yeatman J, Dougherty R, Myall N, Wandell B, Feldman H. Tract profiles of white matter properties: automating fiber-tract quantification. PLoS ONE (2012) 7:e49790. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0049790

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Frye RE, Hasan K, Malmberg B, Desouza L, Swank P, Smith K, et al. Superior longitudinal fasciculus and cognitive dysfunction in adolescents born preterm and at term. Dev Med Child Neurol. (2010) 52:760–6. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2010.03633.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Allin MP, Kontis D, Walshe M, Wyatt J, Barker GJ, Kanaan RA, et al. White matter and cognition in adults who were born preterm. PLoS ONE (2011) 6:e24525. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0024525

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Feldman H, Lee E, Loe I, Yeom K, Grill-Spector K, Luna B. White matter microstructure on diffusion tensor imaging is associated with conventional magnetic resonance imaging findings and cognitive function in adolescents born preterm. Dev Med Child Neurol. (2012) 54:809–14. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2012.04378.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Thompson DK, Lee KJ, Egan GF, Warfield SK, Doyle LW, Anderson PJ, et al. Regional white matter microstructure in very preterm infants: predictors and 7 year outcomes. Cortex (2014) 52:60–74. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2013.11.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Duerden EG, Card D, Lax ID, Donner EJ, Taylor MJ. Alterations in frontostriatal pathways in children born very preterm. Dev Med Child Neurol. (2013) 55:952–8. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.12198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Hagmann P, Sporns O, Madan N, Cammoun L, Pienaar R, Wedeen VJ, et al. White matter maturation reshapes structural connectivity in hte late developing human brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2010) 107:19067–72. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1009073107

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Shirer WR, Ryali S, Rykhlevskaia E, Menon V, Greicius MD. Decoding subject-driven cognitive sates with whole-brain connectivity patterns. Cereb Cortex (2012) 22:158–65. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Pandit AS, Robinson E, Aljabar P, Ball G, Gousias IS, Wang Z, et al. Whole-brain mapping of structural connectivity in infants reveals altered connection strength associated with growth and preterm birth. Cereb Cortex (2014) 24:2324–33. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Feldman HM, Lee ES, Yeatman JD, Yeom KW. Language and reading skills in school-aged children and adolescents born preterm are associated with white matter properties on diffusion tensor imaging. Neuropsychologia (2012) 50:3348–62. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.10.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Loe IM, Lee ES, Feldman HM. Attention and internalizing behaviors in relation to white matter in children born preterm. J Dev Behav Pediatr. (2013) 34:156–64. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e3182842122

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Kalpakidou AK, Allin MP, Walshe M, Giampietro V, McGuire PK, Rifkin L, et al. Functional neuroanatomy of executive function after neonatal brain injury in adults who were born very preterm. PLoS ONE (2014) 9:e113975. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0113975

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Vestergaard M, Madsen KS, Baaré WFC, Skimminge A, Ejersbo LR, Ramsøy TZ, et al. White matter microstructure in superior longitudinal fasciculus associated with spatial working memory performance in children. J Cogn Neurosci. (2010) 23:2135–46. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21592

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Klingberg T, Forssberg H, Westerberg H. Increased brain activity in frontal and parietal cortex underlies the development of visuospatial working memory capacity during childhood. J Cogn Neurosci. (2002) 14:1–10. doi: 10.1162/089892902317205276

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Klingberg T. Development of a superior frontal-intraparietal network for visuo-spatial working memory. Neuropsychologia (2006) 44:2171–7. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.11.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Loe I, Lee E, Luna B, Feldman H. Behavior problems of 9-16 year old preterm children: biological, sociodemograhic, and intellectual contributions. Early Hum Dev. (2011) 87:247–52. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2011.01.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Myall NJ, Yeom KW, Yeatman JD, Gaman-Bean S, Feldman HM. Case series: fractional anisotropy along the trajectory of selected white matter tracts in adolescents born preterm with ventricular dilation. J Child Neurol. (2013) 28:774–80. doi: 10.1177/0883073812449693

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Luciana M, Lindeke L, Georgieff M, Mills M, Nelson CA. Neurobehavioral evidence for working-memory deficits in school-aged children with histories of prematurity. Dev Med Child Neurol. (1999) 41:521–33. doi: 10.1017/S0012162299001140

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Van Asselen M, Kessels RP, Neggers SF, Kappelle LJ, Frijns CJ, Postma A. Brain areas involved in spatial working memory. Neuropsychologia (2006) 44:1185–94. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Johansen-Berg H, Rueckert D, Nichols TE, Mackay CE, et al. Tract-based spatial statistics: voxelwise analysis of multi-subject diffusion data. NeuroImage (2006) 31:1487–505. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Basser PJ, Mattiello J, LeBihan D. MR diffusion tensor spectroscopy and imaging. Biophys J. (1994) 66:259–67. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3495(94)80775-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Andersson JLR, Jenkinson M, Smith S. Non-linear Registration, Aka Spatial Normalisation. Oxford: University of Oxford (2007).

Google Scholar

44. Nichols TE, Holmes AP. Non-parametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: a primer with examples. Hum Brain Mapp. (2002) 15:1–25. doi: 10.1002/hbm.1058

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Smith SM, Nichols TE. Threshold-free cluster enhancement: addressing problems of smoothing, threshold dependence and localisation in cluster inference. Neuroimage (2009) 44:83–98. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Mori S, Wakana S, van Zijl PC, Nagae-Poetscher LM. MRI Atlas of Human White Matter. Amsterdam: Elsevier (2005). p. 1–284.

Google Scholar

47. Charlton RA, Landau S, Schiavone F, Barrick TR, Clark CA, Markus HS, et al. A structural equation modeling investigation of age-related variance in executive function and DTI measured white matter damage. Neurobiol Aging (2008) 29:1547–55. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2007.03.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Sasson E, Doniger GM, Pasternak O, Tarrasch R, Assaf Y. White matter correlates of cognitive domains in normal aging with diffusion tensor imaging. Front Neurosci. (2013) 7:32. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Gu L, Li J, Feng DF, Cheng ET, Li DC, Yang XQ, et al. Detection of white matter lesions in the acute stage of diffuse axonal injury predicts long-term cognitive impairments: a clinical diffusion tensor imaging study. J Trauma Acute Care Surg. (2013) 74:242–7. doi: 10.1097/TA.0b013e3182684fe8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Tartaglia MC, Zhang Y, Racine C, Laluz V, Neuhaus J, Chao L, et al. Executive dysfunction in frontotemporal dementia is related to abnormalities in frontal white matter tracts. J Neurol. (2012) 259:1071–80. doi: 10.1007/s00415-011-6300-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Winston GP, Stretton J, Sidhu MK, Symms MR, Thompson PJ, Duncan JS. Structural correlates of impaired working memory in hippocampal sclerosis. Epilepsia (2013) 54:1143–53. doi: 10.1111/epi.12193

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Urger SE, De Bellis MD, Hooper SR, Woolley DP, Chen SD, Provenzale J. The superior longitudinal fasciculus in typically developing children and adolescents: diffusion tensor imaging and neuropsychological correlates. J Child Neurol. (2015) 30:9–20. doi: 10.1177/0883073813520503

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Voineskos AN, Rajji TK, Lobaugh NJ, Miranda D, Shenton ME, Kennedy JL, et al. Age-related decline in white matter tract integrity and cognitive performance: a DTI tractography and structural equation modeling study. Neurobiol Aging (2012) 33:21–34. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.02.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Olesen PJ, Nagy Z, Westerberg H, Klingberg T. Combined analysis of DTI and fMRI data reveals a joint maturation of white and grey matter in a fronto-parietal network. Cogn Brain Res. (2003) 18:48–57. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2003.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Pérez-Iglesias R, Tordesillas-Gutiérrez D, McGuire PK, Barker GJ, Roiz-Santiañez R, Mata I, et al. White matter integrity and cognitive impairment in first-episode psychosis. Am J Psychiatry (2010) 167:451–8. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09050716

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Casey BJ, Epstein JN, Buhle J, Liston C, Davidson MC, Tonev ST, et al. Frontostriatal connectivity and its role in cognitive control in parent-child dyads with ADHD. Am J Psychiatry (2007) 164:1729–36. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.06101754

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Sheridan MA, Hinshaw S, D'Esposito M. Efficiency of the prefrontal cortex during working memory in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry (2007) 46:1357–66. doi: 10.1097/chi.0b013e31812eecf7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: preterm birth, premature birth, executive function, diffusion tensor imaging, white matter, working memory

Citation: Loe IM, Adams JN and Feldman HM (2019) Executive Function in Relation to White Matter in Preterm and Full Term Children. Front. Pediatr. 6:418. doi: 10.3389/fped.2018.00418

Received: 21 September 2018; Accepted: 18 December 2018;
Published: 15 January 2019.

Edited by:

Andrea Guzzetta, University of Pisa, Italy

Reviewed by:

Chiara Nosarti, King's College London, United Kingdom
Vanessa Kyriakopoulou, King's College London, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2019 Loe, Adams and Feldman. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Irene M. Loe, aWxvZUBzdGFuZm9yZC5lZHU=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.