Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Syst. Neurosci., 04 September 2019

Neuronal Avalanches in Input and Associative Layers of Auditory Cortex

  • 1Department of Biology, University of Maryland, College Park, College Park, MD, United States
  • 2Institute for Systems Research, University of Maryland, College Park, College Park, MD, United States
  • 3Section on Critical Brain Dynamics, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, MD, United States

The primary auditory cortex processes acoustic sequences for the perception of behaviorally meaningful sounds such as speech. Sound information arrives at its input layer four from where activity propagates to associative layer 2/3. It is currently not known whether there is a characteristic organization of neuronal population activity across layers and sound levels during sound processing. Here, we identify neuronal avalanches, which in theory and experiments have been shown to maximize dynamic range and optimize information transfer within and across networks, in primary auditory cortex. We used in vivo 2-photon imaging of pyramidal neurons in cortical layers L4 and L2/3 of mouse A1 to characterize the populations of neurons that were active spontaneously, i.e., in the absence of a sound stimulus, and those recruited by single-frequency tonal stimuli at different sound levels. Single-frequency sounds recruited neurons of widely ranging frequency selectivity in both layers. We defined neuronal ensembles as neurons being active within or during successive temporal windows at the temporal resolution of our imaging. For both layers, neuronal ensembles were highly variable in size during spontaneous activity as well as during sound presentation. Ensemble sizes distributed according to power laws, the hallmark of neuronal avalanches, and were similar across sound levels. Avalanches activated by sound were composed of neurons with diverse tuning preference, yet with selectivity independent of avalanche size. Our results suggest that optimization principles identified for avalanches guide population activity in L4 and L2/3 of auditory cortex during and in-between stimulus processing.

Introduction

The primary auditory cortex (A1) is central to encoding sound sequences (Nelken et al., 2003; Bartho et al., 2009; Francis et al., 2018), yet several major aspects of sound processing in A1 are currently not well understood. First, the selectivity of single A1 neurons to stimulus features such as sound frequency changes with sound level, i.e., across the dynamic range, while perceptual discrimination performance is largely level-invariant (Jesteadt et al., 1977; Wier et al., 1977; Bernstein and Oxenham, 2006). Second, responses of neurons in primary sensory areas are well-established to be unreliable over repeated trials (Shadlen and Newsome, 1998) and for A1 a large part of this variability underlies fluctuations in local population activity (Deweese and Zador, 2004). It was recently shown that auditory stimuli are encoded by populations of A1 neurons (Bathellier et al., 2012; Francis et al., 2018). Here, we sought to identify characteristics of active populations in A1.

Identifying population dynamics central to sound processing needs to take into account that A1 is composed of several cortical layers each transforming incoming information (Atencio and Schreiner, 2009; Atencio et al., 2009). Specifically, sensory inputs arriving in layer 4 (L4) are relayed to layers 2/3 (L2/3) via divergent and heterogeneous feed-forward projections (Meng et al., 2017). In this hierarchy, it is not well understood how population dynamics in L4 contributes to neuronal responses in L2/3 raising the general question of how neural representations of sensory stimuli are reliably transmitted through multilayer networks.

At least two lines of evidence suggest that ensemble dynamics could support level-independent reliable sound encoding across networks of neurons. First, network dynamics that support a large dynamic range, i.e., that differentiate sounds at both high and low levels, should be of particular interest to auditory processing where sound amplitude can vary over many orders of magnitude. Simulations (Kinouchi and Copelli, 2006; Shriki and Yellin, 2016) and experiments in vivo (Gautam et al., 2015) and in vitro (Shew et al., 2009, 2010, 2011; Shew and Plenz, 2013) have shown that cortical networks that display neuronal avalanches (Beggs and Plenz, 2003; Petermann et al., 2009; Shriki et al., 2013; Bellay et al., 2015) maximize their dynamic range for input processing as well as their information capacity (for review, see Chialvo, 2010; Shew and Plenz, 2013). Neuronal avalanches are identified by power law relationships for the size and duration of activity in neuronal groups which naturally links to a second line of evidence identifying conditions for which information transmission between complex networks is maximized. Theory suggests that complex networks exchange maximal information if their complexity, quantified by their respective power law relationships, is matched (West et al., 2008; Aquino et al., 2010, 2011). Accordingly, we hypothesize that if A1 population dynamics are governed by avalanche statistics, these statistics would be similar for L4 and L2/3.

Here, using in vivo 2-photon imaging of pyramidal neurons in mouse A1, we identify common dynamical principles in sound encoding for both layers that allow for the encoding of sounds in neuronal populations in line with predictions for neuronal avalanches.

Materials and Methods

All procedures followed the University of Maryland College Park animal use regulations. Mice were housed under a reversed 12 h-light/12 h-dark light cycle with ad libitum access to food and water. Imaging experiments were generally performed near the end of the light and beginning of the dark cycle. Age range at the time of experiments extended from P46-P100 (75 ± 21 days, mean ± SD). Mice of both sexes were used (n = 12, 8 Male, 4 Female) based on their availability not by any biased selection.

Animals

Mice used for this study expressed the genetically encoded calcium indicator (GCaMP6s) (Chen et al., 2013). GCaMP6s was introduced in one of the following ways. In a first line of experiments, we injected an adeno-associated virus (AAV1) into primary auditory cortex (A1) of C57/BL6 mice (n = 5) for delivery of a calcium indicator with co-expression of a structural marker mRuby2 (Rose et al., 2016). Both proteins were under control of the synapsin promoter. Virus (AAV1.hSyn1.mRuby2.GSG.P2A.GCaMP6s.WPRE.SV40; titer: 3 × 1013) was obtained from the University of Pennsylvania Vector Core. Observed expression in these mice was primarily limited to supragranular and infragranular layers and was mostly absent in L4. Only data acquired from L2/3 in these animals was included. In a second line of experiments, we used transgenic mice that expressed GCaMP6s either under the Thy1 promoter (Dana et al., 2014, Jax: 024274, GP4.3) or a transgenic mouse line that conditionally expresses GCaMP6s when crossed to a mouse driver line expressing Cre recombinase under the control of Emx1 (Gorski et al., 2002; Madisen et al., 2015, GCaMP6s mouse: Jax: 024115; Emx1-Cre mouse: Jax: 005628). Both L2/3 and L4 datasets were acquired from the transgenic mice. The transgenic L2/3 and L4 data sets were always obtained sequentially in the same mouse, meaning the experiment (sound presentations) was conducted in one layer and then sequentially conducted in the other layer. The two transgenic lines exhibited robust indicator expression in both L4 and L2/3. The number of neurons identified in L2/3 was 210 ± 43 (mean ± std) in transgenic mice and 238 ± 38 (mean ± SD) in mice with viral expression. These distributions were not significantly different (p > 0.16, two-sample t-test). The number of neurons identified in L4 from transgenic mice was 99 ± 52 (mean ± SD). Since, we found no significant differences of indicator expression within layers when we compared across transgenic mouse lines and animals with viral expression, we combined the data from different sources according to laminar position. For a subset of the experiments (13), we recorded the pupil of the mouse to ensure the animals maintained close to an intermediate arousal state (McGinley et al., 2015a, b). The pupil videos were analyzed by measuring the pupil diameter in each video frame using “imfindcircles” in MATLAB. Overall there were no abnormalities in mouse arousal state (Supplementary Figure S1).

Surgery and Animal Preparation

Mice were given a subcutaneous injection of dexamethasone (5 mg/kg) at least 2 h prior to surgery to reduce potential inflammation and edema from surgery. Mice were deeply anesthetized using isoflurane (5% induction, 2% for maintenance) and given subcutaneous injections of atropine (0.2 mg/kg) and cefazolin (500 mg/kg). Internal body temperature was maintained at 37.5°C using a feedback-controlled heating blanket. The scalp fur was trimmed using scissors and any remaining fur was removed using Nair. The scalp was disinfected with alternating swabs of 70% ethanol and betadine. A patch of skin was removed, the underlying bone was cleared of connective tissue using a bone curette, the temporal muscle was detached from the skull and pushed aside, and the skull was thoroughly cleaned and dried. A thin layer of cyanoacrylate glue (VetBond) adhesive was applied to the exposed skull surface and a custom machined titanium head plate (based on the design described in Guo et al. (2014) was affixed to the skull overlying the auditory cortex using VetBond followed by dental acrylic (C&B Metabond). A circular craniotomy (∼3 mm diameter) was made in the center opening of the head plate and the patch of bone was removed. For wild-type mice, virus (AAV1-syn-mRuby2-GC6s) was loaded into beveled glass pipettes and injected slowly into the areas corresponding to primary auditory cortex in 3–5 sites (∼30 nL/site; ∼250–300 μm from the surface; ∼2–3 min/each injection site). Pipettes were left in place for at least 5 min after completion of each injection to prevent backflow. Then, a chronic imaging window was implanted. The window consisted of a stack of 2–3 mm diameter coverslips glued with optical adhesive (Norland 71, Edmund Optics) to a 5 mm diameter coverslip. The edges of the window between the glass and the skull were sealed with a silicone elastomer (Kwik-Sil) and then covered with dental acrylic. The entire implant except for the imaging window was then coated with black dental cement created by mixing standard white powder (Dentsply) with iron oxide powder (AlphaChemical, 3:1 ratio) (Goldey et al., 2014). Meloxicam (0.5 mg/kg) and a supplemental dose of dexamethasone were provided subcutaneously as a post-operative analgesic. Animals were allowed to recover for at least 1 week prior to the beginning of experiments.

Acoustic Stimulation

Sound stimuli were synthesized in MATLAB using custom software (courtesy of P. Watkins, UMD), passed through a multifunction processor (RX6, TDT), attenuated (PA5, Programmable Attenuator), and delivered via ES1 speaker placed ∼5 cm directly in front of the mouse. The sound system was calibrated between 2.5 and 80 kHz and showed a flat (± 3 dB) spectrum over this range. Overall sound pressure level (SPL) at 0 dB attenuation was ∼90 dB SPL (for tones). Sounds were played at three sound levels (40, 60, and 80 dB SPL). Auditory stimuli consisted of sinusoidal amplitude-modulated (SAM) tones (20 Hz modulation, cosine phase), ranging from 3 to 48 kHz. For wide-field imaging, the frequency resolution of the stimuli was 1 tone/octave; for 2-photon imaging, the frequency resolution was 2 tones/octave (0.5 octave spacing). Each of these tonal stimuli was repeated 5 times with a 4–6 s interstimulus interval for a total of either 75 (wide-field) or 135 (2-photon) iterations.

Wide-Field Imaging

In order to construct sound-evoked response maps using wide-field imaging, awake mice were placed into a plastic tube with a head restraint system similar to that described by Guo et al. (2014). Blue excitation light was provided by an LED (470 nm, Thorlabs) or xenon-arc lamp (Lambda LS, Sutter Instruments) equipped with an excitation filter (470 nm CWL, 40 nm FWHM; Chroma ET470/40x) and directed toward the cranial window. Emitted light was collected through a tandem lens combination (Ratzlaff and Grinvald, 1991) consisting of a 55 mm lens and 85 mm lens affixed to the camera and passed through a longpass (495 nm cutoff, Chroma Q495lp) followed by a bandpass emission filter (525 nm CWL; 50 nm FWHM; Chroma HQ525/50). Images were acquired using StreamPix software (NorPix) controlling a CoolSNAP HQ2 CCD camera (Photometrics). After acquiring an image of the surface vasculature, the focal plane was advanced to a depth corresponding to ∼300–400 μm below the brain surface. One trial of stimulation consisted of ∼1–2 s of quiet, followed by sound onset (3–48 kHz frequency; 1 octave spacing; 1 s duration; 20 Hz modulation rate; 40, 60, and 80 dB SPL) then 1–2 s of quiet. Each frequency-level combination was randomly repeated five times for a total of 75 iterations. Inter-trial interval was ∼10–15 s. Acquisition of each frame was individually triggered and synchronized with the sound presentation using the Ephus software suite1 (Suter et al., 2010).

2-Photon Imaging

To study cellular neuronal activity using 2-photon imaging, we used a scanning microscope (Bergamo II series, B248, Thorlabs) coupled to a pulsed femtosecond Ti:Sapphire 2-photon laser with dispersion compensation (Vision S, Coherent). The microscope was controlled by ThorImageLS software. The laser was tuned to a wavelength of λ = 940 nm in order to simultaneously excite GCaMP6s and mRuby2. Red and green signals were collected through a 16 × 0.8 NA microscope objective (Nikon). Emitted photons were directed through 525/50-25 (green) and 607/70-25 (red) band pass filters onto GaAsP photomultiplier tubes. The field of view was 370 μm × 370 μm. Imaging frames of 512 × 512 pixels (0.52 μm2 pixel size) were acquired at 30 Hz by bi-directional scanning of an 8 kHz resonant scanner. Beam turnarounds at the edges of the image were blanked with a Pockels cell. The average power for imaging in both L2/3 and L4 was < ∼70 mW, measured at the sample plane.

Data Analysis

Wide-field image sequences were analyzed using custom routines written in Matlab (MathWorks). Images were parsed into trial-based epochs in which each frame sequence represented a single trial consisting of the presentation of a single sound frequency-intensity combination. For each trial, response amplitude (ΔF/F0) as a function of time was determined for each pixel using the formula [(FF0)/F0] where F corresponds to the time varying fluorescence signal at a given pixel and F0 was estimated by averaging the fluorescence values over 4 frames (∼1 s) prior to sound onset for a given trial and pixel. For construction of sound-evoked response maps, the amplitude of the ΔF/F0 pixel response during 1 s after stimulus onset (∼4 frames) was averaged across time and repetitions yielding an average response magnitude that was assigned to each pixel. Responsive areas in the average response maps were defined on a pixel-by-pixel basis as pixels in which the average brightness of the pixel during the 1 s after stimulus onset exceeded 2 standard deviations of the pixel brightness during the 1 s before the stimulus across stimulus repetitions.

2-Photon Image Analysis

Image sequences were corrected for x–y drifts and movement artifacts using either the TurboReg in ImageJ (Thevenaz et al., 1998; Schindelin et al., 2012) or discrete Fourier transform registration (Guizar-Sicairos et al., 2008) implemented in Matlab (MathWorks) taking advantage of mRuby2 labeled neurons. Neurons were identified manually from the average image of the motion corrected sequence. Ring-like regions of interest (ROI) boundaries were drawn based on the method described in Chen et al. (2013). Overlapping ROI pixels (due to closely juxtaposed neurons) were excluded from analysis. For each labeled neuron, a raw fluorescence signal over time (F) was extracted from the ROI overlying the soma. The mean fluorescence for each neuron was calculated across frames and converted to a relative fluorescence measure (ΔF/F0), where ΔF = (FF0). F0 was estimated by using a sliding window that calculated the average fluorescence of points less than the 50th percentile during the previous 10-s window (300 frames). Neuropil (NP) subtraction was performed on all soma ROIs (Peron et al., 2015). In short, the neuropil ROI was drawn based on the outer boundary of the soma ROI and extended from 1 pixel beyond the soma ROI outer boundary to 15 μm excluding any pixels assigned to neighboring somata. Thus, the final ΔF/F0 used for analysis was calculated as ΔF/F0 = (ΔF/F0)soma − [α × (ΔF/F0)NP], where we used α = 0.9 (Peron et al., 2015) to reduce fluorescence contamination from the neuropil. Neurons in which the ΔF/F0 signal was significantly modulated by sound presentation were defined by ANOVA (p < 0.01) across baseline (pre-stimulus) and all sound presentation periods. Extraction of spikes from neuropil-corrected ΔF/F0 traces was performed using the Suite2P implementation of OASIS without an L1 penalty (Pachitariu et al., 2018). Suite2P generates a spike probability for each neuron in each imaging frame (λ). This continuous variable was then thresholded (λthr) to yield a spike raster (Figures 3A, 4A). Single neuron receptive fields (RF) were determined as the average ΔF/F0 response to each frequency-intensity combination across 5 stimulus repetitions during the stimulus window (1 s). Best frequency (BF) for each neuron was determined as the center of mass of the RF. Previous studies (Steinmetz et al., 2017) have reported epileptiform activity in Emx1-Cre mice. To assess if our mice exhibit epileptiform activity, we have followed the procedure outlined by Steinmetz et al. to calculate the mean fluorescence of the entire 2-photon FOV over time and then analyze the peaks of this trace. Epileptiform activity would manifest as rapid consecutive peaks in the mean fluorescence trace, quantified by analyzing the prominence and width of peaks identified from MATLAB’s “findpeaks” function. Epileptiform activity would be evident from an easily identifiable cluster of points with high prominence values at low width (Steinmetz et al., 2017). It is evident from our results (Supplementary Figure S2) that all the mice, including Emx1-Cre, have no identifiable epileptiform activity.

Neuronal Ensemble and Avalanche Analysis

Neuronal ensembles were defined based on contiguous frames in which at least 1 neuron was active (Figure 3B) following the approach by Bellay et al. (2015). Thus, ensembles were bounded by periods of silence in the population activity. Ensembles typically contained a varying number of active pyramidal neurons which could be active for only one or numerous frames within an ensemble. Ensemble size was defined as the number of neurons active summed over the number of frames each neuron was active. Ensemble duration, also called ensemble lifetime was defined as the number of frames constituting an ensemble. We note that like thresholding in electrophysiological recordings, the specific value of λthr determines the number of spatiotemporal activity ensembles. If λthr is too low, most neurons are deemed active all the time, resulting in a low number of very large activity ensembles. Conversely, if λthr is too high, most neurons are considered inactive, again reducing the number of ensembles. Following the approach by Bellay (Bellay et al., 2015), we set λthr for each experiment to maximize the number of ensembles. Our results were not affected by small variations of λthr as demonstrated previously (Bellay et al., 2015). For analysis of evoked responses, we looked specifically at ensembles that were initiated during the stimulus presentation. Thus, an ensemble that was initiated during the 1 s stimulus window, even if it continued after stimulus offset, was still categorized as an evoked ensemble.

To test if ensembles exhibit statistical properties of neuronal avalanches, we analyzed the probability distributions of both ensemble size and ensemble duration. First, the distributions for both size and duration were binned logarithmically, and probability density functions were obtained. For both the size and duration, we fit a power law or exponential distribution. We compared these fits and estimated parameters by calculating the log-likelihood ratio (LLR) by likelihood maximization as described previously (Clauset et al., 2009; Klaus et al., 2011). Slopes of the power law distributions (alpha values) were obtained using Kolmogorov–Smirnov goodness-of-fit to minimize the KS distance between the empirical distribution and a theoretical distribution for a range of alpha values (Clauset et al., 2009; Klaus et al., 2011). For controls, shuffled ensemble distributions were calculated from thresholded and shuffled spike density rasters using the same analysis outlined above. For shuffled rasters, each neuron separately had its spike probability estimates randomly permuted in time. This means the per-frame spike probabilities were maintained for each neuron, therefore maintaining the total number of active frames for each neuron. However, the individual temporal shuffling of neurons abolishes any temporal correlations between neurons. The shuffling process was repeated 10 times and the shuffled raster that produced the maximum number of ensembles was used.

To assess scaling of power laws with respect to stimulus sound level, we produced plots of P(s)sα vs. sLb, where variable s indicates ensemble size, P(s) indicates probability of ensemble size s, α is the best fit slope from the original ensemble distributions, L is the stimulus sound level, and b is the scaling factor. Values of b were iterated between −0.5 and 0.5 (0.001 step size) until the error between the three sound level distributions was minimized.

Pairwise cross-correlation analysis was computed using zero-lag correlation of the λ estimates for all active neurons across the entire imaging session.

Results

To compare single neurons and neuronal ensembles responding to auditory stimuli, we used in vivo 2-photon intracellular calcium imaging to sequentially measure activity of primarily pyramidal neurons expressing GCaMP6s in layer 2/3 (L2/3) and layer 4 (L4) in awake mice (n = 12 mice; Figure 1A). Throughout the experiment the awake animal rested under the microscope while listening passively to semi-random presentations of 1 s short tonal sounds at 9 frequencies (3–48 kHz, two tones/octave) and three different sound pressure levels (40, 60, and 80 dB) separated by 4–6 s. Each stimulus was repeated five times over the course of the experiment. We identified primary auditory cortex (A1) using widefield imaging (Figure 1B). We then imaged the activity of neurons in L4 and L2/3 by imaging in different focal planes (Figures 1C,D). For each neuron we extracted spike trains using standard methods (Pachitariu et al., 2018).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Imaging A1 population activity in the awake mouse. (A) Cartoon of awake mouse under the microscope. (B) Wide-field functional imaging identifies A1. Average activation maps from four different sound frequencies and thresholded activity overlaid on image of brain surface. Tonotopic gradient indicates location of A1. Scale bar = 1 mm. (C) Imaging L4 and L2/3. Histogram shows distribution of imaging plane depths across all experiments. (D) Representative fields of view from L2/3 (left) and L4 (right) showing GCaMP6s expression present in both layers. Scale bar: 100 μm. (E) Sound-evoked intracellular calcium responses for three pyramidal neurons in A1 to nine different frequencies (column), and three sound levels (row). Each black line indicates a single trial; five repeats per condition, red line indicates sound stimulus onset. Right: Frequency response areas.

First, we characterized single neuron receptive fields (RF) from the responses to single tones. Many neurons recorded in each layer were responsive to sound, i.e., showed an increase in firing during the stimulus period. Figure 1E shows three examples of RFs from A1 quantifying the change in stimulus specificity as a function of sound level. Stimulus specificity was found to broaden with an increase in sound volume (Figure 1E, left), to exist only at a particular sound level (Figure 1E, middle), or to broaden with a decrease in sound volume (Figure 1E, right). Simultaneously imaged neurons in both layers showed heterogeneity of tuning in their responses (Figure 1E) as reported previously in anesthetized and awake mice (Bandyopadhyay et al., 2010; Rothschild et al., 2010; Winkowski and Kanold, 2013; Kanold et al., 2014; Maor et al., 2016; Liu et al., 2019).

Population Activity in A1 Shows High Variability

Given that observed local tuning heterogeneity of the imaged population is based on time-averaged measures, we sought to gain insight into statistical characteristics of the active neurons at a finer temporal scale. We first analyzed the properties of neuronal populations during ongoing activity in the absence of sound presentation. Ongoing activity could show periods of relative quiescence and periods of higher activity (Figure 2A). On average ongoing activity was largely similar in both layers. Average firing rate was ∼1.5 spikes/s (L4: 1.8 ± 1.11; L2/3: 1.83 ± 1.08; p > 0.84). Firing in both layers was highly irregular as indicated by CVs between 1 and 2, with most irregular firing found in L4 neurons (Figure 2B). Despite this irregularity, the distributions of pairwise cross-correlation were heavy-tailed with higher correlations found in L2/3 (Figure 2C). These positive correlations indicate that neurons share common input and/or interact over distances during ongoing activity. These results are consistent with in vitro studies showing common inputs and connections in L2/3 of A1 (Oswald et al., 2009; Levy and Reyes, 2012; Watkins et al., 2014; Meng et al., 2017).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. A1 activity is variable and correlated. (A) Cumulative probability distributions of average spike rate for all recorded neurons in L2/3 (blue) and L4 (orange) during ongoing activity (prate > 0.84, ranksum). (B) Corresponding cumulative probability distribution of inferred CV inter-spike interval (CVISI) for all neurons in L4 (orange) and L2/3 (blue) (pCV < 10–20, ranksum). (C) Probability distribution of pairwise cross-correlation values for all neurons and experiments for L4 (orange) and L2/3 (blue) (pCC < 10–65, ranksum). (D–F) Average spike rate, CVISI, and pairwise cross-correlation for evoked neuronal activity (prate < 10–55, pCV < 10–20, pCC < 10–301, ranksum).

Presence of a tonal stimulus increased firing rates and evoked rates were higher in L4 than L2/3 (Figure 2D) consistent with electrophysiological recordings (Atencio and Schreiner, 2010). Evoked activity in both layers also showed high CV and heavy-tailed cross-correlations (Figures 2E,F). The heavy-tailed nature of the correlation distribution suggests that despite the overall high variability observed during both ongoing and evoked activities, at least some neurons show relatively strong coordinated firing.

Ongoing Ensemble Activity in A1 Organizes as Neuronal Avalanches

To better understand the highly variable, yet seemingly coordinated organization of neuronal population activity, we grouped neurons based on the temporal association of their firing with other neuronal firings irrespective of spatial location.

We identified neuronal ensembles as groups of neurons active over successive imaging frames. Specifically, at a frame rate of 30 Hz, each frame defined a time bin of ∼33 ms. Ensembles were bound by time bins that did not contain any active neuron, i.e., they exhibited successive time intervals in which at least 1 neuron was active (Figure 3B). The total number of active neurons could vary between time bins with individual neurons becoming active and inactive, as long as contiguous population activity was maintained. Thus, ensembles could range from very small (a single neuron firing once within just one time bin) to large (many neurons firing over many successive time bins). To gain insight into the variability of ensembles, we quantified their sizes by summing the number of spikes (binarized) for each active neuron in each time bin. We then calculated the probability density function of ensemble sizes (Figure 3C), for which qualitative inspection revealed a high probability of occurrence of small ensembles and a lower probability of occurrence of very large ensembles. These probability distributions appeared linear in a logarithmic plot suggestive of a power law distribution. Indeed, the probability density distributions of ensemble size were significantly better fit by a power law compared to an exponential function (Figures 3C,E; LLRL4 = 480.0 to 1251.9; LLRL2/3 = 551.8 to 1243.8; p < 10–13; FOVL4 = 9; FOVL2/3 = 10). These power laws were abolished by temporal shuffling of each neuron’s inferred spike raster (Figure 3C; LLRL4–shuff = −690.9 to −175.1; LLRL2/3–shuff = −424.9 to −108.8; p < 10–8, favors exponential over power law) indicating that the temporal structure of ensemble activity is essential to maintain the power law distribution of ensemble size.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Imaging neuronal ensemble activity in awake mouse A1. (A) Top: Binarized raster plot of ongoing activity recorded from L2/3 pyramidal neurons with corresponding population activities (black). Middle: Expanded view of raster plot revealing variable ongoing activity with population activities. (B) Schematic depicting definition of activity ensembles from a neuronal activity raster. Activity from successive time bins/frames in which at least one neuron is firing (black rectangles) is concatenated into an activity ensemble (shading; five ensembles shown). Ensembles are separated by at least 1 frame with no activity. Vertical gray lines indicate boundaries of frame acquisition (Δt, 33 ms). Ensemble sizes defined as the number of spikes in each ensemble are indicated below. (C) Probability density distributions of ensemble sizes from individual experiments in L4 and in L2/3. Gray lines indicate distributions from shuffled data. (D) Probability distributions of activity ensemble duration of ongoing activity from all experiments in L2/3 and in L4. Gray lines indicate distributions from shuffled data. Faded lines indicate individual experiments. (E) Boxplots showing slopes of probability distributions in ensemble size (top) and ensemble duration (bottom) for each individual experiment in L4 (orange) and L2/3 (blue), Size: αL2/3: –2.23 ± 0.39, αL4: –2.33 ± 0.52, Duration: αL2/3: –2.47 ± 0.49, αL4: –2.46 ± 0.26, mean ± SD p > 0.65, unpaired two-sample t-test.

We next quantified the temporal duration of each ensemble by summing the number of consecutive time bins the ensemble was active. The distribution of ensemble duration also obeyed a power law distribution over an exponential (Figures 3D,E; LLRL4 = 353.9 to 796.7; LLRL2/3 = 381.8 to 849.9; p < 10–34), which was once again abolished by temporal shuffling (Figure 3D; LLRL4–shuff = 232.3 to 586.4; LLRL2/3–shuff = 57.2 to 459.4; p < 0.01, favors exponential over power law).

The presence of power-law distributions for ensemble size and duration suggests that ongoing ensembles in L4 and L2/3 organize as scale-invariant neuronal avalanches similar to ongoing activity in other cortical regions (Bellay et al., 2015).

Evoked Ensemble Activity in A1 Organizes as Neuronal Avalanches

We next investigated how the presence of a sound stimulus alters ensemble activity in A1. During an auditory stimulus, more A1 neurons fire and are thus recruited into ongoing active ensembles. Accordingly, sound-evoked ensembles could deviate from a power law organization of avalanches by selectively increasing the incidence of large ensembles. Alternatively, sound-evoked A1 ensemble activity could maintain scale-invariant organization by increasing the incidence of ensembles of all sizes.

From the event structure, we defined evoked ensembles as initiated during the 1 s of sound presentation (Figure 4). We then characterized the size and duration (lifetime) of the evoked ensembles by plotting the probability distributions of ensemble size and duration. Since varying sound levels potentially recruit different populations of neurons, we separately analyzed ensemble activity at the three sound levels. Similar to the ongoing activity, all probability distributions appeared linear in a logarithmic plot suggestive of a power law distribution. Indeed, the probability density distributions of ensemble size for all sound levels were significantly better fit by a power law compared to an exponential function (Figures 4B,D and Supplementary Table S2). These power laws were abolished by temporal shuffling (Figure 4B and Supplementary Table S2; favors exponential over power law). We next quantified the temporal duration of each activity ensemble, which is the number of consecutive time bins for each ensemble. The distribution of ensemble durations also obeyed a power-law distribution (Figures 4C,D and Supplementary Table S2), which was once again abolished by temporal shuffling (Figure 4C and Supplementary Table S2, favors exponential over power law). The presence of power law distributions for ensemble size and duration suggests that evoked activity in L4 and L2/3 organizes as scale-invariant neuronal avalanches across different sound amplitudes. These results suggest that population activity patterns are obeying the same statistical rules despite their diversity in size and duration. Furthermore, we explored whether the reported power law distributions collapse according to a scaling exponent b. We found that b was close to zero for each set of distributions (Supplementary Figure S3; L4 size: b = −0.023, L4 duration: b = −0.222, L2/3 size: b = −0.321, L2/3 duration: b = 0.02) indicating that the collapse is not significant, confirming our initial assessment that the power laws are independent of sound level.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Neuronal ensembles in A1 during ongoing activity organize as neuronal avalanches. (A) Activity raster of simultaneously recorded L4 (left) and L2/3 (right) neurons to brief auditory inputs (arrow heads). Middle: Corresponding population activities summed over all neurons. Bottom: Enlarged views revealing variable, transient evoked responses. (B,C) Probability density distributions of ensemble sizes and ensemble duration reveal power laws for the evoked responses and different sound intensity in L4 (left) and L2/3 (right). Dashed lines indicate probability density distributions from shuffled data sets. (D) Boxplots showing slopes of probability distributions in ensemble size (top) and ensemble duration (bottom) for each individual experiment in L4 (orange) and L2/3 (blue), Size: αL2/3: –1.93 ± 0.25, αL4: –1.95 ± 0.20, Duration: αL2/3: –2.20 ± 0.26, αL4: –2.08 ± 0.25, mean ± SD; psize > 0.84, pdur > 0.29, unpaired two-sample t-test.

Given the increase in activity of single neurons by sound stimulation it is expected that there are specific changes in the activity patterns due to the sensory stimulus. We calculated the average size and duration of evoked ensembles and found that both increased systematically with sound level (i.e., input strength) (Figures 5A,B). Therefore, while evoked ensembles in L4 and L2/3 remained power-law distributed in both size and duration (Figures 4B,C and Supplementary Tables S1, S2) the specific range from this distribution from which ensembles are chosen varies with sound level. Thus, scale-invariant avalanche dynamics in both layers L4 and L2/3 are preserved even in the presence of distinct sound input to A1 and across different sound amplitudes.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Average activity ensemble size and duration depend on sound level. (A) Boxplots showing average ensemble size in L2/3 (left; p < 0.04, asterisk indicates significant difference at p < 0.05) and L4 (right) as a function of sound level. (B) Conventions as in (A), but for average ensemble duration (L2/3 p < 0.04, L4 p < 0.03, asterisk indicates significant difference at p < 0.05).

Evoked Avalanches in A1 Recruit Neurons With Widely Varying Tuning Preference

Most behaviorally relevant sound stimuli are suprathreshold and thus recruit neurons with varying tuning preference (e.g., see Figure 1E). Since we observe a wide range of ensemble sizes and duration we sought to determine if the range of frequency preference varied with ensemble size and duration. We thus quantified the tuning diversity in each evoked ensemble by measuring the interquartile range of BFs (IQRBF) from each active cell in the ensemble. We find that IQRBF systematically increases with both ensemble size and duration (Figures 6B,C) indicating that neurons of widely varying tuning preference are recruited into neuronal avalanches.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. BF variability increases with ensemble size and duration, but bandwidth stays constant. (A) Illustration showing the assembly of the ensemble RF from the weighted average of the RFs of individual neurons active in the ensemble. (B,C) Ensemble IQRBF as function of ensemble size (B) or duration (C). IQRBF are binned according to logarithmically spaced bins of size and duration. Solid lines indicate mean. Dashed lines indicate one standard deviation. (D,E) Conventions as in (B,C), but for ensemble bandwidth.

A neuron’s BF gives a limited description of a neuron’s overall receptive field. We thus also determined the compound receptive field of the evoked ensemble by averaging the receptive fields of the neurons active in the ensemble, weighted according to the amount of time each cell was active (Figure 6A). The use of a weighted average is on the basis that the activity level of a neuron corresponds to its importance for encoding. We then determined the bandwidth of the ensemble receptive field. We find that ensemble bandwidth is not altered with respect to the size or duration of ensembles (Figures 6D,E), indicating that stimulus selectivity is scale-invariant in the population activity. Furthermore, the bandwidth of the recruited ensemble did not vary with sound level indicating that ensemble receptive fields are sound level invariant, unlike many single-cell receptive fields.

Discussion

We identified several fundamental properties of how A1 reliably represents stimuli in its spatiotemporal population response. First, we demonstrated that A1 represents even simple sound stimuli as temporally varying activity across distributed populations of diversely tuned pyramidal neurons. Second, pyramidal group activity in both layers obey similar statistical laws in that the size and duration of ongoing and sound-evoked neuronal ensembles show scale-invariance, the hallmark of neuronal avalanches. Finally, by introducing a novel approach of ensemble tuning, we found that neuronal ensembles recruit neurons of increasingly diverse tuning preference yet maintain constant selectivity with respect to ensemble size.

Our finding that both ongoing and evoked activities show signatures of neuronal avalanches is in line with experiment and theory associating avalanches with the optimization of numerous aspects of information processing such as dynamic range, information processing and transmission within layers (Shew et al., 2009, 2010, 2011; Shew and Plenz, 2013; Gautam et al., 2015) and between layers (West et al., 2008; Aquino et al., 2010, 2011). Avalanche organization being maintained during stimulus presentation extends previous findings in ex vivo turtle visual cortex based on the local field potential demonstrating a return to avalanche organization after visual stimulation (Shew et al., 2015; Clawson et al., 2017). The existence of avalanche organization suggest that the network is poised to process incoming sensory stimuli with maximal dynamic range (Kinouchi and Copelli, 2006). The selective labeling of pyramidal groups in our experiments also specifically links avalanche dynamics to pyramidal groups, thereby extending previous reports on evoked avalanches in mice using a non-selective bulk labeling approach (Karimipanah et al., 2017). In summary, our work firmly suggests that A1 networks across layers and stimulus presentation optimize transmission of sound information by maintaining neuronal avalanche organization.

The reported statistics of neuronal ensembles are sensitive to sampling conditions. Spatial subsampling, in which only a small percentage of the population is measured, will change an expected power law distribution to an exponential form, an effect clearly visible at subsampling below 10% or more as shown in simulations (Priesemann et al., 2009; Ribeiro et al., 2010, 2014; Levina and Priesemann, 2017). Subsampling might be the main reason why avalanches have not been observed in spike recordings using microelectrode arrays representing much lower subsampling conditions (Petermann et al., 2009; Ribeiro et al., 2010; Touboul and Destexhe, 2010). 2-photon imaging at cellular resolution allows for much higher neuronal sampling conditions and has been used previously to identify neuronal avalanches (Bellay et al., 2015; Seshadri et al., 2018) using non-negative spike deconvolution (Vogelstein et al., 2010). In the present study, we combined 2-photon imaging with a well-tested, robust spike density estimation (Pachitariu et al., 2018) providing further support that spatiotemporal activity in spiking pyramidal groups organizes as neuronal avalanches. We also note that spiking between pyramidal neurons is significantly correlated, excluding models for the generation of power laws that feature no interaction between neurons (Martinello et al., 2017; Touboul and Destexhe, 2017). Our findings of robust power laws even under non-stationary conditions, i.e., evoked responses, are in line with previous reports in non-human primates based on the local field potential (Yu et al., 2017). We conclude that 2-photon imaging of cortical layers at cellular resolution in combination with robust spike density estimates allows for the clear identification of scale-invariant activity in ongoing and evoked population activity in primary auditory cortex.

We further demonstrate that avalanche organization is largely independent of sound level. We introduce an ensemble RF measure that shows selectivity invariance with respect to stimulus sound level, in contrast to the widening of single-cell RFs across many auditory structures with increasing sound level (e.g., see Figure 1E). This in vivo demonstration of a large dynamic range for A1 network activity matches reports on level-invariant perceptual discrimination performances (Jesteadt et al., 1977; Wier et al., 1977; Bernstein and Oxenham, 2006). It suggests that population coding of sound information might underlie level-invariant performances. Furthermore, we find that large activity ensembles recruit a diverse range of tuning preference, suggesting that BF does not fully represent a neuron’s role in population encoding. Our finding that avalanches contain tuned and untuned neurons is in line with recent findings in primary visual cortex showing that pairwise covariability of both tuned and untuned neurons predicts moment to moment changes in population activity (Dechery and MacLean, 2018).

In summary, our work reveals several key aspects of auditory cortical dynamics and in general cortical sensory coding. Even simple sensory stimuli are represented across distributed populations of diversely tuned neurons organized as scale-invariant spatiotemporal avalanches. We suggest that avalanche dynamics maximize information transmission between and within layers while providing an ordered framework for diversely tuned groups of neurons for which the relative timing of neuronal firing might be an important contributor to neuronal population encoding.

Data Availability

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

Animal Subjects: The animal study was reviewed and approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC) of the University of Maryland, College Park.

Author Contributions

DW, DP, and PK designed the research. DW performed the experiments. ZB, DW, and SS analyzed the data. ZB, DW, SS, DP, and PK discussed the results and wrote the manuscript.

Funding

This study was supported by the NINDS U01NS090569 (PK and DP), U19NS107464 (PK and DP), NIH RO1 DC009607 (PK), DOD W81XWH-16-1-0143 (PK and DW), and the Division of Intramural Research of the NIMH ZIAMH002797 (DP).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Dr. Karel Svoboda for sharing the designs enabling awake imaging, Dr. Mark Andermann for the advice on awake imaging, and Dr. Wolfgang Losert for the helpful discussions. This study utilized the high-performance computational capabilities of the University of Maryland supercomputing resources (http://hpcc.umd.edu).

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnsys.2019.00045/full#supplementary-material

FIGURE S1 | Mouse pupil diameter indicates an intermediate arousal state on average.(a) Histogram of median pupil diameter computed from each experiment. (b) Histogram of pupil diameters over the course of each experiment where each subplot represents an individual imaging session. Most experiments exhibit a peak near the middle of the pupil diameter range, indicating an intermediate arousal state.

FIGURE S2 | No epileptiform activity evident in any mouse strains. Scatter plots of prominence and width of peaks taken from the mean fluorescence trace over time of the entire 2-photon field of view. Each plot represents an individual experiment. Epileptiform activity would be evident as a secondary cluster of points at high prominence and low width.

FIGURE S3 | Power laws are independent of stimulus sound level. (a) Re-scaled power law distributions for ensemble sizes from L4 (left) and L2/3 (right) data. Variable s indicates ensemble size, P(s) indicates probability of ensemble size s, a is the best fit slope from the original ensemble distributions, L is the stimulus sound level, and b is the scaling factor. (b) Conventions as in a, except for ensemble duration where variable d is ensemble duration.

TABLE S1 | Statistical comparisons of the log-likelihood ratio values of avalanche size distributions and shuffled avalanche size distributions.

TABLE S2 | Statistical comparisons of the log-likelihood ratio values from avalanche duration distributions and shuffled avalanche duration distributions.

Footnotes

  1. ^ http://www.ephus.org

References

Aquino, G., Bologna, M., Grigolini, P., and West, B. J. (2010). Beyond the death of linear response: 1/f optimal information transport. Phys. Rev. Lett. 105:040601. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.105.040601

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aquino, G., Bologna, M., West, B. J., and Grigolini, P. (2011). Transmission of information between complex systems: 1/f resonance. Phys. Rev. E Stat. Nonlin. Soft Matter Phys. 83(5 Pt 1):051130. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.83.051130

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Atencio, C. A., and Schreiner, C. E. (2009). Laminar diversity of dynamic sound processing in cat primary auditory cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 103, 192–205. doi: 10.1152/jn.00624.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Atencio, C. A., and Schreiner, C. E. (2010). Columnar connectivity and laminar processing in cat primary auditory cortex. PLoS One 5:e9521. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0009521

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Atencio, C. A., Sharpee, T. O., and Schreiner, C. E. (2009). Hierarchical computation in the canonical auditory cortical circuit. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 21894–21899. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0908383106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bandyopadhyay, S., Shamma, S. A., and Kanold, P. O. (2010). Dichotomy of functional organization in the mouse auditory cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 361–368. doi: 10.1038/nn.2490

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bartho, P., Curto, C., Luczak, A., Marguet, S. L., and Harris, K. D. (2009). Population coding of tone stimuli in auditory cortex: dynamic rate vector analysis. Eur. J. Neurosci. 30, 1767–1778. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06954.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bathellier, B., Ushakova, L., and Rumpel, S. (2012). Discrete neocortical dynamics predict behavioral categorization of sounds. Neuron 76, 435–449. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.07.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beggs, J. M., and Plenz, D. (2003). Neuronal avalanches in neocortical circuits. J. Neurosci. 23, 11167–11177. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.23-35-11167.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bellay, T., Klaus, A., Seshadri, S., and Plenz, D. (2015). Irregular spiking of pyramidal neurons organizes as scale-invariant neuronal avalanches in the awake state. elife 4:e07224. doi: 10.7554/eLife.07224

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernstein, J. G., and Oxenham, A. J. (2006). The relationship between frequency selectivity and pitch discrimination: sensorineural hearing loss. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 120, 3929–3945. doi: 10.1121/1.2372452

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, T. W., Wardill, T. J., Sun, Y., Pulver, S. R., Renninger, S. L., Baohan, A., et al. (2013). Ultrasensitive fluorescent proteins for imaging neuronal activity. Nature 499, 295–300. doi: 10.1038/nature12354

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chialvo, D. R. (2010). Emergent complex neural dynamics. Nat. Phys. 6, 744–750. doi: 10.1038/nphys1803

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clauset, A., Shalizi, C., and Newman, M. (2009). Power-Law distributions in empirical data. Siam Rev. 51, 661–703. doi: 10.1137/070710111

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clawson, W. P., Wright, N. C., Wessel, R., and Shew, W. L. (2017). Adaptation towards scale-free dynamics improves cortical stimulus discrimination at the cost of reduced detection. PLoS Comput. Biol. 13:e1005574. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005574

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dana, H., Chen, T.-W., Hu, A., Shields, B. C., Guo, C., Looger, L. L., et al. (2014). Thy1-GCaMP6 transgenic mice for neuronal population imaging in vivo. PLoS One 9:e108697. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0108697

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dechery, J. B., and MacLean, J. N. (2018). Functional triplet motifs underlie accurate predictions of single-trial responses in populations of tuned and untuned V1 neurons. PLoS Comput. biol. 14:e1006153. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1006153

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deweese, M. R., and Zador, A. M. (2004). Shared and private variability in the auditory cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 92, 1840–1855. doi: 10.1152/jn.00197.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Francis, N. A., Winkowski, D. E., Sheikhattar, A., Armengol, K., Babadi, B., and Kanold, P. O. (2018). Small networks encode decision-making in primary auditory. Cortex. Neuron 97, 5.e–897.e. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.01.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gautam, H., Hoang, T. T., McClanahan, K., Grady, S. K., and Shew, W. L. (2015). Maximizing sensory dynamic range by tuning the cortical state to criticality. PLoS Comput. Biol. 11:e1004576. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004576

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldey, G. J., Roumis, D. K., Glickfeld, L. L., Kerlin, A. M., Reid, R. C., Bonin, V., et al. (2014). Removable cranial windows for long-term imaging in awake mice. Nat Protoc. 9, 2515–2538. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2014.165

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gorski, J. A., Talley, T., Qiu, M., Puelles, L., Rubenstein, J. L., and Jones, K. R. (2002). Cortical excitatory neurons and glia, but not GABAergic neurons, are produced in the Emx1-expressing lineage. J. Neurosci. 22, 6309–6314. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-15-06309.2002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guizar-Sicairos, M., Thurman, S. T., and Fienup, J. R. (2008). Efficient subpixel image registration algorithms. Opt. Lett. 33, 156–158.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Guo, Z. V., Hires, S. A., Li, N., O’Connor, D. H., Komiyama, T., Ophir, E., et al. (2014). Procedures for behavioral experiments in head-fixed mice. PLoS One 9:e88678. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0088678

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jesteadt, W., Wier, C. C., and Green, D. M. (1977). Intensity discrimination as a function of frequency and sensation level. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 61, 169–177. doi: 10.1121/1.381278

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karimipanah, Y., Ma, Z., Miller, J. K., Yuste, R., and Wessel, R. (2017). Neocortical activity is stimulus- and scale-invariant. PLoS One 12:e0177396. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0177396

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanold, P. O., Nelken, I., and Polley, D. B. (2014). Local versus global scales of organization in auditory cortex. Trends Neurosci. 37, 502–510. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2014.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kinouchi, O., and Copelli, M. (2006). Optimal dynamical range of excitable networks at criticality. Nat. Phys. 2, 348–351. doi: 10.1038/nphys289

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klaus, A., Yu, S., and Plenz, D. (2011). Statistical analyses support power law distributions found in neuronal avalanches. PLoS One 6:e19779. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0019779

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levina, A., and Priesemann, V. (2017). Subsampling scaling. Nat. Commun. 8, 15140. doi: 10.1038/ncomms15140

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levy, R. B., and Reyes, A. D. (2012). Spatial profile of excitatory and inhibitory synaptic connectivity in mouse primary auditory cortex. J. Neurosci. 32, 5609–5619. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5158-11.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, J., Whiteway, M. B., Sheikhattar, A., Butts, D. A., Babadi, B., and Kanold, P. O. (2019). Parallel processing of sound dynamics across mouse auditory cortex via spatially patterned thalamic inputs and distinct areal intracortical circuits. Cell Rep. 27, 872–885. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2019.03.069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Madisen, L., Garner, A. R., Shimaoka, D., Chuong, A. S., Klapoetke, N. C., Li, L., et al. (2015). Transgenic mice for intersectional targeting of neural sensors and effectors with high specificity and performance. Neuron 85, 942–958. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.02.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maor, I., Shalev, A., and Mizrahi, A. (2016). Distinct spatiotemporal response properties of excitatory versus inhibitory neurons in the mouse auditory cortex. Cereb. Cortex 26, 4242–4252. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw266

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martinello, M., Hidalgo, J., Maritan, A., di Santo, S., Plenz, D., and Muñoz, M. A. (2017). Neutral theory and scale-free neural dynamics. Phys. Rev. X 7:041071.

Google Scholar

McGinley, M. J., David, S. V., and McCormick, D. A. (2015a). Cortical membrane potential signature of optimal states for sensory signal detection. Neuron 87, 179–192. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.05.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGinley, M. J., Vinck, M., Reimer, J., Batista-Brito, R., Zagha, E., Cadwell, C. R., et al. (2015b). Waking state: rapid variations modulate neural and behavioral responses. Neuron 87, 1143–1161. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meng, X., Winkowski, D. E., Kao, J. P. Y., and Kanold, P. O. (2017). Sublaminar subdivision of mouse auditory cortex layer 2/3 based on functional translaminar connections. J. Neurosci. 37, 10200–10214. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1361-17.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nelken, I., Fishbach, A., Las, L., Ulanovsky, N., and Farkas, D. (2003). Primary auditory cortex of cats: feature detection or something else? Biol Cybern. 89, 397–406. doi: 10.1007/s00422-003-0445-443

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oswald, A. M., Doiron, B., Rinzel, J., and Reyes, A. D. (2009). Spatial profile and differential recruitment of GABAB modulate oscillatory activity in auditory cortex. J. Neurosci. 29, 10321–10334. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1703-09.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pachitariu, M., Stringer, C., and Harris, K. D. (2018). Robustness of spike deconvolution for neuronal calcium imaging. J. Neurosci. 38, 7976. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3339-17.2018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peron, S., Chen, T.-W., and Svoboda, K. (2015). Comprehensive imaging of cortical networks. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 32, 115–123. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2015.03.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petermann, T., Thiagarajan, T., Lebedev, M. A., Nicolelis, M. A., Chialvo, D. R., and Plenz, D. (2009). Spontaneous cortical activity in awake monkeys composed of neuronal avalanches. Proc.Natl.Acad.Sci.U.S.A 106, 15921–15926. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0904089106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Priesemann, V., Munk, M. H., and Wibral, M. (2009). Subsampling effects in neuronal avalanche distributions recorded in vivo. BMC.Neurosci. 10:40. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-10-40

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ratzlaff, E. H., and Grinvald, A. (1991). A tandem-lens epifluorescence macroscope: hundred-fold brightness advantage for wide-field imaging. J. Neurosci. Methods 36, 127–137. doi: 10.1016/0165-0270(91)90038-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ribeiro, T. L., Copelli, M., Caixeta, F., Belchior, H., Chialvo, D. R., Nicolelis, M. A., et al. (2010). Spike avalanches exhibit universal dynamics across the sleep-wake cycle. PLoS One 5:e14129. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0014129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ribeiro, T. L., Ribeiro, S., Belchior, H., Caixeta, F., and Copelli, M. (2014). Undersampled critical branching processes on small-world and random networks fail to reproduce the statistics of spike avalanches. PLoS One 9:e94992. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0094992

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rose, T., Jaepel, J., Hubener, M., and Bonhoeffer, T. (2016). Cell-specific restoration of stimulus preference after monocular deprivation in the visual cortex. Science 352, 1319–1322. doi: 10.1126/science.aad3358

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rothschild, G., Nelken, I., and Mizrahi, A. (2010). Functional organization and population dynamics in the mouse primary auditory cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 353–360. doi: 10.1038/nn.2484

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schindelin, J., Arganda-Carreras, I., Frise, E., Kaynig, V., Longair, M., Pietzsch, T., et al. (2012). Fiji: an open-source platform for biological-image analysis. Nat. Methods 9, 676–682. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seshadri, S., Klaus, A., Winkowski, D. E., Kanold, P. O., and Plenz, D. (2018). Altered avalanche dynamics in a developmental NMDAR hypofunction model of cognitive impairment. Transl Psychiatry 8:3. doi: 10.1038/s41398-017-0060-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shadlen, M. N., and Newsome, W. T. (1998). The variable discharge of cortical neurons: implications for connectivity, computation, and information coding. J. Neurosci. 18, 3870–3896. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.18-10-03870.1998

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shew, W. L., Bellay, T., and Plenz, D. (2010). Simultaneous multi-electrode array recording and two-photon calcium imaging of neural activity. J. Neurosci. Methods 192, 75–82. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2010.07.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shew, W. L., Clawson, W. P., Pobst, J., Karimipanah, Y., Wright, N. C., and Wessel, R. (2015). Adaptation to sensory input tunes visual cortex to criticality. Nat. Phys. 11, 659–663. doi: 10.1038/nphys3370

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shew, W. L., and Plenz, D. (2013). The functional benefits of criticality in the cortex. Neuroscientist 19, 88–100. doi: 10.1177/1073858412445487

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shew, W. L., Yang, H., Petermann, T., Roy, R., and Plenz, D. (2009). Neuronal avalanches imply maximum dynamic range in cortical networks at criticality. J. Neurosci. 29, 15595–15600. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3864-09.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shew, W. L., Yang, H., Yu, S., Roy, R., and Plenz, D. (2011). Information capacity and transmission are maximized in balanced cortical networks with neuronal avalanches. J. Neurosci. 31, 55–63. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4637-10.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shriki, O., Alstott, J., Carver, F., Holroyd, T., Henson, R. N., Smith, M. L., et al. (2013). Neuronal avalanches in the resting MEG of the human brain. J. Neurosci. 33, 7079–7090. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4286-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shriki, O., and Yellin, D. (2016). Optimal information representation and criticality in an adaptive sensory recurrent neuronal network. PLoS Comput. Biol. 12:e1004698. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004698

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steinmetz, N. A., Buetfering, C., Lecoq, J., Lee, C. R., Peters, A. J., Jacobs, E. A., et al. (2017). Aberrant cortical activity in multiple GCaMP6-expressing transgenic mouse lines. eNeuro 4:ENEURO.0207-17 doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0207-17.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suter, B. A., O’Connor, T., Iyer, V., Petreanu, L. T., Hooks, B. M., Kiritani, T., et al. (2010). Ephus: multipurpose data acquisition software for neuroscience experiments. Front. Neural. Circuits 4:100. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2010.00100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thevenaz, P., Ruttimann, U. E., and Unser, M. (1998). A pyramid approach to subpixel registration based on intensity. IEEE Trans Image Process. 7, 27–41. doi: 10.1109/83.650848

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Touboul, J., and Destexhe, A. (2010). Can power-law scaling and neuronal avalanches arise from stochastic dynamics? PLoS One. 5:e8982. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0008982

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Touboul, J., and Destexhe, A. (2017). Power-law statistics and universal scaling in the absence of criticality. Phys. Rev. E 95:012413. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.95.012413

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vogelstein, J. T., Packer, A. M., Machado, T. A., Sippy, T., Babadi, B., Yuste, R., et al. (2010). Fast nonnegative deconvolution for spike train inference from population calcium imaging. J. Neurophysiol. 104, 3691–3704. doi: 10.1152/jn.01073.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watkins, P. V., Kao, J. P., and Kanold, P. O. (2014). Spatial pattern of intra-laminar connectivity in supragranular mouse auditory cortex. Front. Neural. Circuits 8:15. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2014.00015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

West, B. J., Geneston, E. L., and Grigolini, P. (2008). Maximizing information exchange between complex networks. Phys. Rep. 468, 1–99. doi: 10.1016/j.physrep.2008.06.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wier, C. C., Jesteadt, W., and Green, D. M. (1977). Frequency discrimination as a function of frequency and sensation level. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 61, 178–184. doi: 10.1121/1.381251

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winkowski, D. E., and Kanold, P. O. (2013). Laminar transformation of frequency organization in auditory cortex. J. Neurosci. 33, 1498–1508. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3101-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yu, S., Ribeiro, T. L., Meisel, C., Chou, S., Mitz, A., Saunders, R., et al. (2017). Maintained avalanche dynamics during task-induced changes of neuronal activity in nonhuman primates. eLife 6:e27119. doi: 10.7554/eLife.27119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: auditory cortex, mouse, avalanche, pattern, population, imaging, 2-photon, activity

Citation: Bowen Z, Winkowski DE, Seshadri S, Plenz D and Kanold PO (2019) Neuronal Avalanches in Input and Associative Layers of Auditory Cortex. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 13:45. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2019.00045

Received: 11 June 2019; Accepted: 16 August 2019;
Published: 04 September 2019.

Edited by:

Paolo Massobrio, University of Genoa, Italy

Reviewed by:

Fabrizio Lombardi, ETH Zurich, Switzerland
Marco Brondi, Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia, Italy

Copyright © 2019 Bowen, Winkowski, Seshadri, Plenz and Kanold. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Patrick O. Kanold, cGthbm9sZEB1bWQuZWR1

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.