Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 21 August 2020
Sec. Auditory Cognitive Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Association between Peripheral and Central Hearing Loss and Cognitive Impairment in Older Adults with Neurodegenerative Diseases View all 18 articles

The Peripheral Hearing and Central Auditory Processing Skills of Individuals With Subjective Memory Complaints

  • 1Ear Science Institute Australia, Subiaco, WA, Australia
  • 2Ear Sciences Centre Faculty of Health and Medical Sciences, The University of Western Australia, Crawley, WA, Australia
  • 3School of Human Sciences, The University of Western Australia, Crawley, WA, Australia
  • 4Department of Otolaryngology Head Neck Skull Base Surgery, Sir Charles Gairdner Hospital, Nedlands, WA, Australia
  • 5School of Medicine, University Notre Dame, Fremantle, WA, Australia
  • 6School of Medical and Health Sciences, Edith Cowan University, Joondalup, WA, Australia
  • 7Collaborative Genomics Group, School of Medical and Health Sciences, Edith Cowan University, Joondalup, WA, Australia
  • 8School of Pharmacy and Biomedical Sciences, Faculty of Health Sciences, Curtin Health Innovation Research Institute, Curtin University, Bentley, WA, Australia
  • 9School of Psychological Science, The University of Western Australia, Nedlands, WA, Australia
  • 10Department of Biomedical Sciences, Faculty of Medicine and Health Sciences, Macquarie University, Sydney, NSW, Australia
  • 11Centre for Healthy Ageing, School of Psychology and Exercise Science, Murdoch University, Murdoch, WA, Australia

Purpose: This study examined the central auditory processing (CAP) assessment results of adults between 45 and 85 years of age with probable pre-clinical Alzheimer’s disease – i.e., individuals with subjective memory complaints (SMCs) as compared to those who were not reporting significant levels of memory complaints (non-SMCs). It was hypothesized that the SMC group would perform significantly poorer on tests of central auditory skills compared to participants with non-SMCs (control group).

Methods: A total of 95 participants were recruited from the larger Western Australia Memory Study and were classified as SMCs (N = 61; 20 males and 41 females, mean age 71.47 ±7.18 years) and non-SMCs (N = 34; 10 males, 24 females, mean age 68.85 ±7.69 years). All participants completed a peripheral hearing assessment, a CAP assessment battery including Dichotic Digits, Duration Pattern Test, Dichotic Sentence Identification, Synthetic Sentence Identification with Ipsilateral Competing Message (SSI-ICM) and the Quick-Speech-in-Noise, and a cognitive screening assessment.

Results: The SMCs group performed significantly poorer than the control group on SSI-ICM −10 and −20 dB signal-to-noise conditions. No significant differences were found between the two groups on the peripheral hearing threshold measurements and other CAP assessments.

Conclusions: The results suggest that individuals with SMCs perform poorly on specific CAP assessments in comparison to the controls. The poor CAP in SMC individuals may result in a higher cost to their finite pool of cognitive resources. The CAP results provide yet another biomarker that supports the hypothesis that SMCs may be a primary indication of neuropathological changes in the brain. Longitudinal follow up of individuals with SMCs, and decreased CAP abilities should inform whether this group is at higher risk of developing dementia as compared to non-SMCs and those SMC individuals without CAP difficulties.

Introduction

With over 47 million individuals living with dementia worldwide in 2015, this syndrome is considered a growing global epidemic in older adults (Prince et al., 2015). According to the World Alzheimer Report, the number of people with a dementia diagnosis will rise to 74.7 million in 2030 and 132 million by 2050 (Prince et al., 2015). Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is the most common cause of dementia in older adults accounting for 60–80% of all-cause dementia (Lambert et al., 2014).

The neuropathological changes associated with AD, including the deposition of amyloid plaques and neurofibrillary tangles, start 20–30 years before the clinical diagnosis (Serrano-Pozo et al., 2011). The National Institute of Aging-Alzheimer’s Association Work Groups on diagnostic guidelines suggest that the course of AD can be divided into three subsequent stages: (1) the pre-clinical stage of AD (no impairment in cognition on standard assessments and biomarker evidence for AD), (2) mild cognitive impairment (MCI) due to AD (impairment on memory or other domains of cognition on a standard assessment and biomarker evidence for AD), and (3) dementia due to AD (dementia and biomarker evidence for AD plus subtle cognitive decline) (Jack et al., 2011; McKhann et al., 2011; Sperling et al., 2011; Albert et al., 2013). Current evidence suggests that the self-reported decline in memory or other cognitive functions in the presence of normal performance on neuropsychological measures is associated with an increased risk for future cognitive decline and AD dementia (Glodzik-Sobanska et al., 2007; Wang et al., 2011; Scheef et al., 2012).

With no cure or effective treatment currently insight, it is critical to identify those factors that may prevent or delay cognitive decline and dementia in older adults. Of the potentially modifiable risk factors, untreated hearing loss contributes up to 8% of the modifiable risk factors in mid-life (Livingston et al., 2020). Age-related hearing loss (ARHL) or presbycusis is a multifactorial disorder affecting hearing acuity that varies from mild to profound and results from lifetime insults to the auditory system (Gates and Mills, 2005). Prevalence data indicates that 63% of adults aged 70 years and older have a >25 dBHL speech frequency (4 PTA average of 0.5, 1, 2, and 4 kHz) hearing loss in their better ear (Lin et al., 2011b). Evidence from both cross-sectional (Jayakody et al., 2017) and longitudinal (Lin et al., 2011a; Deal et al., 2016) studies confirmed an increase in the risk of cognitive impairment and incident dementia among older adults with ARHL (Lin et al., 2011a; Deal et al., 2016). In an 11-year longitudinal study, baseline peripheral hearing loss was associated with the increased risk of incident AD (1.27 times per 10 dB hearing loss).

Central auditory processing (CAP) impairment refers to auditory perceptual difficulties that cannot be explained by impairment in peripheral hearing but refers to the impairment in the central auditory pathway affecting speech understanding such as neural transmission, feature extraction and detecting small gaps in the speech which is crucial in speech discrimination, integrating and separating binaural auditory information (Humes et al., 2012; Musiek and Chermak, 2013; Fortunato et al., 2016). A typical hearing complaint of older adults is their inability to understand speech, especially in the presence of background noise (CHABA, 1988). Together, peripheral hearing loss, cognitive decline, aging, and diminished CAP skills (i.e., decline in speech-in-noise processing, dichotic processing and temporal processing skills, or a combination of all these abilities) contribute to the poor speech understanding skills of older adults (Humes et al., 2012; Musiek and Chermak, 2013).

Results from several longitudinal studies suggest that central hearing or CAP skills, in the absence of a severe peripheral hearing loss, are associated with high incidences of cognitive decline and AD dementia (Gates et al., 2002, 2008, 2011) and may precede cognitive impairments and dementia diagnosis by three to 12 years (Gates et al., 1996). These studies have demonstrated that individuals with CAP dysfunction were at a significantly higher risk for incident dementia with hazard ratios ranging from 9.9 (95% CI, 3.6–26.7) to 23.3 (95% CI, 6.6–82.7) (Gates et al., 1996, 2011). Central auditory processing impairment is now considered the primary auditory impairment associated with an increased risk of AD (Panza et al., 2018).

A few studies have previously investigated the CAP skills in individuals diagnosed with MCI (Idrizbegovic et al., 2013; Edwards et al., 2017) and dementia (Gates et al., 2011; Golden et al., 2015; Quaranta et al., 2015). However, to date, no study has investigated the CAP skills in individuals with subjective memory complaints (SMCs). The SMCs include self-reported concerns about one’s memory, which is a major component of subjective cognitive decline (SCD) and is considered a risk factor for future dementia due to AD (Choe et al., 2018). Investigating whether SMC individuals have poor CAP skills compared to those with non-SMCs would provide a unique opportunity to identify people at high risk of developing dementia or who are probably at pre-clinical phases of AD and dementia, years before clinical diagnosis (Jessen et al., 2014; Rönnlund et al., 2015).

The primary aim of our study was to measure the peripheral and central hearing of participants with SMCs and non-SMCs. We hypothesized that participants in the SMC group would perform significantly poorer on tests of peripheral and central hearing assessment compared to non-SMC participants.

Many studies have explored the contribution of genetic factors to ARHL (Karlsson et al., 1997; Gates et al., 1999; Friedman et al., 2009), and to AD (Poorkaj et al., 1998; Cai et al., 2015; Hardy, 2017). However, there is no conclusive evidence on whether cognitive impairment/dementia and ARHL share a common genetic etiology. Kurniawan et al. (2012) reported that apolipoprotein E (APOE) ε4 allele, a major genetic risk factor for AD, is strongly associated with poorer hearing thresholds, whereas, Mener et al. (2016) reported that participants with two APOE ε4 alleles had better hearing thresholds at 1.0 kHz (β = −8.56, p = 0.021) compared to those with no APOE ε4 alleles. However, whether APOE ε4 allele status is associated with impaired peripheral hearing sensitivity and CAP skills is yet to be investigated. Therefore, we also investigated the association between peripheral and central auditory functions and cognitive functions, and the impact of APOE ε4 carriage as the primary genetic risk factor for late-onset AD. We hypothesized that the APOE ε4 carriers would perform significantly poorer than the non-carries on central and peripheral hearing assessments.

Methods

Study Design: Prospective Study

Participants were recruited from the Western Australia Memory Study (WAMS), a longitudinal study into aging and dementia. The ethics approval for this study was received from the Ramsay Health Care WA| SA Human Research Ethics Committee (previously, the Hollywood Private Hospital Ethics Committee, Western Australia). All procedures were undertaken per this approval. The participants completed an informed consent form prior to taking part in the study. The hearing assessments were conducted by a trained researcher under the supervision of a qualified audiologist, and the cognitive assessments were performed by a qualified psychometric rater trained and certified by the study neuropsychologists.

Participants

A total of 95 individuals who have been speaking Australian English for 10 years or longer, aged between 45–85 years, in a general state of good health and with a 4PTA (average of better ear hearing thresholds at 0.5, 1, 2, and 4 kHz) of less than 40 dBHL were recruited for the study. None of the participants used hearing aids. All participants were recruited from the WAMS (Sohrabi et al., 2012).

Materials and Procedure

The assessment materials included measures of cognition, psychological status, and peripheral and central hearing.

Demographic information: All participants completed a demographic questionnaire that was asking for information on age, sex, education, and family history of stroke, dementia, diabetes, smoking, and high blood pressure.

Hearing Assessment

The hearing assessment consisted of two components: peripheral and central.

Peripheral Hearing Assessment

The hearing assessment included an otoscopic examination and pure-tone audiometric testing. The pure-tone audiometric assessment was conducted using a KUDUwave 5000, Type 2 clinical audiometer (Emoyo, Johannesburg, South Africa). Bilateral air-conduction thresholds between 0.5 and 8 kHz and bone-conduction thresholds between 0.5 and 4 kHz were obtained through standard audiometric assessment in a sound-attenuating booth that meets the standard for audiometric testing. This task took approximately 20 min to complete.

Central Auditory Processing Assessment

The CAP functions were assessed using a battery of five central auditory tests to identify the central auditory deficits. The assessments were presented at 65 dBHL, for participants who had 4PTA > 25 dBHL. For those who had 4PTA < 25 dBHL, test materials were presented at 4PTA + 20 dB.

The following CAP tests were administered.

Dichotic Digits Test

Dichotic Digits Test (DDT; Musiek et al., 1991) is a test of binaural integration. The participants were instructed that “you will hear two numbers in each of your ears. Listen carefully and repeat all the numbers you hear. The order doesn’t matter. If you are unsure of the numbers, please guess.” The participant was presented with one practice presentation and 25 assessed presentations. Each digit repeated back was marked as correct or incorrect. The ears were scored separately, and the order in which the participant repeated the digits back did not matter. Scores above 90% in each ear are considered within the normal limits for adults with normal hearing sensitivity and under the age of 65 years. For adults over the age of 65 years or with a hearing loss, the cut-off is 82% (Musiek, 1983).

Dichotic Sentence Identification

Dichotic Sentence Identification (DSI; Fifer et al., 1983) is a test of binaural separation. During the DSI, the participant was presented with 20 sentence pairs from a printed list of six sentences. In each sentence pair, the participant was presented with one of the six sentences in each ear; the sentences always differed in each ear. The 20 sentence pairs were presented in two blocks of 10; in the first block, the participant was asked to attend to the sentence being presented to their right ear and then indicate the sentence number that corresponded to the sentence presented in their directed ear. In the second block, the participants were asked to attend to their left ear and complete the same task. Each trial was marked as correct or incorrect and then each ear was scored as a percentage correct.

Duration Pattern Test

In the Duration Pattern Test (DPT; Musiek, 1994), participants were presented with 1 kHz tones in sequences of three, where each tone could have a duration of either 250 ms (short) or 500 ms (long) with 300 ms intervals between the tones in the sequence of three tones (Musiek and Chermak, 1994). The participants were asked to linguistically label each of the tone durations as either short (S) or long (L) in each series, for example, “short, long, long.” There were six possible combinations of the three tones (SSL, LLS, LSL, SLS, LSS, SLL). Each three-item sequence of the tones had to be identified with the lengths in the correct order to be scored as correct. The participant was presented with three practice presentations and then with 30 assessed presentations, with 15 in each ear. Percent correct scores for each ear were recorded.

Synthetic Sentence Identification with Ipsilateral Competing Message

In the Synthetic Sentence Identification with Ipsilateral Competing Message (SSI-ICM; Speaks et al., 1967; Orchik and Burgess, 1977) test, the participant selected which nonsense sentence out of 10 was being presented against a competing narrative in the same ear. The competing narrative was a passage about Davy Crockett presented by the same presenter, ipsilateral to the target sentence (Jerger and Jerger, 1974; Supplementary Material 1). Studies have shown both hearing loss and age can affect SSI-ICM scores (Gates et al., 2008), therefore, the lists were presented at 65 dBHL for those who had 4PTA > 25 dBHL. For those who had 4PTA < 25 dBHL, test materials were presented at 4PTA + 20 dB. Four sets of ten sentences were presented at + 10 dB speech-to-noise ratio (SNR), 0 dB SNR, −10 dB SNR, and −20 dB SNR conditions. The target sentences presented at 10 dB above the competing narrative in the + 10 dB SNR condition, 10dB below the competing narrative in the −10dB condition and 20 dB below the competing narrative in the −20 dB condition. The target sentence and competing narrative were presented at the same level in the 0 dB SNR condition.

Percent correct score was calculated for each test condition and each ear separately.

Quick Speech-in-Noise

In the Quick Speech-in-Noise (QuickSIN; Killion et al., 2004) test, the participant was presented with two practice sentences and two pre-recorded test lists in each ear; each list is comprised of six sentences with five target words in each. The multi-talker babble increased by 5 dB per sentence between 40 and 65 dBHL, creating varying conditions between a 25 dB speech-to-noise ratio and a 0 dB SNR for the final sentence. The participant was asked to repeat the sentence back to the assessor and was scored one point for each word they correctly repeated. The total score was then subtracted from 25.5 to calculate the signal-to-noise ratio loss (Killion et al., 2004). Examples are provided in Supplementary Material 2.

Cognitive Assessment

The WAMS is a longitudinal study of community-dwelling older adults in Perth Western Australia, specifically enriched for recruiting individuals with SMCs. All participants complete a comprehensive neuropsychological assessment, including self and informant scales and questionnaires. The WAMS assessment battery takes up to 3 h and has various measures assessing global cognition, verbal and visual episodic memory, attention, executive functions, language, orientation, visuospatial skill, and so on. In addition, the participants provide blood samples and undergo brain imaging. Self-reports and informant-reports surveys and questioners are collected for the psychological, cognitive, and personality characteristics of the WAMS participants. As this paper is focused on the relationship between hearing loss and SMCs, we have not reported on the neuropsychological measures. Further information on the neuropsychological measures used as part of WAMS can be found in Sohrabi et al. (2019).

To assess the participants’ general cognitive abilities and screen for potential cognitive impairment, the WAMS utilizes the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) (Nasreddine et al., 2005). Only participants with a MoCA cut-off score of ≥23 were considered cognitively normal and recruited for this study (Rossetti et al., 2011). For those participants with borderline or concerning results, the full neurocognitive battery, as well as the clinical data, are examined by the study Lead Neuropsychs (HRS and MW) to ensure that such results are appropriately communicated with participants and their medical doctors. Of note, the endpoint for participation in the WAMS is a diagnosis of MCI, AD, or other types of dementia.

In this study, participants were divided into two groups: a control (non-SMC), and a high-risk group (SMC) based on completing a self-reported survey, namely the Memory Assessment Clinics Questionnaire (MAC-Q; Crook et al., 1992). The MAC-Q is a self-report questionnaire designed to capture concerns about memory decline. The first five questions assess age-related decline in memory on daily activities and the last question asks about overall memory decline. Participants in the current study were asked to rate all six items on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from “Much better now” to “Much poorer now,” compared to when they were at high school or college. The total score on the MAC-Q ranges from 7 to 35, and higher scores indicate more complaints about one’s memory abilities. Participants were considered SMCs if they scored ≥25 on the MAC-Q (Crook et al., 1992).

Genotyping

In this study, the APOE ε4 allele was determined using TaqMan® genotyping assays (rs7412, assay ID: C ____904973_10; rs429358, assay ID: C___3084793_20) from DNA extracted from 5ml of a whole blood sample using QIAamp DNA Blood Maxi Kits (Qiagen, Hilden, Germany). TaqMan® genotyping assays were performed on a QuantStudio 12K FlexTM Real-Time-PCR systems (Applied Biosystems, Foster City, CA) using the TaqMan® GTXpressTM Master Mix (Life Technologies) as per manufacturer instructions. APOE ε4 carriers included participants with at least one ε4 allele (i.e., ε2/ε4, ε3/ε4, ε4/ε4) and non-carriers included no copies of the ε4 allele (ε2/ε2, ε2/ε3, ε3/ε3).

Statistical Analysis

Data were recorded in Excel and statistically analyzed using IBM SPSS Statistics, Version 25.0 (IBM Corp, Armonk, NY). Analyses of Co-Variance (ANCOVA) were run for each ear in each test to test the hypotheses that SMC participants perform significantly poorer than non-SMC participants in tests of peripheral and central hearing. Age was considered a covariate and adjustments for multiple comparisons were made using Bonferroni corrections. The participants of this study cohort had a wide age range and (50–85 years). Also, age is the primary risk factor for both ARHL and cognitive impairment in older adults (Glisky, 2007). In addition, SMCs increases in frequency (number of times given individual complaints) and prevalence as a factor of age (Koivisto et al., 1995; Coley et al., 2008). Therefore, adjusting for age was required to be able to disentangle the effect of age on the relationship between hearing loss and SMCs. Assumptions for linearity, homogeneity and homoscedasticity were met for each test and there were no outliers in the data, as assessed by no cases with standardized residuals greater than ±3 standard deviations. Data are reported as unadjusted mean ± standard deviation unless otherwise stated.

To investigate the relationship between SMC and age, gender, better ear 4PTA and CAP tests, we have conducted binomial logistic regression with the presence of SMCs as the dependent variables and age, gender, better ear 4PTA and CAP tests as independent variables.

Results

Demographic details of the participants are reported in Table 1. Of the 95 participants who were selected for the study, 61 participants (20 males and 41 females, mean age 71.47 ±7.18 years) had MAC-Q scores ≥25, hence, categorized as SMC. Thirty-four participants (10 males, 24 females, mean age 68.85 ±7.69 years) had MAC-Q scores < 25, hence categorized as non-SMC. Mean and SD values for both SMC and non-SMC groups for all the CAP tests are reported in Table 2. There was no significant interaural asymmetry observed in any of the participants. CAPS test protocols suggest assessing each ear separately and the scores were calculated for each ear as well. Hence, rather than averaging scores of right and left ear, we analyzed results for each ear which is a routine way of analyzing hearing assessment results.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic data of the participants.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Mean and standard deviation scores of central auditory processing tests for both participant groups.

Analysis of Co-variance results of the SMC and non-SMC participants revealed no significant difference between peripheral left and right ear 4PTA and 2 HF PTA (high frequency average of 6 and 8 kHz) of the SMC and non-SMC participant groups (Table 3). Both groups of participants had their 4PTA mean hearing thresholds within normal limits (<25 dBHL). The SMC group performed significantly poorer than the non-SMC group on SSI-ICM −10 dB SNR [right ear, F (1, 89) = 6.59, p = 0.012; left ear F (1, 89) = 7.87, p = 0.006) and −20 dB SNR (right ear F (1, 87) = 13.85, p < 0.001; and left ear F (1,88) = 20.06, p < 0.001] conditions. No significant differences were found between participant groups for the other CAP measures i.e., Quick-SIN, DDT, DSI, and DPT tasks (Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Analysis of Co-variance results of the subjective memory complainers and non-memory complainers.

Results of the binomial logistic regression failed to reveal any significant association between 4PTA and SMC. However, SSI-ICM −20 and −10 dB SNR conditions were statistically significant. For SSI-ICM left ear −20dB SNR condition the model explained 24.8% of the total variance, sensitivity was 79.6% and the specificity was 57.6% and the positive predictive value was 75.43%. For SSI-ICM right ear −20 dB SNR condition the model explained 33.1% of the total variance, sensitivity was 74.5% and the specificity was 54.5% and the positive predictive value was 73.21%. Results of the binomial logistic regression analysis are reported in Supplementary Table 3.

AD Genetic Risk Factor and Hearing

Of the 95 participants tested 26 were APOE ε4 carriers (10 males, 16 females; mean age 68.85 ±8.33 years), and 69 were non-carriers (20 males, 49 females; mean age 71.17 ±7.03 years). No significant difference was observed between carriers and non-carriers on better and poorer ear 4PTA scores or better and poorer ear 2HF PTA average. The performance of carriers and non-carriers on all central auditory test measures were not significantly different (Table 4). Of the total cohort, 20 were SMC and positive for APOE ε4 carriage. However, when compared with the remaining 75 participants, there was no significant difference between the two groups on any of the hearing tests.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Analysis of Co-variance results for the APOE e4 carriers and non-carriers.

Discussion

A significant difference between the SMC and non-SMC groups was observed on tests of SSI-ICM −10 and −20 dB SNR conditions. Of note, mean values of right and left ear 4PTA results of both groups were not significantly different. The hearing thresholds of left ear, right ear, and better ear 4PTA for both groups were approximately 25 dB. Therefore, we conclude that the SSI-ICM results were not influenced by the peripheral hearing loss.

Central auditory processing difficulties are known to place additional demands and strain on an individual’s cognitive processing capacity (Bellis and Jorgensen, 2014). This is due to the requirement for additional “upstream” resources to facilitate auditory perception and other higher-order central auditory processes (Bellis and Jorgensen, 2014). This study’s findings suggest that while there may be a decline in some CAP skills in individuals with SMCs, this deficit may only become visible when the CAP system is significantly challenged, and cognitive resources are exhausted or depleted, such as in the SSI-ICM −10 or −20 SNR subtests. Moreover, speech processing is one of the most challenging and cognitively demanding CAP tasks that rely on several auditory processing skills, including auditory attention, temporal resolution, temporal patterning, and auditory closure (Chermak and Lee, 2005). Speech processing becomes even more difficult and cognitively complex when the speech signal is distorted by other competing messages, meaning apt auditory attention is also required to discriminate and accurately understand what is being said (Gates et al., 2010).

The SSI-ICM test appears to be sensitive to the decline of CAP skills in MCI populations and accurately predicts the future clinical diagnosis of AD (Gates et al., 2008 and 2010; Edwards et al., 2017). Gates et al. (2008) found that individuals with a diagnosis of MCI performed significantly poorer on the 0 dB SNR condition of the SSI-ICM compared to a control group. The −10 and −20 dB SNR conditions of the SSI-ICM were not assessed in the Gates et al. (2008) study. However, given the known progression of the neuropathology and differences in cognition between MCI and SMC groups, it can be assumed that a more difficult task, where the signal is more distorted and therefore requires more cognitive resources, is necessary to differentiate between SMC and control groups as compared to other groups.

To understand why the SMC group performed poorly in the SSI-ICM task, we need to understand the nature of this task. The human auditory system perceptually segregates the competing acoustic stimuli into different streams in a process known as auditory scene analysis (Bregman, 1990). The auditory scene analysis makes use of the spatial, spectral and supra-segmental composition, as well as temporal information to segregate the foreground from background information (Bregman, 1990). It does this by matching the incoming acoustic information to previously stored auditory “templates,” mediated by attentional mechanisms (Carlyon et al., 2001; Billig et al., 2013). Synthetic Sentence Identification with Ipsilateral Competing Message is a test of auditory closure that helps fill in the missing information in the presence of a competing signal (Speaks et al., 1967). Synthetic sentences are third-order approximations of English sentences. These sentences contain seven semantically meaningless words. They are designed to reduce the listener’s reliance on linguistic skills while preserving the syntax and temporal features of the English language (Jerger and Jerger, 1974). The SSI-ICM test sentences were presented at different signal to noise ratios in the same ear making the task more challenging at higher signal-to-noise ratios. The −10 and −20 dB SNR condition of the SSI-ICM was considered the most challenging test of speech processing used. The competing message was 10 dB louder than the target signal/sentence in −10 dB SNR, and 20 dB louder than the target signal in −20 dB SNR conditions, which results in a more distorted signal in comparison to the Quick-SIN and 0 and +10 dB SNR conditions of the SSI-ICM.

Consequently, the SMC group performed significantly poorer than the control group only in a subset of CAP tests, despite there being no significant difference between the peripheral hearing thresholds of the two groups. This is not surprising, as a more distorted speech signal requires greater processing for speech understanding, which then subsequently places increased strain on the higher-order auditory processing skills and consequently requires more cognitive resources (Bellis and Jorgensen, 2014). To recognize speech in challenging situations, proficient listeners rely either on high predictability or clarity of the speech (Bradlow and Alexander, 2007). The SSI-ICM target sentences are semantically and syntactically unrelated; hence predictability is poor. Medwetsky (1994, 2011), reported that the spatial location of the sound sources and fundamental frequency differences in talkers allows the speakers to segregate the acoustic signals into meaningful units. In the absence of both spatial and fundamental frequency difference cues, short sentences with 5–6 word length can still be recalled to a certain extent (Medwetsky, 1994). In this study SSI-ICM sentences contained seven words in each sentence list, hence the listeners could not rely on spatial cues or talker gender differences to segregate the information into different acoustic streams, making the task extremely difficult.

Several neural networks are required to work synchronously to facilitate speech recognition. For example, the lateral prefrontal cortex is involved in the organization and execution of behavior, speech and reasoning (Fuster, 2001), the posterior portion of Broca’s area plays a vital role in processing phonological working memory information (Gelfand and Bookheimer, 2003), primary and associated auditory cortices with stream segregation during auditory scene analysis (Deike et al., 2004, 2010; Gutschalk et al., 2007), posterior superior temporal gyrus and planum temporale with the detailed perceptual representation of segregated objects (Schönwiesner et al., 2007), parietal cortex in mapping sensory inputs to motor actions (Cusack, 2005; Cohen, 2009) and mid-ventrolateral prefrontal cortex driving the top-down attentional processes (Schönwiesner et al., 2007).

Tuwaig et al. (2017) reported an association between poor SSI-ICM scores (−10 dB SNR) and atrophy in the right Heschl’s gyrus cortical thickness and thinner right parahippocampal and entorhinal cortices, bilateral precuneus, occipital cortex, left inferior parietal lobule, inferior and mid temporal gyri on structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), after adjusting for age, gender, education, pure-tone hearing, and APOE ε4 status. Heschl’s gyrus is involved in the temporal processing of the auditory signal (Zatorre and Belin, 2001). The inferior parietal lobe plays a vital role in auditory working memory: integration of sensory-motor information and monitoring and updating the location of the sound source (Alain et al., 2008). Changes in the cortical thickness of inferior parietal cortices are observed early in the neurodegeneration from normal to MCI, and neurodegeneration of parahippocampal/entorhinal regions is seen in progression from MCI to AD (Devanand et al., 2012). Neuroimaging studies that examined the biomarkers of early AD (accumulation of global Aβ and tau burdens) in SMC individuals have found an increased accumulation of early tauopathy in the entorhinal cortex (Buckley et al., 2017) as well as an increase in global Aβ burden (Rodda et al., 2010; Amariglio et al., 2012); both are considered neuropathological hallmarks of AD (Braak and Braak, 1997). Attending to a challenging task such as SSI-ICM requires the optimal functioning of neural networks. Hence, we propose that neuropathological impairment associated with SMC may have impeded their ability in segregating foreground from background information under higher signal-to-noise ratios reflecting in poor performances in the SSI-ICM task.

Furthermore, if numerous cognitive processes and auditory processing skills are required for the SSI-ICM, this suggests the task is highly taxing on an individual’s finite pool of cognitive resources. In this study perhaps individuals with SMC could compensate for impaired CAP skills by drawing on more upstream cognitive resources until the task became too demanding and the cognitive resources are exhausted, creating significant differences between the SMCs and control group. Therefore, given the numerous cortical areas and cognitive processes involved in attending to the SSI-ICM task, it can be expected that this task would be sensitive to the effects of potential neurodegeneration in SMC populations.

In our study, the SMC group did not perform significantly poorer than the non-SMCs in the peripheral hearing assessment. Both participant groups performed within the normal range for the global cognitive functions, yet SMCs performed poorly in some of the CAP functions. Two prominent theories have been proposed to explain the interaction between peripheral hearing and CAP in older adults: the central effects of biological aging (CEBA) and central effects of peripheral pathology (CEPP; Willott, 1996). Central effects of biological aging hypothesis is that changes in central hearing occur due to the anatomical and physiological changes that occur in the central nervous system as a result of aging without any peripheral deficits. Evidence from animal studies that have reported age-related pathophysiological changes in the central auditory pathway without any peripheral impairment (Walton et al., 1998; Ouda and Syka, 2012) seems to support this hypothesis. The CEPP hypothesis argues that the pathophysiological changes in the central auditory pathway occur as a result of peripheral hearing impairment (Willott, 1996). Increase in the cognitive load resulting from peripheral hearing loss (CHABA, 1988; Lindenberger and Baltes, 1994) or longstanding hearing loss could lead to impaired cognitive functions (CHABA, 1988; Lindenberger and Baltes, 1994; Schneider and Pichora-Fuller, 2000; Lin et al., 2011b, 2013; Humes et al., 2012). As there was no significant difference between the peripheral hearing thresholds of the SMC and non-SMC groups, our study supports the CEBA theory; therefore, we conclude that CAP deficits that we have seen may indicate early signs of neurodegeneration.

Similar to previous findings (O’Grady et al., 2007; Tuwaig et al., 2017), we also failed to observe any significant difference between APOE ε4 carriers and non-carriers on peripheral hearing thresholds. However, in the Leiden 85-plus Study (Kurniawan et al., 2012), participants with the APOE ε4/ε4 genotype had more than moderately-severe levels of hearing loss (n = 6; mean 56.1 dBHL) and participants with the APOE ε3/ε4 or ε2/ε4 genotype showed moderate levels of (n = 89, mean 51.0 dBHL) while those without the APOE-ε4 allele (n = 340) had the poorest levels of hearing loss (48.9 dBHL). In our study, the recruited participants had 4PTA [4-frequency pure-tone average 500, 1000, 2000, and 4000 Hz) hearing thresholds within approximately 25 dBHL but were significantly younger than the Kurniawan et al. (2012) study, furthermore, the number of carriers was not large enough to confirm whether such differences would exist in higher levels of hearing losses. However, the mechanism underlying these observations should be explored in a larger sample size.

This study was considered a cross-sectional pilot study and consequently was limited in its interpretation and relatively small sample size. Ideally, the CAP skills of more participants should be examined longitudinally to note any deterioration in participants’ cognition and/or CAP skills. Our study also did not have access to brain imaging that could have been informative in substantiating the presence or absence of neurodegenerative processes (if any) in their earlier stages.

In summary, the results of this study are cautiously promising and have established a potential diagnostic opportunity in identifying probable pre-clinical AD that may assist in delaying or arresting the cognitive decline in older adults. However, these results should be validated and replicated in larger groups over a more extended period. Furthermore, future research can combine these two measures (CAP and SMC) to examine the predictive value of each one as well as the increased accuracy when they are used together. While SSI and SMC are independent, the added benefit of using these two in identifying individuals at higher risk of dementia is another potential research that can inform the field.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets presented in this article are not readily available due to Australian privacy laws. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to DJ (dona.jayakody@earscience.org.au).

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Hollywood Private Hospital Ethics Committee, Western Australia (HPH-139). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

DJ: study design and manuscript preparation. HM, JG-S, and JY: hearing data collection. KT, SL, and MW: genetic assessment. RM and HS: cognitive assessment. All authors provided interpretation of the results and reviewed the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest

HS has received ruminations for working with Pfizer and Takeda. HS research is partially supported by the Australian Alzheimer’s Research Foundation.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by the Australian Alzheimer’s Research Foundation and the Ear Science Institute, Australia.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnins.2020.00888/full#supplementary-material

References

Alain, C., He, Y., and Grady, C. (2008). The contribution of the inferior parietal lobe to auditory spatial working memory. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 20, 285–295. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Albert, M. S., Dekosky, S. T., Dickson, D., Dubois, B., Feldman, H. H., Fox, N. C., et al. (2013). The Diagnosis of Mild Cognitive Impairment due to Alzheimer’s Disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association Workgroups on Diagnostic Guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. FOCUS 11, 96–106. doi: 10.1176/appi.focus.11.1.96

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amariglio, R. E., Becker, J. A., Carmasin, J., Wadsworth, L. P., Lorius, N., Sullivan, C., et al. (2012). Subjective cognitive complaints and amyloid burden in cognitively normal older individuals. Neuropsychologia 50, 2880–2886. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.08.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bellis, T. J., and Jorgensen, L. E. (2014). “Aging of the auditory system and differential diagnosis of central auditory processing disorders in older listeners,” in Handbook of Central Auditory Processing Disorders, Vol. I, eds E. F. Musiek and G. D. Chermak (San Diego, CA: Plural Publishing Inc), 598–642.

Google Scholar

Billig, A. J., Davis, M. H., Deeks, J. M., Monstrey, J., and Carlyon, R. P. (2013). Lexical Influences on Auditory Streaming. Curr. Biol. 23, 1585–1589. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.06.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braak, H., and Braak, E. (1997). Frequency of Stages of Alzheimer-Related Lesions in Different Age Categories. Neurobiol. Aging 18, 351–357. doi: 10.1016/S0197-4580(97)00056-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradlow, A. R., and Alexander, J. A. (2007). Semantic and phonetic enhancements for speech-in-noise recognition by native and non-native listeners. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 121, 2339–2349. doi: 10.1121/1.2642103

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bregman, A. S. (1990). Auditory Scene Analysis: The Perceptual Organization of Sound. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Buckley, R. F., Hanseeuw, B., Schultz, A. P., Vannini, P., Aghjayan, S. L., Properzi, M. J., et al. (2017). Region-Specific Association of Subjective Cognitive Decline With Tauopathy Independent of Global β-Amyloid Burden. JAMA Neurol. 74, 1455–1463. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2017.2216

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cai, Y., An, S. S., and Kim, S. (2015). Mutations in presenilin 2 and its implications in Alzheimer’s disease and other dementia-associated disorders. Clin. Intervent. Aging 10, 1163–1172. doi: 10.2147/CIA.S85808

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carlyon, R. P., Cusack, R., Foxton, J. M., and Robertson, I. H. (2001). Effects of attention and unilateral neglect on auditory stream segregation. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 27, 115–127. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.27.1.11510.1016/j.cub.2013.06.042

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

CHABA (1988). Speech Understanding and Aging. Working Group on Speech Understanding and Aging. Committee on Hearing, Bioacoustics, and Biomechanics, Commission on Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education, National Research Council. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 83, 859–895. doi: 10.1121/1.395965

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chermak, G. D., and Lee, J. (2005). Comparison of children’s performance on four tests of temporal resolution. J. Am. Acad. Audiol. 16, 554–563.

Google Scholar

Choe, N., Ousset, P. J., Andrieu, S., Mathiex-Fortunet, H., Vellas, B., and GuidAge Study Group. (2018). Memory complaints to the general practitioner: data from the GuidAge study. J. Nutr. Health Aging 12, S66–S72.

Google Scholar

Cohen, Y. E. (2009). Multimodal activity in the parietal cortex. Hear. Res. 258, 100–105. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2009.01.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coley, N., Ousset, P. J., Andrieu, S., Mathiex-Fortunet, H., Vellas, B., and The, et al. (2008). Memory complaints to the general practitioner: data from the GuidAge study. J. Nutr. Health Aging 12, S66–S72. doi: 10.1007/BF02982590

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crook, T. H., Feher, E. P., and Larrabee, G. J. (1992). Assessment of Memory Complaint in Age-Associated Memory Impairment: the MAC-Q. Int. Psychogeriatr. 4, 165–176. doi: 10.1017/S1041610292000991

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cusack, R. (2005). The Intraparietal Sulcus and Perceptual Organization. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 641–651. doi: 10.1162/0898929053467541

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deal, J. A., Betz, J., Yaffe, K., Harris, T., Purchase-Helzner, E., Satterfield, S., et al. (2016). Hearing impairment and incident dementia and cognitive decline in older adults: the Health ABC Study. J. Gerontol. Ser. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 72, 703–709. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glw069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deike, S., Gaschler-Markefski, B., Brechmann, A., and Scheich, H. (2004). Auditory stream segregation relying on timbre involves left auditory cortex. NeuroReport 15, 1511–1514. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000132919.12990.34

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deike, S., Scheich, H., and Brechmann, A. (2010). Active stream segregation specifically involves the left human auditory cortex. Hear. Res. 265, 30–37. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2010.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Devanand, D. P., Bansal, R., Liu, J., Hao, X., Pradhaban, G., and Peterson, B. S. (2012). MRI hippocampal and entorhinal cortex mapping in predicting conversion to Alzheimer’s disease. NeuroImage 60, 1622–1629. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edwards, J. D., Lister, J. J., Elias, M. N., Tetlow, A. M., Sardina, A. L., Sadeq, N. A., et al. (2017). Auditory Processing of Older Adults With Probable Mild Cognitive Impairment. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 60, 1427–1435.

Google Scholar

Fifer, R. C., Jerger, J. F., Berlin, C. I., Tobey, E. A., and Campbell, J. C. (1983). Development of a dichotic sentence identification test for hearing-impaired adults. Ear Hear. 4, 300–305.

Google Scholar

Fortunato, S., Forli, F., Guglielmi, V., De Corso, E., Paludetti, G., Berrettini, S., et al. (2016). A review of new insights on the association between hearing loss and cognitive decline in ageing. Acta Otorhinolaryngol. Ital. 36, 155–166. doi: 10.14639/0392-100x-993

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friedman, R. A., Van Laer, L., Huentelman, M. J., Sheth, S. S., Van Eyken, E., Corneveaux, J. J., et al. (2009). GRM7 variants confer susceptibility to age-related hearing impairment. Hum. Mol. Genet. 18, 785–796. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddn402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fuster, J. M. (2001). The Prefrontal Cortex—An Update: Time Is of the Essence Neuron, Vol. 30. Amsterdam: Elsevier Inc, 319–333.

Google Scholar

Gates, G. A., Anderson, M. L., Feeney, M. P., McCurry, S. M., and Larson, E. B. (2008). Central auditory dysfunction in older persons with memory impairment or Alzheimer’s dementia. Arch. Otolaryngol. Head Neck Surg. 134, 771–777.

Google Scholar

Gates, G. A., Anderson, M. L., McCurry, S. M., Feeney, M. P., and Larson, E. B. (2011). Central auditory dysfunction as a harbinger of Alzheimer’s dementia. (Clinical report). Arch. Otolaryngol. Head Neck Surg. 137:390.

Google Scholar

Gates, G. A., Beiser, A., Rees, T. S., D’ Agostino, R. B., Wolf, P. A., Gates, G. A., et al. (2002). Central Auditory Dysfunction May Precede the Onset of Clinical Dementia in People with Probable Alzheimer’s Disease. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 50, 482–488. doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2002.50114.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gates, G. A., Cobb, J. L., Linn, R. T., Rees, T., Wolf, P. A., and D’Agostino, R. B. (1996). Central auditory dysfunction, cognitive dysfunction, and dementia in older people. Arch. Otolaryngol. Head Neck Surg. 122, 161–167.

Google Scholar

Gates, G. A., Couropmitree, N. N., and Myers, R. H. (1999). Genetic Associations in Age-Related Hearing Thresholds. Arch. Otolaryngol. Head Neck Surg. 125, 654.

Google Scholar

Gates, G. A., Gibbons, L. E., McCurry, S. M., Crane, P. K., Feeney, M. P., and Larson, E. B. (2010). Executive dysfunction and presbycusis in older persons with and without memory loss and dementia. Cogn. Behav. Neurol. 23, 218. doi: 10.1097/WNN.0b013e3181d748d7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gates, G. A., and Mills, J. H. (2005). Presbycusis. Lancet 366, 1111–1120.

Google Scholar

Gelfand, J. R., and Bookheimer, S. Y. (2003). Dissociating Neural Mechanisms of Temporal Sequencing and Processing Phonemes. Neuron 38, 831–842. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00285-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glisky, E. L. (2007). Changes in Cognitive Function in Human Aging Brain Aging: Models, Methods, and Mechanisms. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press, 3–20.

Google Scholar

Glodzik-Sobanska, L., Reisberg, B., De Santi, S., Babb, J. S., Pirraglia, E., Rich, K. E., et al. (2007). Subjective memory complaints: presence, severity and future outcome in normal older subjects. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 24, 177–184. doi: 10.1159/000105604

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Golden, H. L., Nicholas, J. M., Yong, K. X. X., Downey, L. E., Schott, J. M., Mummery, C. J., et al. (2015). Auditory spatial processing in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 138, 189–202. doi: 10.1093/brain/awu337

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gutschalk, A., Oxenham, A. J., Micheyl, C., Wilson, E. C., and Melcher, J. R. (2007). Human Cortical Activity during Streaming without Spectral Cues Suggests a General Neural Substrate for Auditory Stream Segregation. J. Neurosci. 27, 13074–13081. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2299-07.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hardy, J. (2017). The discovery of Alzheimer-causing mutations in the APP gene and the formulation of the “amyloid cascade hypothesis”. FEBS J. 284, 1040–1044. doi: 10.1111/febs.14004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Humes, L. E., Dubno, J. R., Gordon-Salant, S., Lister, J. J., Cacace, A. T., Cruickshanks, K. J., et al. (2012). Central presbycusis: a review and evaluation of the evidence. J. Am. Acad. Audiol. 23, 635–666. doi: 10.3766/jaaa.23.8.5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Idrizbegovic, E., Hederstierna, C., Dahlquist, M., and Rosenhall, U. (2013). Short-term longitudinal study of central auditory function in Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Dis. Extra 3, 468–471. doi: 10.1159/000355371

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jack, C. R., Albert, M. S., Knopman, D. S., McKhann, G. M., Sperling, R. A., Carrillo, M. C., et al. (2011). Introduction to the recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s Dement. 7, 257–262. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jayakody, D. M. P., Friedland, P. L., Eikelboom, R. H., Martins, R. N., and Sohrabi, H. R. (2017). A novel study on the association between untreated hearing loss and cognitive functions of older adults: baseline non-verbal cognitive assessment results. Clin. Otolaryngol. 43, 182–191. doi: 10.1111/coa.12937

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jerger, J., and Jerger, S. (1974). Auditory findings in brain stem disorders. Arch. Otolaryngol. 99, 342–350. doi: 10.1001/archotol.1974.00780030354006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jessen, F., Amariglio, R. E., van Boxtel, M., Breteler, M., Ceccaldi, M., Chételat, G., et al. (2014). A conceptual framework for research on subjective cognitive decline in pre-clinical Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s Dement. 10, 844–852. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2014.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karlsson, K. K., Harris, J. R., and Svartengren, M. (1997). Description and primary results from an audiometric study of male twins. Ear Hear. 18, 114. doi: 10.1097/00003446-199704000-00003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Killion, M. C., Niquette, P. A., Gudmundsen, G. I., Revit, L. J., and Banerjee, S. (2004). Development of a quick speech-in-noise test for measuring signal-to-noise ratio loss in normal-hearing and hearing-impaired listeners. J. Acous. Soc. Am. 16, 2395–2405.

Google Scholar

Koivisto, K., Reinikainen, K. J., Hänninen, T., Vanhanen, M., Helkala, E. L., Mykkänen, L., et al., (1995). Prevalence of age-associated memory impairment in a randomly selected population from eastern Finland. Neurology 45, 741–747.

Google Scholar

Kurniawan, C., Westendorp, R. G., de Craen, A. J., Gussekloo, J., de Laat, J., and van Exel, E. (2012). Gene dose of apolipoprotein E and age-related hearing loss. Neurobiol. Aging 33, 2230.e2237–2230.e2212. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2012.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lambert, M. A., Bickel, H., Prince, M., Fratiglioni, L., Von Strauss, E., Frydecka, D., et al. (2014). Estimating the burden of early-onset dementia; systematic review of disease prevalence. Eur. J. Neurol. 21, 563–569.

Google Scholar

Lin, F. R., Metter, E. J., O’Brien, R. J., Resnick, S. M., Zonderman, A. B., and Ferrucci, L. (2011a). Hearing loss and incident dementia. Arch. Neurol. 68, 214–220. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2010.362

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, F. R., Thorpe, R., Gordon-Salant, S., and Ferrucci, L. (2011b). Hearing Loss Prevalence and Risk Factors Among Older Adults in the United States. J. Gerontol. Ser. A 66A, 582–590. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glr002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, F. R., Yaffe, K., Xia, J., Xue, Q.-L., Harris, T. B., Purchase-Helzner, E., et al. (2013). Hearing loss and cognitive decline in older adults. JAMA Int. Med. 173, 293–299.

Google Scholar

Lindenberger, U., and Baltes, P. B. (1994). Sensory Functioning and Intelligence in Old Age: a Strong Connection. Psychol. Aging 9, 339–355.

Google Scholar

Livingston, G., Huntley, J., Sommerlad, A., Ames, D., Ballard, C., Banerjee, S., et al. (2020). Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the Lancet Commission. Lancet 396, 413–446. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30367-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McKhann, G. M., Knopman, D. S., Chertkow, H., Hyman, B. T., Jack, C. R., Kawas, C. H., et al. (2011). The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s Dement. 7, 263–269. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Medwetsky, L. (1994). The Importance of Spatial and Talker Cues in Competing Sentence Recall. Unpublished Doctoral thesis, City University of New York, New York, NY.

Google Scholar

Medwetsky, L. (2011). Spoken language processing model: bridging auditory and language processing to guide assessment and intervention. Lang. Speech Hear. Serv. Sch. 42, 286–296. doi: 10.1044/0161-1461(2011/10-0036)

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mener, D. J., Betz, J., Yaffe, K., Harris, T. B., Helzner, E. P., Satterfield, S., et al. (2016). Apolipoprotein E Allele and Hearing Thresholds in Older Adults. Am. J. Alzheimers Dis. Other Dement. 31, 34–39. doi: 10.1177/1533317514537549

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Musiek, E. F. (1983). Assessment of Central Auditory Dysfunction: the Dichotic Digit Test Revisited. Ear Hear. 4, 79–83. doi: 10.1097/00003446-198303000-00002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Musiek, F. E. (1994). Frequency (pitch) and duration pattern tests. J. Am. Acad. Audiol. 5, 265–268.

Google Scholar

Musiek, F. E., and Chermak, G. D. (1994). Three commonly asked questions about central auditory processing disorders: assessment. Am. J. Audiol. 3, 23–27. doi: 10.1044/1059-0889.0303.23

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Musiek, F. E., and Chermak, G. D. (2013). Handbook of Central Auditory Processing Disorder Volume 1 Auditory Neuroscience and Diagnosis, 2nd Edn, Vol. 1. San Diego, CA: Plural Publishing, Inc.

Google Scholar

Musiek, F. E., Gollegly, K. M., Kibbe, K. S., and Verkest-Lenz, S. B. (1991). Proposed screening test for central auditory disorders: follow up on the dichotic digits test. Test Otol. Neurotol. 12, 109–113.

Google Scholar

Nasreddine, Z. S., Phillips, N. A., Bedirian, V., Charbonneau, S., Whitehead, V., Collin, I., et al. (2005). The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 53, 695–699. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Grady, G., Boyles, A. L., Speer, M., DeRuyter, F., Strittmatter, W., and Worley, G. (2007). Apolipoprotein E alleles and sensorineural hearing loss. Int. J. Audiol. 46, 183–186. doi: 10.1080/14992020601145294

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Orchik, D. J., and Burgess, J. (1977). Synthetic sentence identification as a function of the age of the listener. Ear Hear. 3, 42–46.

Google Scholar

Ouda, L., and Syka, J. (2012). Immunocytochemical profiles of inferior colliculus neurons in the rat and their changes with aging. Front. Neural Circ. 6:68. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2012.00068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panza, F., Lozupone, M., Sardone, R., Battista, P., Piccininni, M., Dibello, V., et al. (2018). Sensorial frailty: age-related hearing loss and the risk of cognitive impairment and dementia in later life. Ther. Adv. Chronic Dis. 10:2040622318811000.

Google Scholar

Poorkaj, P., Sharma, V., Anderson, L., Nemens, E., Alonso, M. E., Orr, H., et al. (1998). Missense mutations in the chromosome 14 familial Alzheimer’s disease presenilin 1 gene. Hum. Mutat. 11, 216–221. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1098-1004199811:3<216::aid-humu6>3.0.co;2-f

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prince, M., Wimo, A., Guerchet, M., Ali, G. C., Wu, Y. T., and Prina, M. (2015). World Alzheimer’s Report 2015—the Global Impact of Dementia: An Analysis of Prevalence, Incidence, Cost and Trends. London: Alzheimer’s Disease International.

Google Scholar

Quaranta, N., Coppola, F., Casulli, M., Barulli, O., Lanza, F., Tortelli, R., et al. (2015). The prevalence of peripheral and central hearing impairment and its relation to cognition in older adults. Audiol. Neurootol. 19, 10–14. doi: 10.1159/000371597

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodda, J., Okello, A., Edison, P., Dannhauser, T., Brooks, D. J., and Walker, Z. (2010). (11)C-PIB PET in subjective cognitive impairment. Eur. Psychiatry 25, 123–125. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2009.07.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rönnlund, M., Sundström, A., Adolfsson, R., Nilsson, L.-G., Rönnlund, M., Sundström, A., et al. (2015). Subjective memory impairment in older adults predicts future dementia independent of baseline memory performance: Evidence from the Betula prospective cohort study. Alzheimers Dement. 11, 1385–1392. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2014.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossetti, H. C., Lacritz, L. H., Cullum, C. M., and Weiner, M. F. (2011). Normative data for the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) in a population-based sample. Neurology 77, 1272–1275.

Google Scholar

Scheef, T. L., Spottke, J. A., Daerr, H. M., Joe, H. A., Striepens, H. N., Kölsch, H. H., et al. (2012). Glucose metabolism, gray matter structure, and memory decline in subjective memory impairment. Neurology 79, 1332–1339. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31826c1a8d

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, B. A., and Pichora-Fuller, M. K. (2000). Implications of Perceptual Deterioration for Cognitive Aging Research The Handbook of Aging and Cognition, 2nd Edn. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers, 155–219.

Google Scholar

Schönwiesner, M., Novitski, N., Pakarinen, S., Carlson, S., Tervaniemi, M., and Näätänen, R. (2007). Heschl’s Gyrus, Posterior Superior Temporal Gyrus, and Mid-Ventrolateral Prefrontal Cortex Have Different Roles in the Detection of Acoustic Changes. J. Neurophysiol. 97, 2075–2082. doi: 10.1152/jn.01083.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Serrano-Pozo, A., Frosch, M. P., Masliah, E., and Hyman, B. T. (2011). Neuropathological alterations in Alzheimer’s disease. Cold Spring Harb. Perspect. Med. 1:a006189. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a006189

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sohrabi, H. R., Bates, K. A., Weinborn, M. G., Johnston, A. N. B., Bahramian, A., Taddei, K., et al. (2012). Olfactory discrimination predicts cognitive decline among community-dwelling older adults. Transl. Psychiatry 2:e118.

Google Scholar

Sohrabi, H. R., Weinborn, M., Laske, C., Bates, K. A., Christensen, D., Taddei, K., et al. (2019). Subjective memory complaints predict baseline but not future cognitive function over three years: results from the Western Australia Memory Study. Int. Psychogeriatr. 31, 513–525.

Google Scholar

Speaks, C., Karmen, J. L., and Benitez, L. (1967). Effect of a competing message on synthetic sentence identification. J. Speech Hear. Res. 10, 390–395. doi: 10.1044/jshr.1002.390

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sperling, R. A., Aisen, P. S., Beckett, L. A., Bennett, D. A., Craft, S., Fagan, A. M., et al. (2011). Toward defining the pre-clinical stages of Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s Dement. 7, 280–292. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tuwaig, M., Savard, M., Jutras, B., Poirier, J., Collins, D. L., Rosa-Neto, P., et al. (2017). Deficit in central auditory processing as a biomarker of pre-clinical Alzheimer’s disease. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 60, 1589–1600. doi: 10.3233/JAD-170545

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walton, J. P., Frisina, R. D., and O’Neill, W. E. (1998). Age-related alteration in the processing of temporal sound features in the auditory midbrain of the CBA mouse. J. Neurosci. 18, 2764–2776. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-07-02764.1998

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Y., West, J. D., Flashman, L. A., Wishart, H. A., Santulli, R. B., Rabin, L. A., et al. (2011). Selective changes in white matter integrity in MCI and older adults with cognitive complaints. BBA Mol. Basis Dis. 1822:2. doi: 10.1016/j.bbadis.2011.08.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Willott, J. F. (1996). Anatomic and physiologic aging: a behavioral neuroscience perspective. J. Am. Acad. Audiol. 7, 141–151.

Google Scholar

Zatorre, R. J., and Belin, P. (2001). Spectral and temporal processing in human auditory cortex. Cereb. Cortex 11, 946–953. doi: 10.1093/cercor/11.10.946

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: hearing loss, central auditory processing, dementia, subjective memory complaints, APOE-ε4

Citation: Jayakody DMP, Menegola HK, Yiannos JM, Goodman-Simpson J, Friedland PL, Taddei K, Laws SM, Weinborn M, Martins RN and Sohrabi HR (2020) The Peripheral Hearing and Central Auditory Processing Skills of Individuals With Subjective Memory Complaints. Front. Neurosci. 14:888. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2020.00888

Received: 04 October 2019; Accepted: 30 July 2020;
Published: 21 August 2020.

Edited by:

Mary Rudner, Linköping University, Sweden

Reviewed by:

Samira Anderson, University of Maryland, College Park, United States
Kathy Pichora-Fuller, University of Toronto Mississauga, Canada

Copyright © 2020 Jayakody, Menegola, Yiannos, Goodman-Simpson, Friedland, Taddei, Laws, Weinborn, Martins and Sohrabi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Dona M. P. Jayakody, dona.jayakody@earscience.org.au; Hamid R. Sohrabi, Hamid.Sohrabi@murdoch.edu.au

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.