Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 14 August 2019
Sec. Neurodegeneration
This article is part of the Research Topic Prevention of Alzheimer’s Disease: from Cognitive Reserve to Precision Medicine View all 11 articles

Risk Factors and Neuropsychological Assessments of Subjective Cognitive Decline (plus) in Chinese Memory Clinic

\r\nLixiao Hao,Lixiao Hao1,2Yue XingYue Xing3Xuanyu LiXuanyu Li4Bin MuBin Mu4Weina Zhao,Weina Zhao4,5Gubing WangGubing Wang6Ting WangTing Wang7Jianguo Jia*Jianguo Jia8*Ying Han,,,*Ying Han4,9,10,11*
  • 1Department of Geriatrics, Xuanwu Hospital of Capital Medical University, Beijing, China
  • 2Department of General Practice, Xuanwu Hospital of Capital Medical University, Beijing, China
  • 3Radiological Sciences, Division of Clinical Neuroscience, Queen’s Medical Centre, The University of Nottingham, Nottingham, United Kingdom
  • 4Department of Neurology, Xuanwu Hospital of Capital Medical University, Beijing, China
  • 5Department of Neurology, Hongqi Hospital of Mudanjiang Medical University, Mudanjiang, China
  • 6Faculty of Industrial Design Engineering, Delft University of Technology, Delft, Netherlands
  • 7Department of General Practice, School of General Practice and Continuing Education of Capital Medical University, Beijing, China
  • 8Department of General Surgery, Xuanwu Hospital of Capital Medical University, Beijing, China
  • 9Beijing Institute of Geriatrics, Beijing, China
  • 10National Clinical Research Center for Geriatric Disorders, Beijing, China
  • 11Center of Alzheimer’s Disease, Beijing Institute for Brain Disorders, Beijing, China

Background: Since subjective cognitive decline (SCD) was standardized in 2014, many studies have investigated its features. However, the risk of SCD (plus) progressing to AD is much higher, and yet there have been few studies reporting the risk factors and neuropsychological assessment characteristics of SCD (plus).

Objective: To characterize SCD (plus) by comparing it with normal control (NC), amnesic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI), and Alzheimer Disease (AD) regarding their demographics, lifestyle, family history of dementia, multimorbidity and the neuropsychological assessments.

Methods: A total of 135 participants were recruited, including 23 NC, 30 SCD (plus), 45 aMCI and 37 AD. Descriptive statistics were provided. A logistic regression model was used to analyze the affecting factors of SCD (plus), and finally the Receiver Operating Characteristic (ROC) analysis was applied to distinguish between SCD (plus) and NC.

Results: (1) SCD (plus) group was younger than both the aMCI group and AD group. It consisted of more participants with mental work and higher body mass index (BMI) than the AD group. (2) Scores of Auditory Verbal Learning Test - Immediate recall (AVLT-IR) and AVLT-Long delayed recall (AVLT-LR) decreased in the following order: NC→SCD (plus)→aMCI→AD. (3) The Area Under Curve (AUC) for discriminating SCD (plus) and NC group was from 0.673 to 0.838.

Conclusion: Aging is an important risk factor of both NC progressing to SCD (plus), and SCD (plus) progressing to aMCI or AD. In addition to aging, lower education level and lower BMI were significantly associated with greater odds of SCD (plus) progressing to aMCI or AD patients, whereas mental work was a protective factor of SCD (plus) progressing to AD. Finally, AVLT is a sensitive indicator of the cognitive decline and impairment in SCD (plus) in relative to normal controls.

Introduction

World Alzheimer Reports showed that there were 47 million people living with dementia worldwide in 2016 (Alzheimer’s Disease International, 2016). This number is expected to increase to more than 131 million by 2050. The total estimated worldwide cost for dementia is US$ 818 billion, and by 2018 dementia will become a trillion-dollar disease. However, no effective modifying therapy has been validated yet, even for mild cognitive impairment (MCI) (Sperling et al., 2011).

Pre-mild cognitive impairment subjective cognitive decline (pre-MCI SCD), which has been defined as a self-experienced persistent decline in cognitive capacity in comparison with a previously normal status and objective cognitive performance within normal ranges (Jessen et al., 2014), is the first symptomatic manifestation of Alzheimer Disease (AD) and has received increasing attention (Rabin et al., 2015; Fernandez-Blazquez et al., 2016; Vogel et al., 2016). Individuals present with several specific features (e.g., age at onset of SCD ≥60 years; complaints about SCD within the past 5 years; the complainers feel their performance are not as good as their peers and concerning associated with SCD, etc.) associated with pre-MCI SCD would be identified as SCD (plus) (Jessen et al., 2014)* – one of the preclinical stages of AD [See the appendix for the details of clinical features of SCD (plus)]. To our best knowledge, the risk of progressing to AD is higher for SCD (plus) than for SCD (Jessen et al., 2014). Accurate identification of SCD (plus) is therefore fundamental and crucial for early and successful intervention, which may help slow down its progression to AD and improve the prognosis.

Previous studies on SCD demographics showed people with SCD were younger and had a higher education than patients with MCI and AD dementia (Jonker et al., 2000; Garcia-Ptacek et al., 2014). Women were reported to be more likely to have SCD than men (Jonker et al., 2000). Less physical activity, hypertension, smoking and depression were found to be associated with the increase occurrence of SCD (Paradise et al., 2011; Chen et al., 2014). Also, among workers, those with cognitively demanding work were more sensitive to the changes of cognitive decline, and were thus more likely to report SCD (Rijs et al., 2015). Moreover, Aarts et al. (2011) demonstrated that SCD was related with multiple comorbidities (e.g., diabetes mellitus, stroke/transient ischemic attack, myocardial infarction, etc.). Cerebral trauma, middle-aged obesity, marital status (unmarried and widowed), born in the countryside, low social contact, and daily drinking were regarded as the risk factors for cognitive decline (Ao and Liu, 2004; Williams et al., 2010; Deng, 2014; Hao et al., 2017). In addition, a few studies showed that lower body mass index (BMI) associated with sarcopenia, was closely linked with the development of AD (Sugimoto et al., 2016; Ogawa et al., 2018). However, all these studies focused either on SCD or pre-MCI SCD, and little is known about the risk factors for SCD (plus) as a different category of patients.

Subjective cognitive decline may not a demonstrate detectable objective impairment when using the standardized neuropsychological assessment (Rabin et al., 2017), but it is still unknown about whether those assessments are useful for identifying SCD (plus). In addition, many studies have shown the distinction between SCD and MCI or AD, by using these neuropsychological assessments, such as MoCA, CDT and AVLT (Fernaeus et al., 2014; Zhao et al., 2015; Vyhnalek et al., 2017; Wang et al., 2019). Their high sensitivity and specificity have also been reported previously, suggesting that those assessments were able to distinguish SCD from MCI and AD (Vyhnalek et al., 2014; Huang et al., 2018; Park et al., 2018; Xu et al., 2018). However, little is known about the diagnostic power of these tools in discriminating SCD (plus) and NC. Furthermore, according to the diagnosis framework of SCD or SCD (plus), like NC, their scores of objective neuropsychological assessments are within the normal range. However, the chance of SCD (plus) progressing to MCI or dementia was significantly higher than the normal controls. Therefore, the distinguishing features between the SCD (plus) and NC groups are of great significance to study, as these could be more practically important for identifying people with SCD (plus) at an early stage and facilitate early intervention.

Therefore, our current study aims to (1) examine the relationship between SCD (plus) and the following potential risk factors including: demographics, family history of dementia, comorbidities, history of cigarettes smoking and drinking, and (2) to assess the sensitivity of the standard neuropsychological assessments on detecting SCD (plus) by comparing its scores with normal controls (NC), amnesic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI) and AD dementia. The ultimate purpose is to characterize SCD (plus) in order to provide more information for its early identification and intervention.

Materials and Methods

Ethics Statement

This study was approved by the Medical Ethics Committee of Xuanwu Hospital of Capital Medical University, Beijing, China. Written informed consent was obtained from either participants or their legally agreed surrogates.

Participants

One Hundred thirty-five right-handed, Han Chinese subjects, including 30 SCD (plus), 45 aMCI, and 37 AD patients were recruited from the memory clinic of the Neurology Department, Xuanwu Hospital, Capital Medical University, Beijing, China from December 1, 2010 to June 30, 2016. 23 NC subjects were recruited from the hospital by advertisements.

Assessment and Diagnosis Procedure

All of the subjects underwent a series of standardized clinical evaluations, including the Chinese version of Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) (Katzman et al., 1988), Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA)- the Beijing version (Lu et al., 2011), Clinical Dementia Rating Scale (CDR) (Morris, 1993), World Health Organization-University of California Los Angeles Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT) (Maj et al., 1994), Hachinski Ischemic Index (HIS) (Hachinski et al., 1975), Hamilton Anxiety Scale (HAMA) (Tang, 1984), and Hamilton Depression Scale (HAMD) (Hamilton, 1980).

Inclusion criteria for each group: All SCD (plus) subjects met the criteria for SCD (plus) proposed by SCD-I (Jessen et al., 2014): (a) presence of self-perceived continuous memory decline within the last 5 years, confirmed by informant report; (b) onset age ≥ 60 years old; (c) feeling cognitive decline worse than their peers and concerned about SCD; (d) normal performance on both MMSE, MoCA and AVLT after age and education adjustment; (e) CDR score = 0; (f) no impairment of daily life activities; and (g) HIS score ≤4.

The aMCI patients met the following criteria (Petersen, 2004): (a) memory complaint, preferably confirmed by an informant; (b) objective memory impairment; (c) normal or near-normal performance on general cognitive function with no or minimum impairment of daily life activities; (d) CDR score = 0.5; (e) HIS score ≤4; and (f) failure to meet the criteria of dementia according to DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association, 1994).

The diagnosis of AD fulfilled standardized diagnostic criteria (McKhann et al., 1984; American Psychiatric Association, 1994; Dubois et al., 2007): (a) met the diagnostic criteria of dementia; (b) gradual and progressive decline in memory function over more than 6 months; (c) impaired episodic memory on objective testing; (d) HIS score ≤4; and (e) hippocampal atrophy confirmed by structural MRI.

Criteria of NC was defined as: (a) having no report of any cognition complaint; (b) normal performance on MMSE, MoCA and AVLT after age and education-adjusted; (c) CDR score = 0; and (d) no impairment of daily life activities.

Exclusion criteria for all the subjects: (a) a history of stroke (HIS >4); (b) severe depression and anxiety (HAMD >30, and HAMA ≥29); (c) other CNS diseases which could cause cognitive decline (e.g., brain tumors, Parkinson’s disease, encephalitis, or epilepsy); (d) other systemic diseases which could cause cognitive decline (e.g., thyroid dysfunction, severe anemia, syphilis, or HIV); (e) a history of psychosis or congenital mental growth retardation; (f) cognitive decline caused by traumatic brain injury; or (g) those who could not complete neuropsychological tests or with contraindication to MRI.

Statistical Analysis

We conducted all analyses using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences version 17.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, United States). Descriptive statistics (sociodemographic characteristics, lifestyle, comorbidities, family history of dementia and scores of neuropsychological assessments) were calculated by percentages or median. The χ2 or Kruskal–Wallis test was used to assess group differences between SCD (plus) and the other three groups (NC, aMCI and AD group). For the four groups comparison, p < 0.05 was considered to be statistically significant, and corrected p′ value (p < 0.007) was used in the Partitions of Pearson’s chi-square statistics for post hoc pairwise comparisons. To examine the potential risk factors for each group in relation to the SCD (plus) group, we performed multiple logistic regression analysis with the removed backwards approach by including the sociodemographic characteristics, lifestyle, comorbidities and family history of dementia as the independent variables, and diagnosis as the dependent variable. In addition, odds ratios (ORs) were calculated for each variable. p < 0.05 was required for variables to be remained in the model. Finally, we obtained the receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curves and calculated the area under curve (AUC) of the characteristic factors that distinguish the NC and SCD (plus) group.

Results

Clinical Characteristics of the Participants

The clinical characteristics of the total sample were summarized in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Clinical characteristics of the study sample.

Comparisons Between NC, SCD (plus), aMCI, and AD Groups

The difference between age among the four groups was significant (p < 0.05). Further pairwise comparison showed that the age of SCD (plus) was lower than the other two groups (aMCI and AD) at the corrected test level p1 (p < 0.001).

For BMI, the difference among the four groups was also significant (p < 0.05). For a pairwise comparison, we only found the number of people with the BMI ≤23.9 Kg/m2 in NC group (39.1%) was smaller than that in the AD group, but no difference was shown between SCD (plus) group (46.7%) and AD group (78.4%) (p > 0.007).

A family history of dementia was presented in 26.7% of the SCD (plus) group. The proportion of currently smoking and drinking in SCD (plus) group was 10.0 and 16.7%, respectively. No significant differences among the four groups were found for family history of dementia, smoking and drinking (p > 0.05).

The proportion of having hypertension, cerebrovascular disease, cardiovascular disease, diabetes, and hyperlipidemia as multimorbidity in the SCD (plus) group was 40.0, 13.3, 6.7, 23.3, 36.7 and 30.0%, respectively, but again there were no differences between the SCD (plus) group and the other groups (p > 0.05) (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Comparisons between NC, SCD (plus), aMCI, and AD groups for clinical characteristics.

Comparison of Scores of Neuropsychological Assessments Between NC, SCD (plus), aMCI, and AD

The scores of AVLT-First Immediate Free Recall (median AVLT-IR1 scores = 6) and AVLT-Long Delay Free Recall (median AVLT-LR = 9) of SCD (plus) group were lower than those of NC group (median values of AVLT-IR1 and AVLT-LR 7 and 11, respectively) (p < 0.05). We found no difference of the total score of MoCA between the SCD (plus) group and the NC group, the median scores of which was 25.5 and 28.0, respectively (p = 0.08).

All the other scores of neuropsychological assessments, including AVLT-Second Immediate Free Recall (AVLT-IR2), AVLT-Third Immediate Free Recall (AVLT-IR3), AVLT-Recognition Recall (AVLT-RR), total scores of MMSE and MoCA, and single cognitive domain scores of MoCA test -the Trail Making Test (TMT), copy cube, clock drawing test (CDT), naming, digit span, alertness test, continuous subtraction 7, repeat sentence, Verbal Fluency Test (VFT), abstract test, MoCA-Delay Free Recall (MoCA-DR) and orientation test, were higher in SCD (plus) group compared to those in the aMCI and AD groups (p < 0.05). However, no significant differences were found between SCD (plus) and NC group (p > 0.05) (Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Comparison between SCD (plus) and NC, aMCI and AD group for scores of neuropsychological assessments.

Affecting Factors for NC, SCD (plus), aMCI and AD Groups

Our results of the multiple logistic regression analysis showed that aging, years of education, job category and BMI were affecting factors of SCD (plus). Aging was an important risk factor for SCD (plus) progressing to aMCI (OR = 0.05, 95% CI = 0.01–0.41) and AD (OR = 0.03, 95% CI = 0.01–0.39), which also showed a certain risk effect on the progression of NC to SCD (plus) subjects (OR = 0.10, 95% CI = 0.01–0.93) (p < 0.05). Mental work had a protective effect on SCD (plus) progressing to AD patients (p < 0.05), whereas lower education (OR = 4.43, 95% CI = 1.03–19.18) and lower BMI (OR = 3.73, 95% CI = 1.08–12.98) were significantly associated with greater odds of SCD (plus) progressing to AD patients (p < 0.05) (see Table 4).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Affecting Factors for NC, SCD (plus), aMCI and AD groups.

ROC of NC and SCD (plus)

We used the ROC curves to evaluate the goodness of the affecting features and neuropsychological scores, respectively, on discriminating the SCD (plus) group from the NC group. As a variable that differed significantly between all the groups, we first used age as the factor and found its AUC was 0.592 (95% CI, 0.434–0.750), which was low. To further explore the optimal discriminating model, we continued to add more factors and we found that by using age, gender, years of education, job category, BMI, current smoking, and current drinking, the AUC reached 0.673 (95% CI, 0.524–0.823).

Based on the results of the neuropsychological assessments, we also performed ROC analysis by using scores of AVLT-LR and AVLT-IR1 as variables, and the AUCs were 0.679 (95% CI, 0.535–0.823) and 0.662 (95% CI, 0.506–0.819), respectively. Finally, we added the clinical features including the demographics and life styles as variables in the logistic regression in addition to AVLT-LR or AVLT-IR1, respectively, and we found that the AUC increased to 0.823 (95% CI, 0.708–0.938) and 0.764 (95% CI, 0.631–0.897). When combining AVLT-IR1, AVLT-LR and clinical variables in the regression model, the AUC increased from 0.673 to 0.838 (95% CI, 0.729–0.948) (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. ROCs for NC and SCD (plus) group. ROC curves for NC and SCD (plus) group using clinical features, AVLT-IR and AVLT-LR. AVLT-IR1, Auditory Verbal Learning Test-First Immediate Free Recall; AVLT-LR, Auditory Verbal Learning Test-Long Delay Free Recall; ROC, receiver operating characteristic; CF, clinical features; RE, reference.

Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, the risk of progressing to AD is higher for SCD (plus) than for SCD, but there have been few studies reporting its risk factors and neuropsychological assessment characteristics. This is the first study that reveals the presence of early episodic memory impairment in SCD (plus) population in memory clinic by fulfilling all the six items of the diagnostic framework. This study also characterized the clinical features and neuropsychological assessments of SCD (plus) in relative to normal controls, amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI) and Alzheimer disease (AD).

We identified lower age, longer education period, and more mental work as the demographic characteristics of SCD (plus) group, compared with aMCI and AD group, which are in agreement with the previous studies (Mohs et al., 2001; Sando et al., 2008; Hebert et al., 2010; Iliffe and Pealing, 2010; Jefferson et al., 2011; Meng and D’Arcy, 2012; Alzheimer’s Association, 2014; Beydoun et al., 2014; Garcia-Ptacek et al., 2014; Liang et al., 2018). We also showed that higher BMI was a protective factor for SCD (plus) progressing to AD patients, which was consistent with some of the previous studies (Barnes et al., 2009; Fitzpatrick et al., 2009; Xu et al., 2011; Gustafson and Luchsinger, 2013; Qizilbash et al., 2015). Latent AD may be accompanied by the metabolic changes that are not yet fully understood. Yet this phenomenon may be associated with the changes of body composition with aging, such as the morphological changes of fat cells and the adipose tissue compartment, diminished muscle mass, sarcopenia, and somatic frailty. For women who are affected by the reproductive aging and changes in the sex hormone milieu, it may be related to the alteration of adipose tissue. On the contrary, other researchers argued that obesity increased the risk of dementia (Beydoun et al., 2008; Profenno et al., 2010; Sellbom and Gunstad, 2012; Loef and Walach, 2013), which is also greatly correlated with other morbidities, such as hypertension and diabetes (Luchsinger and Gustafson, 2009). These inconsistencies may be attributed to the error in the measurement of adiposity. Besides, other factors (Devore et al., 2010; Littlejohns et al., 2014; Emmerzaal et al., 2015; Qizilbash et al., 2015), such as cholesterol levels, age-related regulatory changes in carbohydrate, lipid or protein metabolism, increased intake of vitamin E, anti-oxidant and vitamin D may all affect the relationship between BMI and dementia. It is also possible that the higher BMI was the result of having olfaction firstly affected in the progression of AD (Gustafson and Luchsinger, 2013).

Smoking was regarded as a risk factor of dementia (Lee et al., 2010; Beydoun et al., 2014; Zhong et al., 2015), but we found no statistical differences between all the groups, which may be due to the low percentage of smokers in our groups. Also, there was no significant correlation between alcohol consumption and cognitive impairment, which was in line with previous findings (Baumgart et al., 2015).

Earlier studies have found that hypertension in later life was a protective factor for cognitive decline (Lee et al., 2010; Beydoun et al., 2014), whereas diabetes, hyperlipidemia and cerebrovascular disease increased the risk of dementia (Honig et al., 2003; Profenno et al., 2010; Gudala et al., 2013; Roberts et al., 2014; Cooper et al., 2015). In our study, we did not find significant difference of comorbidities including hypertension, cerebrovascular disease, cardiovascular disease, diabetes and hyperlipidemia between groups, and this inconsistency might be due to the following reasons: (1) Those chronic diseases were not stratified according to the sex, disease duration and severity. A few studies showed that middle-aged individuals with hypertension and diabetes for longer than 6 years had a positive correlation with cognitive decline (Helzner et al., 2009), Sex difference in the presence of comorbidities which involve the vascular contributions to the cognitive impairment and dementia should be considered (Gannon et al., 2018); and (2) our patients did not co-present as many diseases as they were shown in one previous study, such as arthritis, prostate disease, lung disease and so on (Aarts et al., 2011).

In our study, after controlling the clinical characteristic variables, the SCD (plus) group showed lower scores of AVLT1 and AVLT-LR than those of the NC group (p < 0.05). This suggests that memory impairment has already presented in SCD (plus) population at the AD preclinical stage. We also showed that the combination of AVLT-IR1 and AVLT-LR improved the diagnostic accuracy of SCD (plus) compared to the condition when they were used separately, which indicates that AVLT may allow for distinguishing SCD (plus) form NC individuals. It also suggests that in order to better identify SCD (plus), episodic memory should be included as part of the neuropsychological assessment. Delayed recall in AVLT is considered to be the most sensitive measure of early AD (Zhao et al., 2012). However, not all studies have included this test (Yang et al., 2015). Our SCD (plus) individuals showed slightly worse performance on the challenging cognitive tasks than individuals without cognitive complaints (Koppara et al., 2015; Smart and Krawitz, 2015). Also, compared to no complaints, reduced episodic memory learning effect and poorer performance on psychomotor speed and language were present in SCD participants (Kielb et al., 2017). However, a different cutoff value from what is currently used for discriminating MCI from NC may need to be provided in the future to differ SCD (plus) from NC with better sensitivity and reliability. For the global cognition revealed by MoCA and MMSE, although the differences between the SCD (plus) and NC groups were not significant, MoCA showed a higher sensitivity of assessment compared with MMSE. Thus, MoCA may be helpful to be included in the SCD (plus) screening scales, but further verification is needed by follow-up studies.

In addition, our study found that except for memory impairment, the other cognitive domains also had begun to decline from the aMCI stage (p < 0.05). It indicates that once a patient enters the stage of MCI, other cognitive domains may also have been damaged. Deng (2014) found that 54.2% elderly in the a different cohort had cognitive impairment, and the abstract scores were lower in both the normal control and cognitive impairment groups. The memory impairment group scored lower in the domains of execution, visual space, language and delayed recall. MCI is often characterized by slight but noticeable deficits in attention, learning and memory, executive function, processing speed, and semantic language (Storandt et al., 2006; Saunders and Summers, 2011; Summers and Saunders, 2012), and the early cognitive impairment of these domain are also strong predictors of the progression from MCI to AD (Brandt et al., 2009; Klekociuk et al., 2014). In order to be sensitive to the impairment of single cognitive domain in SCD (plus), questionnaires designed for screening patients with cognitive impairments need to report not only the global cognition scales (such as MMSE, MoCA), but also the scores of single cognitive domains, such as language and execution etc.

The limitations of this study are: (1) our sample size is relatively small, which might be the cause of some of the negative results between groups. Further investigations with larger sample sizes are needed; (2) Our study is a cross-sectional survey and follow-up studies should be performed to further confirm the final conclusions; (3) We adopted the standardized criteria of pre-MCI SCD proposed in 2014 by Jessen (Jessen et al., 2014) given depression and anxiety maybe the early presentations of the SCD (plus). We admit that using this criterion, some of the subjects would have mild to moderate anxiety and depression. However, the present study did not address this issue, which would be of interest to study further. (4) The diagnosis of SCD (plus) was not validated by the other tests. For instance, it lacks the completeness of Aβ-PET, APOEε4, cerebrospinal fluid tau or Aβ examinations, given that only ∼60% of the included population had genetic tested and Aβ-PET undertaken; and (5) finally, other related biomarkers and imaging approaches need to be investigated to gain more understanding of SCD (plus).

In summary, we characterized the SCD (plus) and unraveled that aging, shorter education period, physical labor work and lower BMI are risk factors for SCD (plus) progressing to aMCI or AD. This study may provide a reference to the inclusion criteria for the future early interventional studies and may pave the way for exploring more sensitive neuropsychological assessments for the cognitive decline in SCD (plus) individuals.

Data Availability

All datasets generated for this study are included in the manuscript and/or the supplementary files.

Ethics Statement

This study was approved by the Medical Ethics Committee of Xuanwu Hospital of Capital Medical University, Beijing, China. Written informed consent was obtained from either participants or their legally agreed surrogates.

Author Contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Key Research and Development Program of China (2016YFC1306300 and 2016YFC0103000), National Natural Science Foundation of China (Grant Nos. 61633018, 81801052, 81522021, 81430037, 81471731, and 31371007), Beijing Nature Science Foundation (7132147), Beijing Municipal Commission of Health and Family Planning (PXM2018_026283_000002).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Footnotes

  1. ^ p′ = 0.05/[k(k−1)/2 + 1] (k = 4), α′ = 0.007.

References

Aarts, S., van den Akker, M., Hajema, K. J., van Ingen, A. M., Metsemakers, J. F., Verhey, F. R., et al. (2011). Multimorbidity and its relation to subjective memory complaints in a large general population of older adults. Int. Psychogeriatr. 23, 616–624. doi: 10.1017/S1041610210002024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alzheimer’s Association, (2014). Alzheimer’s disease facts and figures. Alzheimers Dement. 10, e47–e92.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Alzheimer’s Disease International, (2016). World Alzheimer Report. Available at: https://www.alz.co.uk/research/world-report-2016

Google Scholar

Ao, J., and Liu, Y. (2004). Cognitive decline and related factors of chinese oldest old. Chin. Ment. Health J. 18, 119–122.

Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association, (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

Google Scholar

Barnes, D. E., Covinsky, K. E., Whitmer, R. A., Kuller, L. H., Lopez, O. L., Yaffe, K., et al. (2009). Predicting risk of dementia in older adults: the late-life dementia risk index. Neurology 73, 173–179. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181a81636

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumgart, M., Snyder, H. M., Carrillo, M. C., Fazio, S., Kim, H., Johns, H., et al. (2015). Summary of the evidence on modifiable risk factors for cognitive decline and dementia: a population-based perspective. Alzheimers Dement. 11, 718–726. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2015.05.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beydoun, M. A., Beydoun, H. A., Gamaldo, A. A., Teel, A., Zonderman, A. B., Wang, Y., et al. (2014). Epidemiologic studies of modifiable factors associated with cognition and dementia: systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Public Health 14:643. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-14-643

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beydoun, M. A., Beydoun, H. A., and Wang, Y. (2008). Obesity and central obesity as risk factors for incident dementia and its subtypes: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Obes. Rev. 9, 204–218. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2008.00473.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brandt, J., Aretouli, E., Neijstrom, E., Samek, J., Manning, K., Albert, M. S., et al. (2009). Selectivity of executive function deficits in mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychology 23, 607–618. doi: 10.1037/a0015851

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, S. T., Siddarth, P., Ercoli, L. M., Merrill, D. A., Torres-Gil, F., Small, G. W., et al. (2014). Modifiable risk factors for Alzheimer disease and subjective memory impairment across age groups. PLoS One 9:e98630. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0098630

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cooper, C., Sommerlad, A., Lyketsos, C. G., and Livingston, G. (2015). Modifiable predictors of dementia in mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Am. J. Psychiatry 172, 323–334. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.14070878

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deng, L. Y. Y. L. J. (2014). An analysis of cognitive status for elderly people with subjective memory complaint in seven community of Shapingba district in Chongqing city. Chin. J. Behav. Med. Brain Sci. 23, 1120–1123.

Google Scholar

Devore, E. E., Grodstein, F., van Rooij, F. J., Hofman, A., Stampfer, M. J., Witteman, J. C., et al. (2010). Dietary antioxidants and long-term risk of dementia. Arch. Neurol. 67, 819–825. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2010.144

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dubois, B., Feldman, H. H., Jacova, C., Dekosky, S. T., Barberger-Gateau, P., Cummings, J., et al. (2007). Research criteria for the diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: revising the NINCDS-ADRDA criteria. Lancet Neurol. 6, 734–746. doi: 10.1016/s1474-4422(07)70178-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Emmerzaal, T. L., Kiliaan, A. J., and Gustafson, D. R. (2015). 2003-2013: a decade of body mass index, Alzheimer’s disease, and dementia. J. Alzheimers Dis. 43, 739–755. doi: 10.3233/JAD-141086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernaeus, S. E., Ostberg, P., Wahlund, L. O., and Hellström, A. (2014). Memory factors in Rey AVLT: implications for early staging of cognitive decline. Scand. J. Psychol. 55, 546–553. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12157

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernandez-Blazquez, M. A., Avila-Villanueva, M., Maestu, F., and Medina, M. (2016). Specific features of subjective cognitive decline predict faster conversion to mild cognitive impairment. J. Alzheimers Dis. 52, 271–281. doi: 10.3233/JAD-150956

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fitzpatrick, A. L., Kuller, L. H., Lopez, O. L., Diehr, P., O’Meara, E. S., Longstreth, W. T., et al. (2009). Midlife and late-life obesity and the risk of dementia: cardiovascular health study. Arch. Neurol. 66, 336–342. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2008.582

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gannon, O. J., Robison, L. S., Custozzo, A. J., and Zuloaga, K. L. (2018). Sex differences in risk factors for vascular contributions to cognitive impairment & dementia. Neurochem. Int. 127, 38–55. doi: 10.1016/j.neuint.2018.11.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garcia-Ptacek, S., Cavallin, L., Kareholt, I., Kramberger, M. G., Winblad, B., Jelic, V., et al. (2014). Subjective cognitive impairment subjects in our clinical practice. Dement Geriatr. Cogn. Dis. Extra 4, 419–430. doi: 10.1159/000366270

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gudala, K., Bansal, D., Schifano, F., and Bhansali, A. (2013). Diabetes mellitus and risk of dementia: a meta-analysis of prospective observational studies. J. Diabetes Investig. 4, 640–650. doi: 10.1111/jdi.12087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gustafson, D. R., and Luchsinger, J. A. (2013). High adiposity: risk factor for dementia and Alzheimer’s disease? Alzheimers Res. Ther. 5:57. doi: 10.1186/alzrt221

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hachinski, V. C., Iliff, L. D., Zilhka, E., Du Boulay, G. H., McAllister, V. L., Marshall, J., et al. (1975). Cerebral blood flow in dementia. Arch. Neurol. 32, 632–637.

Google Scholar

Hamilton, M. (1980). Rating depressive patients. J. Clin. Psychiatry 41(12 Pt 2), 21–24.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Hao, L., Wang, X., Zhang, L., Xing, Y., Guo, Q., Hu, X., et al. (2017). Prevalence, risk factors, and complaints screening tool exploration of subjective cognitive decline in a large cohort of the chinese population. J. Alzheimers Dis. 60, 371–388. doi: 10.3233/JAD-170347

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hebert, L. E., Bienias, J. L., Aggarwal, N. T., Wilson, R. S., Bennett, D. A., Shah, R. C., et al. (2010). Change in risk of Alzheimer disease over time. Neurology 75, 786–791. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181f0754f

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Helzner, E. P., Luchsinger, J. A., Scarmeas, N., Cosentino, S., Brickman, A. M., Glymour, M. M., et al. (2009). Contribution of vascular risk factors to the progression in Alzheimer disease. Arch. Neurol. 66, 343–348. doi: 10.1001/archneur.66.3.343

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Honig, L. S., Tang, M. X., Albert, S., Costa, R., Luchsinger, J., Manly, J., et al. (2003). Stroke and the risk of Alzheimer disease. Arch. Neurol. 60, 1707–1712.

Google Scholar

Huang, L., Chen, K. L., Lin, B. Y., Tang, L., Zhao, Q. H., Lv, Y. R., et al. (2018). Chinese version of montreal cognitive assessment basic for discrimination among different severities of Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 14, 2133–2140. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S174293

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iliffe, S., and Pealing, L. (2010). Subjective memory problems. BMJ 340:c1425. doi: 10.1136/bmj.c1425

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jefferson, A. L., Gibbons, L. E., Rentz, D. M., Carvalho, J. O., Manly, J., Bennett, D. A., et al. (2011). A life course model of cognitive activities, socioeconomic status, education, reading ability, and cognition. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 59, 1403–1411. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2011.03499.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jessen, F., Amariglio, R. E., van Boxtel, M., Breteler, M., Ceccaldi, M., Chételat, G., et al. (2014). A conceptual framework for research on subjective cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 10, 844–852. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2014.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jonker, C., Geerlings, M. I., and Schmand, B. (2000). Are memory complaints predictive for dementia? A review of clinical and population-based studies. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 15, 983–991. doi: 10.1002/1099-1166(200011)15:11<983::aid-gps238>3.0.co;2-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Katzman, R., Zhang, M. Y., Ouang, Y. Q., Wang, Z. Y., Liu, W. T., Yu, E., et al. (1988). A Chinese version of the mini-mental state examination; impact of illiteracy in a Shanghai dementia survey. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 41, 971–978.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Kielb, S., Rogalski, E., Weintraub, S., and Rademaker, A. (2017). Objective features of subjective cognitive decline in a United States national database. Alzheimers Dement. 13, 1337–1344. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2017.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klekociuk, S. Z., Summers, J. J., Vickers, J. C., and Summers, M. J. (2014). Reducing false positive diagnoses in mild cognitive impairment: the importance of comprehensive neuropsychological assessment. Eur. J. Neurol. 21, e82–e83. doi: 10.1111/ene.12488

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koppara, A., Frommann, I., Polcher, A., Parra, M. A., Maier, W., Jessen, F., et al. (2015). Feature binding deficits in subjective cognitive decline and in mild cognitive impairment. J. Alzheimers Dis. 48(Suppl. 1), S161–S170. doi: 10.3233/JAD-150105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, Y., Back, J. H., Kim, J., Na, D. L., Cheong, H. K., Hong, C. H., et al. (2010). Systematic review of health behavioral risks and cognitive health in older adults. Int. Psychogeriatr. 22, 174–187. doi: 10.1017/S1041610209991189

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liang, X., Chen, Z., Dong, X., Zhao, Q., Guo, Q., Zheng, L., et al. (2018). Mental work demands and late-life cognitive impairment: results from the shanghai aging study. J. Aging Health 31, 883–898. doi: 10.1177/0898264318765034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Littlejohns, T. J., Henley, W. E., Lang, I. A., Annweiler, C., Beauchet, O., Chaves, P. H., et al. (2014). Vitamin D and the risk of dementia and Alzheimer disease. Neurology 83, 920–928.

Google Scholar

Loef, M., and Walach, H. (2013). Midlife obesity and dementia: meta-analysis and adjusted forecast of dementia prevalence in the United States and China. Obesity 21, E51–E55. doi: 10.1002/oby.20037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lu, J., Li, D., Li, F., Wang, F., Zuo, X., Jia, X. F., et al. (2011). Montreal cognitive assessment in detecting cognitive impairment in Chinese elderly individuals: a population-based study. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry Neurol. 24, 184–190. doi: 10.1177/0891988711422528

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luchsinger, J. A., and Gustafson, D. R. (2009). Adiposity, type 2 diabetes, and Alzheimer’s disease. J. Alzheimers Dis. 16, 693–704.

Google Scholar

Maj, M., Satz, P., Janssen, R., Zaudig, M., Starace, F., D’Elia, L., et al. (1994). WHO neuropsychiatric AIDS study, cross-sectional phase II. Neuropsychological and neurological findings. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 51, 51–61.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

McKhann, G., Drachman, D., Folstein, M., Katzman, R., Price, D., Stadlan, E. M., et al. (1984). Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA work group under the auspices of department of health and human services task force on Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 34, 939–944.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Meng, X., and D’Arcy, C. (2012). Education and dementia in the context of the cognitive reserve hypothesis: a systematic review with meta-analyses and qualitative analyses. PLoS One 7:e38268. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0038268

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mohs, R. C., Doody, R. S., Morris, J. C., Ieni, J. R., Rogers, S. L., Perdomo, C. A. et al., (2001). A 1-year, placebo-controlled preservation of function survival study of donepezil in AD patients. Neurology 57, 481–488.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Morris, J. C. (1993). The clinical dementia rating (CDR). : current version and scoring rules. Neurology 43, 2412–2414.

Google Scholar

Ogawa, Y., Kaneko, Y., Sato, T., Shimizu, S., Kanetaka, H., Hanyu, H., et al. (2018). Sarcopenia and muscle functions at various stages of Alzheimer Disease. Front. Neurol. 9:710. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2018.00710

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paradise, M. B., Glozier, N. S., Naismith, S. L., Davenport, T. A., and Hickie, I. B. (2011). Subjective memory complaints, vascular risk factors and psychological distress in the middle-aged: a cross-sectional study. BMC Psychiatry 11:108. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-11-108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, J., Jeong, E., and Seomun, G. (2018). The clock drawing test: a systematic review and meta-analysis of diagnostic accuracy. J. Adv. Nurs. 74, 2742–2754. doi: 10.1111/jan.13810

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, R. C. (2004). Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. J. Intern. Med. 256, 183–194. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01388.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Profenno, L. A., Porsteinsson, A. P., and Faraone, S. V. (2010). Meta-analysis of Alzheimer’s disease risk with obesity, diabetes, and related disorders. Biol. Psychiatry 67, 505–512. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.02.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Qizilbash, N., Gregson, J., Johnson, M. E., Pearce, N., Douglas, I., Wing, K., et al. (2015). BMI and risk of dementia in two million people over two decades: a retrospective cohort study. Lancet Diabetes Endocrinol. 3, 431–436. doi: 10.1016/S2213-8587(15)00033-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rabin, L. A., Smart, C. M., and Amariglio, R. E. (2017). Subjective cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 13, 369–396. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032816-045136

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rabin, L. A., Smart, C. M., Crane, P. K., Amariglio, R. E., Berman, L. M., Boada, M., et al. (2015). Subjective cognitive decline in older adults: an overview of self-report measures used across 19 international research studies. J. Alzheimers Dis. 48(Suppl. 1), S63–S86. doi: 10.3233/JAD-150154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rijs, K. J., Van den Kommer, T. N., Comijs, H. C., and Deeg, D. J. (2015). Prevalence and incidence of memory complaints in employed compared to non-employed aged 55-64 years and the role of employment characteristics. PLoS One 10:e119192. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0119192

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, R. O., Knopman, D. S., Cha, R. H., Mielke, M. M., Pankratz, V. S., Boeve, B. F., et al. (2014). Diabetes and elevated hemoglobin A1c levels are associated with brain hypometabolism but not amyloid accumulation. J. Nucl. Med. 55, 759–764. doi: 10.2967/jnumed.113.132647

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sando, S. B., Melquist, S., Cannon, A., Hutton, M., Sletvold, O., Saltvedt, I., et al. (2008). Risk-reducing effect of education in Alzheimer’s disease. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 23, 1156–1162. doi: 10.1002/gps.2043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saunders, N. L., and Summers, M. J. (2011). Longitudinal deficits to attention, executive, and working memory in subtypes of mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychology 25, 237–248. doi: 10.1037/a0021134

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sellbom, K. S., and Gunstad, J. (2012). Cognitive function and decline in obesity. J. Alzheimers Dis. 30(Suppl. 2), S89–S95. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2011-111073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smart, C. M., and Krawitz, A. (2015). The impact of subjective cognitive decline on iowa gambling task performance. Neuropsychology 29, 971–987. doi: 10.1037/neu0000204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sperling, R. A., Jack, C. J., and Aisen, P. S. (2011). Testing the right target and right drug at the right stage. Sci. Transl. Med. 3, 111c–133c. doi: 10.1126/scitranslmed.3002609

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Storandt, M., Grant, E. A., Miller, J. P., and Morris, J. C. (2006). Longitudinal course and neuropathologic outcomes in original vs revised MCI and in pre-MCI. Neurology 67, 467–473. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000228231.26111.6e

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sugimoto, T., Ono, R., Murata, S., Saji, N., Matsui, Y., Niida, S., et al. (2016). Prevalence and associated factors of sarcopenia in elderly subjects with amnestic mild cognitive impairment or Alzheimer disease. Curr. Alzheimers Res. 13, 718–726. doi: 10.2174/1567205013666160211124828

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Summers, M. J., and Saunders, N. L. (2012). Neuropsychological measures predict decline to Alzheimer’s dementia from mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychology 26, 498–508. doi: 10.1037/a0028576

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tang, Y. Z. M. (1984). Hamilton anxiety scale (HAMA). Shanghai Arch. Psychiatry 12–13.

Google Scholar

Vogel, A., Salem, L. C., Andersen, B. B., and Waldemar, G. (2016). Differences in quantitative methods for measuring subjective cognitive decline – results from a prospective memory clinic study. Int. Psychogeriatr. 28, 1513–1520. doi: 10.1017/S1041610216000272

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vyhnalek, M., Nikolai, T., Andel, R., Nedelska, Z., Rubínová, E., Marková, H., et al. (2014). Neuropsychological correlates of hippocampal atrophy in memory testing in nondemented older adults. J. Alzheimers Dis. 42(Suppl. 3), S81–S90. doi: 10.3233/JAD-132642

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vyhnalek, M., Rubinova, E., Markova, H., Nikolai, T., Laczó, J., Andel, R., et al. (2017). Clock drawing test in screening for Alzheimer’s dementia and mild cognitive impairment in clinical practice. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 32, 933–939. doi: 10.1002/gps.4549

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Z., Qiao, K., Chen, G., Sui, D., Dong, H. M., Wang, Y. S., et al. (2019). Functional connectivity changes across the spectrum of subjective cognitive decline, amnestic mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Front. Neuroinform. 13:26. doi: 10.3389/fninf.2019.00026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, J. W., Plassman, B. L., Burke, J., and Benjamin, S. (2010). Preventing Alzheimer’s disease and cognitive decline. Evid. Rep. Technol. Assess. 1–727.

Google Scholar

Xu, W. L., Atti, A. R., Gatz, M., Pedersen, N. L., Johansson, B., Fratiglioni, L., et al. (2011). Midlife overweight and obesity increase late-life dementia risk: a population-based twin study. Neurology 76, 1568–1574. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182190d09

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, Y., Chen, K., Zhao, Q., Li, F., and Guo, Q. (2018). Short-term delayed recall of auditory verbal learning test provides equivalent value to long-term delayed recall in predicting MCI clinical outcomes: a longitudinal follow-up study. Appl. Neuropsychol. Adult [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Yang, Y., Xiao, L. D., Deng, L., Wang, Y., Li, M., Ullah, S., et al. (2015). Nurse-led cognitive screening model for older adults in primary care. Geriatr. Gerontol. Int. 15, 721–728. doi: 10.1111/ggi.12339

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao, Q., Guo, Q., Liang, X., Chen, M., Zhou, Y., Ding, D., et al. (2015). Auditory verbal learning test is superior to rey-osterrieth complex figure memory for predicting mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer’s disease. Curr. Alzheimer Res. 12, 520–526. doi: 10.2174/1567205012666150530202729

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao, Q., Lv, Y., Zhou, Y., Hong, Z., and Guo, Q. (2012). Short-term delayed recall of auditory verbal learning test is equivalent to long-term delayed recall for identifying amnestic mild cognitive impairment. PLoS One 7:e51157. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0051157

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhong, G., Wang, Y., Zhang, Y., Guo, J. J., and Zhao, Y. (2015). Smoking is associated with an increased risk of dementia: a meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies with investigation of potential effect modifiers. PLoS One 10:e118333. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0118333

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Appendix

Pre-MCI SCD

It is defined as (1) the objective cognitive performance of the individuals to be within the normal range; (2) decline of the self-perceived cognitive abilities; (3) these subtle cognitive declines should not reach the diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) or dementia; and (4) this should not be caused by any psychiatric disorders or neurological and medical conditions.

SCD (plus)

In addition to meeting the diagnosis of pre-MCI SCD, SCD (plus) also need to meet the following criteria: onset age ≥60 years; complaints about SCD within the past 5 years; the complainers feel their performance are not as good as their peers and concerning associated with SCD; a confirmed cognitive decline by the informants; complains were only limited memory problems rather than other cognitive domains; and presence of the APOE ε4 genotype and biomarker evidence for a potential progression to AD.

Keywords: Alzheimer Disease, subjective cognitive decline, risk factors, neuropsychological assessment, objective cognitive features

Citation: Hao L, Xing Y, Li X, Mu B, Zhao W, Wang G, Wang T, Jia J and Han Y (2019) Risk Factors and Neuropsychological Assessments of Subjective Cognitive Decline (plus) in Chinese Memory Clinic. Front. Neurosci. 13:846. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00846

Received: 17 March 2019; Accepted: 30 July 2019;
Published: 14 August 2019.

Edited by:

Hyun Kook Lim, The Catholic University of Korea, South Korea

Reviewed by:

Jiu Chen, Nanjing Medical University, China
Sheng-Min Wang, The Catholic University of Korea, South Korea

Copyright © 2019 Hao, Xing, Li, Mu, Zhao, Wang, Wang, Jia and Han. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jianguo Jia, amlhamlhbmd1b18xQDEyNi5jb20=; Ying Han, aGFueWluZ0B4d2guY2NtdS5lZHUuY24=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.