Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 11 July 2019
Sec. Perception Science

Identifying Cortical Substrates Underlying the Phenomenology of Stereopsis and Realness: A Pilot fMRI Study

  • 1School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of St Andrews, St Andrews, United Kingdom
  • 2Institute of Psychology, University of Regensburg, Regensburg, Germany
  • 3TAyside Medical Science Centre (TASC), NHS Tayside, Dundee, United Kingdom

Viewing a real scene or a stereoscopic image (e.g., 3D movies) with both eyes yields a vivid subjective impression of object solidity, tangibility, immersive negative space and sense of realness; something that is not experienced when viewing single pictures of 3D scenes normally with both eyes. This phenomenology, sometimes referred to as stereopsis, is conventionally ascribed to the derivation of depth from the differences in the two eye’s images (binocular disparity). Here we report on a pilot study designed to explore if dissociable neural activity associated with the phenomenology of realness can be localized in the cortex. In order to dissociate subjective impression from disparity processing, we capitalized on the finding that the impression of realness associated with stereoscopic viewing can also be generated when viewing a single picture of a 3D scene with one eye through an aperture. Under a blocked fMRI design, subjects viewed intact and scrambled images of natural 3-D objects, and scenes under three viewing conditions: (1) single pictures viewed normally with both eyes (binocular); (2) single pictures viewed with one eye through an aperture (monocular-aperture); and (3) stereoscopic anaglyph images of the same scenes viewed with both eyes (binocular stereopsis). Fixed-effects GLM contrasts aimed at isolating the phenomenology of stereopsis demonstrated a selective recruitment of similar posterior parietal regions for both monocular and binocular stereopsis conditions. Our findings provide preliminary evidence that the cortical processing underlying the subjective impression of realness may be dissociable and distinct from the derivation of depth from disparity.

Introduction

A fundamental insight of early enquiries into visual space perception was that only certain types of visual stimulation give rise to a perceptual state in which space and objects appear visually “real” and in vivid “depth relief.” Renaissance artists correctly observed that viewing realistically rendered and optically correct perspective images (paintings) of 3D scenes produced an accurate impression of 3-dimensionality, but that they lacked the phenomenological sense of object solidity and realness that is obtained in natural scenes (Wade et al., 2001). Following Wheatstone’s (1838) discovery of the stereoscope, contemporary understanding of this phenomenology (often referred to as stereopsis) is that it is a by-product of the derivation of depth from binocular disparities, which are the small differences between the images of the two eyes (e.g., Westheimer, 2011). In contrast to pictorial images, stereoscopic images (e.g., 3D movies) generate visual impressions of object solidity, object tangibility (the feeling that you can reach out to touch objects), and empty (negative) space between objects. Together, these impressions give the depicted objects and space a phenomenological “realness.” Motion parallax also produces a similar phenomenology which is ascribed to the fact that parallax and disparity, as sensory cues, are computationally similar (Helmholtz, 1909).

However, since the Renaissance, it has been widely observed and reported that this same phenomenology of solidity, tangibility, negative space, and realness can be obtained under conditions in which neither binocular disparity nor motion parallax are present (Claparède, 1904; Ames, 1925; Schlosberg, 1941; Michotte, 1948; Koenderink et al., 1994; Koenderink, 1998; Vishwanath and Hibbard, 2013; da Vinci, cited in Wheatstone, 1838; Wade et al., 2001; Wijntjes, 2017); an observation that has been recently confirmed empirically in naïve observers, specifically when viewing single images monocularly through an aperture (Vishwanath and Hibbard, 2013). This suggests that the visual phenomenology of stereopsis is not simply tied to binocular disparity or parallax processing but derives from a more general visual property or process. While there have been several decades of detailed investigation of the neural substrates underlying the processing of binocular disparities into depth perception (see reviews, Gonzalez and Perez, 1998; Sakata et al., 2005; Parker, 2007; Orban, 2011; Welchman, 2016), there has been no systematic investigation into the neural substrates that underlie the subjective phenomenology associated with stereopsis and visual realness. In this study, we provide an initial exploratory investigation to determine if cortical processes underlying the subjective visual phenomenology associated with real and stereoscopic 3D scenes (stereopsis) can be dissociated, localized, and distinguished from disparity processing.

A large amount of work in neurophysiology has focussed on the derivation of depth and 3D structure from binocular disparity, initially in the cat and monkey, but more widely in humans in the last two decades. This work has identified substrates for initial processing of disparities, differential coding of absolute and relative disparities, differential processing of zeroth-, first- and second-order disparity relations, derivation of 3D shape from disparities, and integration of disparity with other depth cues (Barlow et al., 1967; Poggio et al., 1985; Ohzawa et al., 1990; Taira et al., 2000; Backus et al., 2001; Cumming, 2002; Tsao et al., 2003; Neri et al., 2004; Preston et al., 2008; Durand et al., 2009; Georgieva et al., 2009; Verhoef et al., 2011; Ban and Welchman, 2015; Goncalves et al., 2015). These studies have shown that areas in both ventral and dorsal streams of visual processing are activated during the perception of 3D structure from disparity, but that different information is processed in each stream. While dorsal regions of the extrastriate cortex (V3A, V3B/KO, and MT) and V7 have been implicated in disparity defined depth and the integration of different cues to derive the 3D structure of viewed surfaces (Backus et al., 2001; Naganuma et al., 2005; Bridge and Parker, 2007; Preston et al., 2008; Durand et al., 2009; Minini et al., 2010; Cottereau et al., 2011; Goncalves et al., 2015), ventral regions of the extrastriate cortex (i.e., V3v, V4 and LOC, PPA) have been implicated in encoding of 3D scenes, object configurations and features required for recognition and categorization (Kourtzi and Kanwisher, 2001; Gilaie-Dotan et al., 2002; Bridge and Parker, 2007; Chandrasekaran et al., 2007). Neurophysiological studies of depth processing from other cues (motion, texture, and shading) are sparser, but those that exist again implicate many of the same regions involved in disparity derived depth (e.g., Orban et al., 1999; Shikata et al., 2001; Taira et al., 2001; Murray et al., 2003; Georgieva et al., 2008).

Given the accumulating evidence that points to the processing of disparity and 3D structure in both ventral and dorsal visual pathways, an obvious question is whether the phenomenology of stereopsis arises in one or both visual streams, or elsewhere in the cortex. Based on the most influential theory of cortical visual processing (the two visual systems hypothesis, Goodale and Milner, 1992; Milner and Goodale, 2008), the most likely candidate for the locus of stereopsis within the visual stream is the ventral pathway, since it is implicated in conscious visual perception and recognition of objects and scenes (Kourtzi and Kanwisher, 2001; Gilaie-Dotan et al., 2002; Neri et al., 2004; Bridge and Parker, 2007; Chandrasekaran et al., 2007) while the dorsal visual pathway is generally implicated in subconscious representations guiding visuo-motor control (see reviews Tunik et al., 2007; Anzai and DeAngelis, 2010; Gallivan and Culham, 2015; Freud et al., 2016; Goodale and Milner, 2018). However, other theoretical proposals do suggest a role for the dorsal stream (Michotte, 1948; Hibbard, 2008a; Vishwanath, 2014). For example, Michotte (1948) proposed that the phenomenology of stereopsis and realness is linked to the conscious visual awareness of the capacity to manipulate objects, implying that it arises from 3D encodings that are built up to guide manual action rather than to recognize objects. This could implicate areas of the parietal cortex involved in visuo-motor control (intraparietal sulcus) or perhaps even other areas involved in visuo-motor planning such as the premotor (Binkofski et al., 1999; Schubotz and Von Cramon, 2002; Hanakawa et al., 2008) or prefrontal cortex (see reviews Kravitz et al., 2011; Gallivan and Culham, 2015; Freud et al., 2016; Goodale and Milner, 2018).

No prior research to the best of our knowledge has specifically aimed to identify the substrate for the phenomenological experience of stereopsis or realness, and it is difficult to extrapolate from previous studies. The stimuli employed in most neurophysiological studies in 3D vision, where stereopsis was putatively experienced, have traditionally employed random dot (or random line) stereograms (Julesz, 1971). These types of stimuli generate an impression of 3D structure only when disparity is present and a 2D planar structure when disparity is absent (see for example: Human: Backus et al., 2001; Neri et al., 2004; Preston et al., 2008; Ban and Welchman, 2015; Goncalves et al., 2015; Monkey: Taira et al., 2000; Cumming, 2002; Tsao et al., 2003; Verhoef et al., 2010). This complicates the ability to distinguish between neural processes underlying disparity processing and representations for discriminating depth structure or guiding movement, from those that give rise to a specific subjective experience of 3D. Interestingly, two recent studies have examined differences in cortical processing during viewing of real objects and pictures used as physical stimuli in fMRI paradigms aimed at identifying neural processes underlying object representation (Snow et al., 2011) and visually guided grasping/reaching (Freud et al., 2018). Both the parietal cortex (intraparietal sulcus) and the lateral occipital cortex were implicated in these studies (Snow et al., 2011; Freud et al., 2018) as potential neural substrates encoding differences between real and pictured stimuli, although in these studies, the difference in the cortical processing might potentially reflect differences in binocular disparity process present in the real objects as compared to the pictured objects, in addition to differences in perceived depth or realness.

In the present study, we aimed to explore the idea that the visual phenomenology of stereopsis and realness is linked to dissociable neural processes underlying 3D vision. Furthermore, we aimed to determine if this processing is distinct from activity specifically related to disparity processes. To this end, we capitalized on the fact that we could induce the phenomenology of stereopsis both in the presence of binocular disparity (binocular viewing of stereoscopic images) and in its absence (monocular-aperture viewing of single pictures). We designed stimulus and viewing conditions that were aimed at providing fMRI BOLD contrasts that would isolate potential distinctive visual processing associated with the phenomenology of realness and stereopsis under whole-brain analysis. We did not aim to establish the functional significance of any such dissociable activity (e.g., visuo-motor representations and object recognition) but simply to determine if conditions in which stereopsis is experienced has differential neural activation when other factors are controlled for.

Materials and Methods

A 3T Siemens Trio MRI scanner with a body transmit and 32-channel receiver-array head coil was used for MR data acquisition. All data acquisition on humans was performed with approval from the relevant ethics committees (NHS Tayside and University of St Andrews). This study was reviewed and approved by the University Teaching and Research Ethics Committee at the University of St Andrews, and NHS Tayside. All procedures were conducted in accordance with the ethical guidelines of University of St Andrews, NHS Tayside, and the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki. Informed consent was obtained from all participants involved in this project. Seven right-handed participants (5 males, 2 females, age = 23.8 ± 7.5) took part in the study. Participants were pre-screened to confirm normal visual acuity (20/30; Snellen chart), stereovision (Randot Test) and capacity to perceive monocular stereopsis with their right eye (abbreviated questionnaire from Vishwanath and Hibbard, 2013). Although there are other stereovision tests that check for disparity processing in the absence of eye movements (van Ee and Richards, 2002; Brouwer et al., 2009), we used a standard Randot test which does not restrict participants from making eye movements during the test.

Data Acquisition

fMRI data were acquired using a GE-EPI sequence (TR = 2500 ms, TE = 30 ms, 37 slices, voxel resolution = 3 mm × 3 mm ×3 mm, slice gap = 0.4 mm, FOV = 190 mm× 190 mm, flip angle = 90°, 141 volumes). Foam padding was placed around the participant’s head to reduce motion-related artifacts. A T1-weighted anatomical image (MPRAGE sequence: TR = 1900 ms, TE = 2.64 ms, 176 slices per slab, voxel resolution = 0.8 mm × 0.8 mm × 1 mm, GRAPPA acceleration factor = 2, FOV = 200x200 mm, 9° flip angle) was also acquired.

Stimulus Display and Response Acquisition

Stimuli were back projected on to a screen that was built into a custom frame manufactured to fit securely into the back of the magnet bore. The screen was a Stewart Filmscreen Starglass 60 back projection screen. The screen display area was 47 cm (W) x 37 cm (H) at the widest dimensions. The projector was a Sanyo ET 30L with a Sanyo TNS-T11 long-throw lens running at a resolution of 1280 × 960 pixels and refresh rate of 85 Hz. It was located in a shielded room and the image back projected onto the screen through a wave guide. The image was viewed via a mirror (Siemens) that was attached to the head coil in front of the participants face when they lay supine on the scanner bed. The total distance to the screen from the subject’s eyes via the mirror (i.e., viewing distance) was approximately 80cm.

Three viewing conditions were tested: monocular-aperture pictorial (MaP), binocular pictorial (BP), and stereoscopic anaglyph (SA); see below for details. In the BP condition, the eye opening of the head coil (5 cm × 5cm) were both left open without any insert. For the other two conditions, custom eyepieces were constructed (plastic and cardboard) that were inserted into the head coil eye openings. For the MaP condition, a black eyepiece with a 7.5 mm circular viewing aperture (adjustable position) was inserted into the right eye opening of the head coil, while a completely occluding black cover was inserted into the left eye opening. This left eye cover was placed over cotton and gauze that applied gentle pressure to keep the eyelid of the left eye shut. For each individual subject, before the start of the scanning, the position of the aperture in the eyepiece was adjusted so that the displayed image could be seen with the right eye but the rectangular edges of the image were occluded. For the SA condition, both eye openings in the head coil inserted with a cover that had a 2 cm × 3 cm rectangular opening with thin transparent plastic filters (cyan for the right eye and red for the left eye). In the BP and SA conditions, the full displayed image and screen could be seen, as well as parts of the magnet bore in the periphery. Responses (right-hand thumb press) were acquired using a Nordic Neurolabs thumb trigger response device.

Stimuli

Stimuli (Figure 1) consisted of grayscale images of natural 3D scenes or objects that were acquired via a stereoscopic camera rig with inter-camera separation of 65 mm (Hibbard, 2008b). Image resolution was 800 × 800 pixels. There were three types of images: (1) a standard photographic (no-disparity) image, (2) the same images in a stereoscopic (red-cyan) anaglyphic format, (3) scrambled versions of the pictorial images (50 × 50 cells). The scrambled images were constructed by randomly repositioning the 16x16 pixels cells created by partitioning the image with a 50 × 50 grid. The aim was to simply eliminate any cues to object structure or object recognition but maintain the same overall luminance since overall pixel values in the scrambled and original image remain the same. The size of the projected images on the screen was 30cm x 30cm.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. (A) Sample of six of the 12 B&W photographic images used in the study. (B) Example of the three versions of each image that were used in the experiment. The leftmost image is the pictorial (no-disparity) version of the image (left-camera view only). The middle image is the stereoscopic (disparity) version of the image combining left and right camera views. The rightmost image is the scrambled version of the pictorial image (50 × 50 cells).

Viewing Conditions

Participants viewed visual stimuli under three types of viewing conditions, which were each tested on separate runs, with bed retractions as necessary between viewing conditions to change the eyepieces. The three viewing conditions were BP: viewing a pictorial (no disparity) image or a scrambled version naturally through both eyes without visual restriction. Monocular-aperture Pictorial: viewing a pictorial (no disparity) image or a scrambled version with the right eye only through a circular aperture that occluded the rectangular edges of the images; SA: viewing either a stereoscopic image (disparity) or pictorial version of the same image (no disparity) through red-cyan anaglyph filters.

fMRI Design

We used a blocked fMRI design (see Figure 2). Each block began with a pre-stimulus fixation phase of 7.5 s, followed by the sequential display of 10 images in random order (2000 ms per image) for a total of 20 s. For the MaP and BP conditions, there were 12 blocks per run alternating between intact pictorial images (6 blocks) and scrambled images (6 blocks). For the SA condition, there were also 12 blocks per run, alternating between stereoscopic images (disparity, 6 Blocks) and pictorial images (no disparity, 6 blocks). The start of each run was preceded by a 10 s fixation phase. For each viewing condition, we acquired data for two consecutive 12-block runs, and the bed was retracted partially in order to change the eyepieces for a subsequent viewing condition. The order of acquisition for the three types of runs (MaP, BP, SA) were partially counterbalanced across participants. The anatomical image was acquired in between two of different viewing condition runs so as to provide a period of task-free rest.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. fMRI design. (A) The upper panel shows the stimulus sequence for the binocular pictorial (BP) and monocular-aperture pictorial (MaP) conditions, while the lower panel shows the sequence for the stereoscopic anaglyph (SA) condition. (B) An example sequence of the functional runs and anatomical scans for one participant. Two runs for each viewing condition were done consecutively with a brief break in between. The order of the viewing conditions and the anatomical scan was partially counterbalanced across participants. The blue arrow represents partial bed retractions to change eyepieces.

Visual Fixation Task

In order to minimize eye movements and maintain equivalent attention across all viewing conditions and stimulus types, participants were required to do the same visual detection task at central fixation for all runs while passively viewing the displayed images. Participants were instructed to keep their eyes on the fixation dot (a 4 mm × 4 mm square), monitor its luminance and press the response button as quickly as possible (right hand thumb trigger) whenever it turned from black to gray. The luminance change occurred at random intervals during the stimulus blocks. Specifically, the luminance change occurred in 50% of randomly selected individual stimulus trials within a block, 1000 ms after stimulus onset. Participants were instructed to remain as still as possible during the entire duration of the experiment and also during bed-retractions.

Analysis

fMRI data were processed using FSL v5.0.91. Data from each participant were motion-corrected (MCFLIRT), spatially smoothed (5 mm FWHM Gaussian kernel), high-pass temporally filtered (100 s cutoff), registered to their T1 anatomical brain image (FLIRT), and normalized to the MNI 2 mm standard brain. GLM analyses were performed using FEAT v6.0. First-level analysis was performed employing two regressors (time-locked to the onset of each 20 s block): (1) intact stimuli, (2) scrambled stimuli for the MaP and BP; and (1) disparity stimuli, (2) non-disparity stimuli for the SA. All regressors were convolved with a double-gamma HRF.

For the MaP and BP conditions, we computed the first-level contrasts (intact > scrambled stimuli), whereas for the SA condition, we computed the first-level contrast (disparity > non-disparity stimuli). For each participant, the first-level results were combined across the two runs of each viewing condition using a second-level, fixed effects analysis to calculate an average response per subject for each viewing condition. To determine areas recruited during the processing of monocular stereopsis, we computed the contrast [MaPINTACT > MaPSCRAMB] > [BPINTACT > BPSCRAMB] at the third-level, fixed effects analysis per participant. These second and third level results were then combined across all participants at the fourth, group-level using a FLAME fixed-effects analysis (Woolrich et al., 2004) for each viewing condition and monocular stereopsis contrast. The critical contrasts were the fourth-level fixed effects results for the monocular stereopsis [MaPINTACT > MaPSCRAMB] > [BPINTACT > BPSCRAMB], and the fourth-level fixed effects results for the binocular stereopsis [SADISP > SANO-DISP]. Since we did not expect to find any results related to the processing of stereopsis outside gray matter based on our a-priori hypothesis, we applied a mask of gray matter (FSL FAST, see text footnote 1) (Zhang et al., 2001) as pre-threshold mask to all group-level statistical maps (Eickhoff et al., 2009; Uji et al., 2018). Main effect (boxcar model of the task) BOLD Z-statistic images were thresholded at uncorrected significance level of p < 0.001 (Woo et al., 2014; Eklund et al., 2016).

We also conducted a conjunction analysis (Friston et al., 2005; Nichols et al., 2005) to obtain the commonly activating brain regions between the effects of monocular and binocular stereopsis. The two Z-statistic images from each effect were tested against the conjunction null hypothesis at uncorrected significance level of p < 0.001.

Due to the limitations of voxelwise analysis and increased risk of Type I error using uncorrected (p < 0.001) thresholds for multiple comparisons, we applied the false discovery rate (FDR) measure (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995) using the FDR analysis tool supplied by the FSL software (Jenkinson et al., 2012; Nichols, 2012). FDR represents the expected proportion of rejected hypotheses that are false positives. To correct for the multiple comparisons, the two p-maps from the binocular and monocular stereopsis contrasts were further thresholded to a FDR of 5%.

Results

All participants performed the visual detection task at fixation as instructed showing a group mean accuracy of 98.8 ± 0.3% (± standard error [SE]) for the MaP condition, 98.8 ± 0.4% for the BP condition, and 94.6 ± 3.8% of the trials for the SA condition. Accuracy did not differ significantly between viewing conditions, F(2,12) = 1.34, p = 0.30.

We first examined areas revealed by the contrasts [MaPINTACT > MaPSCRAMB] and [BPINTACT > BPSCRAMB]. These contrasts aimed to identify activations associated with perception of objects, scenes and 3D structure under each of the two viewing conditions (e.g., Epstein and Kanwisher, 1998; Kourtzi and Kanwisher, 2001). Both contrasts revealed similar statistical maps (z-score > 3.1, p < 0.001, fixed effects) that inclu- ded regions corresponding to both dorsal and ventral aspects of higher occipital areas, parietal cortex and posterior aspects of the cingulate cortex (Figures 3, 4). Peak responses for the monocular aperture condition were found in the left parietal cortex [peak voxel: Z = 11.2, p < 1 × 10−16, MNI coordinates (x, y, z): −32, −72, 38], in the right parietal cortex [peak voxel: Z = 7.99, p < 1 × 10−15, MNI coordinates: 20, −82, 42], and in the left and right posterior cingulate [peak voxels: Z = 6.63, p < 1 × 10−10 and Z = 6.44, p < 1 × 10−10, MNI coordinates: −26, −56, 10, and 28, −58, 4], respectively. Peak responses for the BP condition were found in the left parietal cortex [peak voxel: Z = 6.34, p < 1 × 10−9, MNI coordinates: −6, −80, 44], in the right parietal cortex [peak voxels: Z = 7.05, p < 1 × 10−12 and Z = 5.47, p < 1 × 10−7, MNI coordinates: 44, −76, 20 and 46, −54, 46], respectively, in the left posterior cingulate [peak voxels: Z = 6.76, p < 1 × 10−11 and Z = 4.49, p < 1 × 10−5, MNI coordinates: −26, −54, 4 and −4, −34, 24], respectively, in the right posterior cingulate [peak voxel: Z = 6.61, p < 1 × 10−10, MNI coordinates: 28, −52, 6], and in the left lateral occipital cortex [peak voxel: Z = 6.43, p < 1 × 10−10, MNI coordinates: −38, −60, −6].

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Group results for monocular-aperture viewing, revealed by the fixed-effects (N = 7) GLM contrast [MaPINTACT > MaPSCRAMB images]. All responses are masked to gray matter. Uncorrected p < 0.001.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Group results for binocular viewing, revealed by the fixed-effects (N = 7) GLM contrast [BPINTACT > BPSCRAMB images]. All responses are masked to gray matter. Uncorrected p < 0.001.

To identify areas contributing to the impression of monocular stereopsis, we examined the contrast ([MaPINTACT > MaPSCRAMB] > [BPINTACT > BPSCRAMB]). This contrast revealed a cluster in the left parietal cortex [peak voxel: Z = 5.30, p < 1 × 10−7, MNI coordinates: −28, −60, 54] and in the right parietal cortex [peak voxels: Z = 5.28, p < 1 × 10−7 and Z = 4.01, p < 1 × 10−4, MNI coordinates: 36, −74, 50 and 20, −64, 50] (Figure 5).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Group results for monocular stereopsis, revealed by the fixed-effects (N = 7) GLM contrast [MaPINTACT > MaPSCRAMB] > [BPINTACT> BPSCRAMB]. All responses are masked to gray matter. Uncorrected p < 0.001. The crosshairs represent the peak in the left parietal cortex [MNI coordinates: −28, −60, and 54].

To identify areas that are involved both in the processing of disparity and binocular stereopsis, we used the contrast [SADISP > SANO-DISP]. This contrast revealed a cluster in the left parietal cortex [peak voxels: Z = 3.87, p < 1 × 10−4 and Z = 4.18, p < 1 × 10−4, MNI coordinates: −28, −74, 46 and −30, −44, 50], in the left lateral occipital cortex [peak voxel: Z = 3.66, p < 0.001, MNI coordinates: −26, −84, 18], and in the right lateral occipital cortex [peak voxels: Z = 4.11, p < 1 × 10−4 and Z = 4.16, p < 1 × 10−4, MNI coordinates: 36, −76, 4 and 42, −76, 26] (Figure 6).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Group results for binocular stereopsis, revealed by the fixed-effects (N = 7) GLM contrast [SADISP > SANO-DISP]. All responses are masked to gray matter. Uncorrected p < 0.001. The crosshairs represent the peak in the left parietal cortex [MNI coordinates: −28, −74, and 46].

Figure 7 shows the overlay of regions of significant activation for both the monocular stereopsis and binocular stereopsis contrasts. In order to identify potential activity associated with the phenomenology of stereopsis distinct from activity arising from disparity processing we conducted a conjunction analysis to determine regions that were significantly overlapping between the monocular and binocular stereopsis contrasts. The analysis confirmed a region in the left parietal cortex at [−24, −76, 46] mm [MNI:x,y,z] as significant (p < 0.001).

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Group results (N = 7) for monocular (green) and binocular (red) stereopsis, and conjunction analysis for monocular and binocular stereopsis (red-yellow), revealed by the fMRI fixed effects results. The main effects are masked to gray matter. Uncorrected p < 0.001. The crosshairs represent the significantly overlapping region between monocular and binocular stereopsis [MNI coordinates: −24, −76, and 46].

Due to the probability of type-1 errors in the uncorrected voxelwise analysis, we applied a FDR of 0.05 to the uncorrected (P < 0.001) thresholds. The FDR corrected p < 0.001 threshold revealed significant clusters only in the monocular stereopsis contrast and not in the binocular stereopsis contrast. Specifically, in the monocular stereopsis contrast two clusters in the left parietal cortex [MNI coordinates: −28, −60, 54 and −28, −80, 36] and a cluster in the right parietal cortex [MNI coordinates: 36, −74, 50] were identified (Figure 8).

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. Group results for monocular stereopsis, revealed by the fixed-effects (N = 7) GLM contrast [MaPINTACT > MaPSCRAMB] > [BPINTACT > BPSCRAMB]. All responses are masked to gray matter. FDR (p < 0.05) corrected p < 0.001. The crosshairs represent the peak in the left parietal cortex [MNI coordinates: −28, −60, and 54].

Discussion

The sensation of realness associated with binocular stereopsis is one of the central attributes of human perceptual experience of the visual world and has been the subject of enquiry since the very beginning of empirical science. Our exploratory study developed and tested an experimental paradigm aimed at identifying the potential neural underpinnings of this important visual attribute. Our paradigm capitalized on findings that this sensation can be produced under significantly different viewing and stimulus condition (stereoscopic images and monocular-aperture viewing of single pictorial images). This allowed us to attempt to distinguish experimentally between the processing of sensory information (binocular disparity) that is conventionally believed to give rise to the phenomenology and the visual phenomenology itself.

Specifically, we measured the amplitude of the BOLD signal while participants passively viewed images of natural scenes or objects under three different viewing conditions (BP, SA, and MaP). Both the MaP and SA (disparity) conditions were expected to induce the qualitative impressions of stereopsis and realness. We hypothesized that conditions yielding binocular and monocular stereopsis would reveal both different and similar clusters of voxels; different because the source of signals specifying 3D structure differ in the two cases and similar because in both cases the same 3D objects and scenes are viewed and perceived. We also reasoned that the existence of a generic visual process or attribute related to the phenomenology of stereopsis (and any associated cognitive state) should imply unique dissociable loci of neural processing regardless of the source of sensory signals. On this interpretation, we expected that we would find specific overlapping cortical regions recruited in contrasts aimed at titrating signals linked to binocular and monocular stereopsis. Since we had no a-priori region of interest in the cortex, expecting potential substrates in visual, parietal, temporal, motor, premotor, or prefrontal cortices we conducted a whole-brain analysis.

In order to dissociate activity specifically linked to the perception of objects, scenes and 3D structure, we examined fixed-effects GLM contrasts under both binocular and monocular-aperture viewing of single pictures (BPINTACT > BPSCRAMB and MaPINTACT > MaPSCRAMB). As expected, we found generally similar results in these two contrasts, with significant clusters in both dorsal and ventral aspects of the visual stream as well as the cingulate cortex (Figures 3, 4), consistent with areas typically identified with the perception of objects, scenes and 3D structure (Epstein and Kanwisher, 1998; Kourtzi and Kanwisher, 2001; Denys et al., 2004; Georgieva et al., 2008; also see a review Orban et al., 2004). In order to specifically dissociate activity linked to the phenomenology of realness and stereopsis from the generic perception of objects, scenes and 3D structure, we examined the contrasts of monocular stereopsis ([MaPINTACT > MaPSCRAMB] > [BPINTACT > BPSCRAMB]) and binocular stereopsis (SADISP > SANO-DISP). For the binocular stereopsis contrasts (SADISP > SANO-DISP; Figure 6), we identified significant clusters in left posterior parietal cortex (PPC) and bilateral lateral occipital cortex (LOC), in agreement with previous human and non-human primate fMRI studies implicating the processing of binocular disparity into 3D perception in both ventral and dorsal aspects of the visual stream (LOC: Kourtzi and Kanwisher, 2001; Gilaie-Dotan et al., 2002; Neri et al., 2004; Bridge and Parker, 2007; Chandrasekaran et al., 2007; PPC: Taira et al., 2000; Tsutsui et al., 2002; Tsao et al., 2003; Durand et al., 2007, 2009; Georgieva et al., 2009; Joly et al., 2009; Minini et al., 2010; Rosenberg et al., 2013; Rosenberg and Angelaki, 2014; Van Dromme et al., 2015, 2016; Verhoef et al., 2015). For the monocular stereopsis contrast ([MaPINTACT > MaPSCRAMB] > [BPINTACT > BPSCRAMB]; Figure 5), we identified significant clusters in bilateral PPC without the recruitment of LOC. While the differences between the two stereopsis contrasts may be attributable to differences in the construction of the contrasts themselves (namely, derived from a third vs. a second level contrast, respectively), the processed signals (disparity is present in the stimulus in the binocular stereopsis contrast but not in the monocular stereopsis contrast) did also drive the differences. Finally, a conjunction analysis between the monocular and binocular stereopsis contrasts (Figure 7) identified significant voxels only in the left posterior parietal cortex at [−24, −76, 46] mm [MNI:x,y,z], which is putative posterior intraparietal sulcus (IPS) (Durand et al., 2009; Georgieva et al., 2009; Zlatkina and Petrides, 2014). Previous research has implicated the IPS in binocular disparity defined depth (Non-human primate fMRI: Taira et al., 2000; Tsutsui et al., 2002; Tsao et al., 2003; Durand et al., 2007; Joly et al., 2009; Rosenberg et al., 2013; Rosenberg and Angelaki, 2014; Van Dromme et al., 2015, 2016; Verhoef et al., 2015; Human fMRI: Tsao et al., 2003; Chandrasekaran et al., 2007; Durand et al., 2009; Georgieva et al., 2009; Minini et al., 2010) and also the visuo-motor transformations and visual localization in 3D space that permits guided motor control and interaction (Grefkes and Fink, 2005; Tunik et al., 2007; Konen et al., 2013; Vingerhoets, 2014; Gallivan and Culham, 2015; Freud et al., 2016; Buchwald et al., 2018; Chen et al., 2018). The IPS, specifically BOLD activity in anterior IPS, has also been linked to the level of incongruence (disparity vs. perspective) in depth cues independent of the depth value specified by either cue (see also Brouwer et al., 2005; Brouwer et al., 2009 for related work). This is interesting in light of our study, because our BOLD contrasts involve subtracting incongruent cue stimulus (BP) from two types of congruent cue stimuli (Monocular aperture and SA). In the BP condition, the disparity cue specifies a flat surface, which conflicts with the depth specified by the pictorial cues, while the MaP and SA condition present no incongruence. However, there are some differences as compared to the previous studies. We found greater BOLD activations in the IPS region for congruent vs. incongruent stimuli (MaP > BP, SA > BP), while Brouwer et al. (2009) found greater activation in the anterior IPS for higher incongruence. Also, their study had high levels of incongruence (opposite slants) while our stimuli only had a modest level of incongruence where the disparity signal is quite small due to the small size of the images (close to zero disparity in the SA condition). Moreover, our study is coarser grained in terms of localizations. The results, taken together, do however, raise an interesting potential interpretation due to the intrinsic link between cue-coherence and the sensation of realness. Under natural (real) stimulation, in which the visual system evolved, depth cues are typically congruent. One possibility is that conflict of cues in specific types of artificial stimuli, like pictorial images, blocks successful derivation of action relevant spatial attributes (e.g., scaled depth) in the dorsal stream resulting in both a heightened awareness of conflict (or perceptual duality) on one hand and diminished sensations of negative space and tangibility associated with “realness” on the other.

These results provide the first glimpse that the neural processing specifically associated with the phenomenology of visual realness (stereopsis) can be dissociated. Moreover, the results revealed that binocular and monocular stereopsis share underlying neural substrates. From a theoretical standpoint, the findings of dissociable activity in posterior parietal cortex regions implicated IPS lend support to the claim that the visual impression of realness is associated with the conscious awareness of the capacity to manipulate 3D objects (Michotte, 1948). The results are also consistent with other recent work in fMRI that has implicated similar regions of the parietal cortex in differentiating visual perception guiding movements to either real or pictured objects (Freud et al., 2018) and our own recent EEG data revealing dissociable EEG gamma activity in the posterior parietal cortex associated with the qualitative impression of monocular stereopsis (Uji et al., 2019).

Limitations

The exploratory nature of this study presents several limitations to the interpretation of our results. First and foremost is that our sample size was limited to seven participants due to the restricted scanner time that was funded for this project.2 Ideally, for a whole-brain analysis with no a-priori ROIs, we would have preferred to test 15–20 participants. Due to the limited sample size, we reasoned that we would not have the power to identify dissociative activations based on standard random-effects GLM analyses. Therefore, we used a FLAME fixed-effect analysis at the fourth group-level analysis averaging across all participants (Matthews and Jezzard, 2004; Woolrich et al., 2004). The fixed effect analysis provides an expression of changes in the group mean signal relative to the group pooled within-subject variance, not taking into consideration any intersubjective variability. This makes our findings hard to generalize to a larger population outside the sampled group (Matthews and Jezzard, 2004; Friston et al., 2005). Furthermore, in fixed effect analyses there is a high risk that the results are driven primarily by one or two participants only. This could indeed be an interpretation of our results as individual subject z-scores of the monocular stereopsis contrast show that high z-scores were only seen for two or three participants at the group-wise peak voxel locations identified by the conjunction analysis (see Figure 9). More precisely, for the monocular stereopsis contrast, which was the only contrast where there were surviving clusters after FDR corrections at the group level, it appears that subjects 1 and 4 predominantly contributed to the group level significant clusters in the parietal cortex. Nonetheless, five out of seven participants revealed similar BOLD responses in those significant regions, thus resulting in the fixed-effect FDR corrected significant clusters at the group level (see Figure 9).

FIGURE 9
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 9. Individual participants results by the third-level fixed effects contrast [(MaPINTACT > MaPSCRAMB) > [BPINTACT > BPSCRAMB)] for monocular (green) stereopsis. The crosshairs represent the significantly overlapping region between monocular and binocular stereopsis [MNI coordinates: −24, −76, and 46].

Second, due to the power limitations, we initially did not employ any family wise error correction (e.g., Bonferroni, False Discovery Rate), or apply the more conventional cluster-based thresholding approach (Woo et al., 2014; Eklund et al., 2016, 2018), thresholding-free cluster enhancement (TFCE) approach (Smith and Nichols, 2009), or randomization methods/non-parametric permutation methods (Winkler et al., 2014). Instead, we opted for a simple voxel-based thresholding at uncorrected p < 0.001 instead of more liberal approach of a cluster-based thresholding using Z > 2.3 for multiple comparisons (Woo et al., 2014; Eklund et al., 2016, 2018; Bansal and Peterson, 2018). We are aware of the risks this raises of false positives in our findings. Therefore, we applied FDR corrections (p < 0.05) on both binocular and monocular stereopsis contrasts (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995; Nichols, 2012). Applying FDR corrections revealed no significant clusters in the binocular stereopsis contrast, whereas the monocular stereopsis contrast revealed two significant clusters in the left parietal cortex and one significant cluster in the right parietal cortex (see Figure 8).

Third, our goals here were quite modest, to simply see if under near whole-brain analysis (excluding the cerebellum), we could identify voxels that dissociate between conditions in which stereopsis and the phenomenology or realness is present or absent. We did not aim to test specific hypotheses based on a-priori ROIs or establish what such dissociable activity might entail (e.g., visuo-motor representations, recognition, and cognitive states). Previous fMRI studies on 3D vision have typically limited analysis to the visual cortex and regions of extra striate visual pathways as well as measuring retinotopic mapping in order to increase spatial resolution and localization in the visual cortex (Backus et al., 2001; Neri et al., 2004; Ban and Welchman, 2015; Goncalves et al., 2015). Similarly, studies in visuo-motor control have also limited coverage over the visual, parietal, and motor cortex (Culham et al., 2003; Gallivan et al., 2009, 2011). Future investigation of the phenomenology of stereopsis and realness would likely yield more robust results taking this more fine-grained approach, along with increased sample size both at the participants and runs/trials level and employment of multivoxel pattern analysis methods to analyze distributed patterns of neural activity to infer the functional role of brain areas and networks engaged during this phenomenology (Norman et al., 2006; Haynes, 2015).

Implications: Functional Links to Depth Processing

Interestingly, recent studies have examined differences in the cortical processing of visual stimulation arising from real or pictured objects. The focus of these studies was on the difference between real objects and pictures as physical stimuli in fMRI paradigms aimed at identifying neural processes underlying object representation (Snow et al., 2011) and visually guided grasping/reaching (Freud et al., 2018). Specifically, Snow et al. (2011) found that regions of the IPS and lateral occipital cortex, which showed typical repetition adaptation effects to pictured objects, did not show such effects for real objects. Freud et al. (2018) similarly, found differential activation in the IPS to real 3D objects vs. 2D images during the planning phase (pre-movement initiation) of grasping and reaching motor tasks. Although these studies did not aim to specifically identify which visual attributes, cues or visual processing underlie the difference between real and pictured objects, their findings do provide support for our overall interpretation that dissociable patterns of results exists that distinguishes between the phenomenology of real depth (stereopsis) and pictorial depth.

How can this be linked to existing and future work in the neurophysiology of depth perception? In most observers, binocular disparity produces the strongest impression of the phenomenological attributes of stereopsis (object solidity, negative space, and tangibility). Several IPS regions (dorsal IPS anterior, dorsal IPS medial, parieto-occipital IPS, and ventral IPS regions) have been shown to be recruited in the presence of binocular disparity defined depth structure (Durand et al., 2009; Georgieva et al., 2009). For example, Durand et al. (2009) demonstrated that human anterior IPS (Dorsal IPS medial at [−22, −62, 56] mm [MNI: x, y, z]; dorsal IPS anterior at [−30, −50, 64] mm) was recruited when processing 3D structure, whereas the posterior IPS [−24, −82, 32] mm was processed when processing location in 3D space. The neural processing of binocular disparity and stereopsis might not be possible to disentangle when binocular cues contribute to stereopsis, given the strong role of binocular cues in stereopsis. However, our conjunction analysis demonstrated that dissociated neural activity in the posterior parietal cortex associated with the qualitative impression of stereopsis or realness under a specific viewing condition that generated an impression of stereopsis without binocular disparity (monocular aperture viewing). This shows that binocular disparity and stereopsis occurs along the dorsal visual pathway all the way through the regions responsible for visually guided manual action encoding both location in depth and 3D shape. Visual guidance of movement, in principle, requires the derivation of absolute values of object dimensions scaled to the motor plant (e.g., separation of thumb and fingers for grasping; required distance of hand transport for reaching; Glennerster et al., 1996; Kopiske et al., 2018); on the other hand, the 3D shape of an object can be specified fully by only relative depth values. How might this be linked to the difference between the phenomenology of stereopsis and perception of pictorial depth? A recent view is that the primary phenomenological components of the experience of stereopsis (the impression of tangibility, solidity and negative space) is linked to successful encoding of scaled intra- and inter-objects dimensions required for visuo-motor control, while pictorial depth (no stereopsis) constitutes 3D perception without scale (Vishwanath, 2014). Since binocular disparity is widely considered the best signal for the derivation of scaled depth values (Watt and Bradshaw, 2003), the conjecture can explain why the phenomenology of stereopsis and realness is most vividly seen in the presence of binocular disparity, but like Michotte’s proposal, it does not restrict the phenomenology of stereopsis to conditions where only binocular disparity or motion parallax is present. No studies, to our knowledge, have examined information processing along the dorsal stream in terms of depth or disparity scaling, likely because it is difficult to design parametric stimulus sets that dissociate changes in this attribute (depth scaling) from changes in other variables, such as retinal size, stimulus size, stimulus distance, disparity magnitude, and perceived depth magnitude. Integrating questions arising from visual phenomenology and visual function (depth discrimination, estimation, and visuo-motor capacity) via theoretically motivated models may begin to yield new ways in which to interrogate and operationalize these questions.

Here we have reported on the first attempt at just such an integration utilizing a neuroimaging paradigm. Future neuroimaging research will need to provide more robust confirmatory evidence and determine if the representation and processes that bring about the experience of stereopsis and realness are the same regardless of the source of the depth signal (binocular disparity, motion parallax, synopter, or pictorial cues) and also identify the specific stage of transformation of visual information that underlies this central phenomenological aspect of human 3D space perception.

Data Availability

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the University Teaching and Research Ethics Committee at the University of St. Andrews, and East of Scotland Research Ethics Service at the Tayside Medical Science Centre (TACS). The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

DV and AL designed the experiments. DV and IC conducted the experiments. MU and DV processed and analyzed the data. MU, DV, and AL wrote the manuscript.

Funding

DV and MU were supported by the Leverhulme Trust Project Grant (Grant Reference RGP-2016-269).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Jennifer MacFarlane (NHS Tayside) for facilitating data acquisition. Akira O’Connor for support in data processing and Justin Ales for comments on the manuscript.

Footnotes

  1. ^ https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/
  2. ^ This project was conducted under a research development budget awarded by Clinical Research Centre, NHS Tayside.

References

Ames, A. (1925). The illusion of depth from single pictures. J. Opt. Soc. Am. 10, 137–148. doi: 10.1364/JOSA.10.000137

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anzai, A., and DeAngelis, G. C. (2010). Neural computations underlying depth perception. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 20, 367–375. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2010.04.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Backus, B. T., Fleet, D. J., Parker, A. J., and Heeger, D. J. (2001). Human cortical activity correlates with stereoscopic depth perception. J. Neurophysiol. 86, 2054–2068. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.86.4.2054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ban, H., and Welchman, A. E. (2015). fMRI analysis-by-synthesis reveals a dorsal hierarchy that extracts surface slant. J. Neurosci. 35, 9823–9835. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1255-15.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bansal, R., and Peterson, B. S. (2018). Cluster-level statistical inference in fMRI datasets: the unexpected behavior of random fields in high dimensions. Magn. Reson. Imaging 49, 101–115. doi: 10.1016/j.mri.2018.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barlow, H. B., Blakemore, C., and Pettigrew, J. D. (1967). The neural mechanism of binocular depth discrimination. J. Physiol. 193, 327–342. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1967.sp008360

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benjamini, Y., and Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. Series B 57, 289–300.

Google Scholar

Binkofski, F., Buccino, G., Stephan, K. M., Rizzolatti, G., Seitz, R. J., and Freund, H.-J. (1999). A parieto-premotor network for object manipulation: evidence from neuroimaging. Exp. Brain Res. 128, 210–213. doi: 10.1007/s002210050838

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bridge, H., and Parker, A. J. (2007). Topographical representation of binocular depth in the human visual cortex using fMRI. J. Vis. 7:15. doi: 10.1167/7.14.15

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brouwer, G. J., Tong, F., Hagoort, P., and van Ee, R. (2009). Perceptual incongruence influences bistability and cortical activation. PLoS One 4:e5056. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0005056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brouwer, G. J., van Ee, R., and Schwarzbach, J. (2005). Activation in visual cortex correlates with the awareness of stereoscopic depth. J. Neurosci. 25, 10403–10413. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2408-05.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buchwald, M., Przybylski, Ł, and Króliczak, G. (2018). Decoding brain states for planning functional grasps of tools: a functional magnetic resonance imaging multivoxel pattern analysis study. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 24, 1013–1025. doi: 10.1017/S1355617718000590

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chandrasekaran, C., Canon, V., Dahmen, J. C., Kourtzi, Z., and Welchman, A. E. (2007). Neural correlates of disparity-defined shape discrimination in the human brain. J. Neurophysiol. 97, 1553–1565. doi: 10.1152/jn.01074.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, J., Snow, J. C., Culham, J. C., and Goodale, M. A. (2018). What role does “elongation” play in “tool-specific” activation and connectivity in the dorsal and ventral visual streams? Cereb. Cortex 28, 1117–1131. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Claparède, É (1904). Stereoscopie monoculaire paradoxale. Ann. Ocul. 132, 465–466.

Google Scholar

Cottereau, B. R., McKee, S. P., Ales, J. M., and Norcia, A. M. (2011). Disparity-tuned population responses from human visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 31, 954–965. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3795-10.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Culham, J. C., Danckert, S. L., Souza, J. F. X., De Gati, J. S., Menon, R. S., and Goodale, M. A. (2003). Visually guided grasping produces fMRI activation in dorsal but not ventral stream brain areas. Exp. Brain Res. 153, 180–189. doi: 10.1007/s00221-003-1591-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cumming, B. G. (2002). An unexpected specialization for horizontal disparity in primate primary visual cortex. Nature 418, 633–636. doi: 10.1038/nature00909

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Denys, K., Vanduffel, W., Fize, D., Nelissen, K., Peuskens, H., Van Essen, D., et al. (2004). The processing of visual shape in the cerebral cortex of human and nonhuman primates: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J. Neurosci. 24, 2551–2565. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3569-03.2004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Durand, J.-B., Nelissen, K., Joly, O., Wardak, C., Todd, J. T., Norman, J. F., et al. (2007). Anterior regions of monkey parietal cortex process visual 3D shape. Neuron 55, 493–505. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.06.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Durand, J.-B., Peeters, R., Norman, J. F., Todd, J. T., and Orban, G. A. (2009). Parietal regions processing visual 3D shape extracted from disparity. Neuroimage 46, 1114–1126. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.03.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eickhoff, S. B., Laird, A. R., Grefkes, C., Wang, L. E., Zilles, K., and Fox, P. T. (2009). Coordinate-based activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of neuroimaging data: a random-effects approach based on empirical estimates of spatial uncertainty. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 2907–2926. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20718

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eklund, A., Knutsson, H., and Nichols, T. E. (2018). Cluster failure revisited: impact of first level design and physiological noise on cluster false positive rates. arXiv doi: 10.1002/hbm.24350

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eklund, A., Nichols, T. E., and Knutsson, H. (2016). Cluster failure: why fMRI inferences for spatial extent have inflated false-positive rates. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 113, 7900–7905. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1602413113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Epstein, R., and Kanwisher, N. (1998). A cortical representation of the local visual environment. Nature 392, 598–601. doi: 10.1038/33402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freud, E., Macdonald, S. N., Chen, J., Quinlan, D. J., Goodale, M. A., and Culham, J. C. (2018). Getting a grip on reality: grasping movements directed to real objects and images rely on dissociable neural representations. Cortex 98, 34–48. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.02.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freud, E., Plaut, D. C., and Behrmann, M. (2016). ‘What’ is happening in the dorsal visual pathway. Trends Cogn. Sci. 20, 773–784. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K. J., Penny, W. D., and Glaser, D. E. (2005). Conjunction revisited. Neuroimage 25, 661–667. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.01.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallivan, J. P., Cavina-Pratesi, C., and Culham, J. C. (2009). Is that within reach? fMRI reveals that the human superior parieto-occipital cortex encodes objects reachable by the hand. J. Neurosci. 29, 4381–4391. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0377-09.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallivan, J. P., and Culham, J. C. (2015). Neural coding within human brain areas involved in actions. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 33, 141–149. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2015.03.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallivan, J. P., McLean, A., and Culham, J. C. (2011). Neuroimaging reveals enhanced activation in a reach-selective brain area for objects located within participants’ typical hand workspaces. Neuropsychologia 49, 3710–3721. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.09.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Georgieva, S., Peeters, R., Kolster, H., Todd, J. T., and Orban, G. A. (2009). The processing of three-dimensional shape from disparity in the human brain. J. Neurosci. 29, 727–742. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4753-08.2009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Georgieva, S. S., Todd, J. T., Peeters, R., and Orban, G. A. (2008). The extraction of 3D shape from texture and shading in the human brain. Cereb. Cortex 18, 2416–2438. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilaie-Dotan, S., Ullman, S., Kushnir, T., and Malach, R. (2002). Shape-selective stereo processing in human object-related visual areas. Hum. Brain Mapp. 15, 67–79. doi: 10.1002/hbm.10008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glennerster, A., Rogers, B. J., and Bradshaw, M. F. (1996). Stereoscopic depth constancy depends on the subject’s task. Vision Res. 36, 3441–3456. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(96)00090-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goncalves, N. R., Ban, H., Sanchez-Panchuelo, R. M., Francis, S. T., Schluppeck, D., and Welchman, A. E. (2015). 7 tesla fMRI reveals systematic functional organization for binocular disparity in dorsal visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 35, 3056–3072. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3047-14.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gonzalez, F., and Perez, R. (1998). Neural mechanisms underlying stereoscopic vision. Prog. Neurobiol. 55, 191–224. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0082(98)00012-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodale, M. A., and Milner, A. D. (1992). Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends Neurosci. 15, 20–25. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(92)90344-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodale, M. A., and Milner, A. D. (2018). Two visual pathways – where have they taken us and where will they lead in future? Cortex 98, 283–292. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grefkes, C., and Fink, G. R. (2005). REVIEW: the functional organization of the intraparietal sulcus in humans and monkeys. J. Anat. 207, 3–17. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7580.2005.00426.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanakawa, T., Dimyan, M. A., and Hallett, M. (2008). Motor planning, imagery, and execution in the distributed motor network: a time-course study with functional MRI. Cereb. Cortex 18, 2775–2788. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haynes, J.-D. (2015). A primer on pattern-based approaches to fMRI: principles, pitfalls, and perspectives. Neuron 87, 257–270. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.05.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Helmholtz, H. V. (1909). Treatise on Physiological Optics. New York, NY: Dover.

Google Scholar

Hibbard, P. B. (2008a). Can appearance be so deceptive? Representationalism and binocular vision. Spat. Vis. 21, 549–559. doi: 10.1163/156856808786451444

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hibbard, P. B. (2008b). Binocular energy responses to natural images. Vision Res. 48, 1427–1439. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2008.03.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jenkinson, M., Beckmann, C. F., Behrens, T. E. J., Woolrich, M. W., and Smith, S. M. (2012). FSL. Neuroimage 62, 782–790. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joly, O., Vanduffel, W., and Orban, G. A. (2009). The monkey ventral premotor cortex processes 3D shape from disparity. Neuroimage 47, 262–272. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.04.043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Julesz, B. (1971). Foundations of Cyclopean Perception. Chicago, IL: Univ. Chicago Press.

Google Scholar

Koenderink, J. J. (1998). Pictorial relief. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. A Math. Phys. Eng. Sci. 356, 1071–1086. doi: 10.1098/rsta.1998.0211

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koenderink, J. J., van Doorn, A. J., and Kappers, A. M. (1994). On so-called paradoxical monocular stereoscopy. Perception 23, 583–594. doi: 10.1068/p230583

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Konen, C. S., Mruczek, R. E. B., Montoya, J. L., and Kastner, S. (2013). Functional organization of human posterior parietal cortex: grasping- and reaching-related activations relative to topographically organized cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 109, 2897–2908. doi: 10.1152/jn.00657.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kopiske, K. K., Bozzacchi, C., Volcic, R., and Domini, F. (2018). Multiple distance cues do not prevent systematic biases in reach to grasp movements. Psychol. Res. 83, 147–158. doi: 10.1007/s00426-018-1101-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kourtzi, Z., and Kanwisher, N. (2001). Representation of perceived object shape by the human lateral occipital complex. Science 293, 1506–1509. doi: 10.1126/science.1061133

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kravitz, D. J., Saleem, K. S., Baker, C. I., and Mishkin, M. (2011). A new neural framework for visuospatial processing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 12, 217–230. doi: 10.1038/nrn3008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matthews, P. M., and Jezzard, P. (2004). Functional magnetic resonance imaging. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 75, 6–12.

Google Scholar

Michotte, A. (1948). The psychological enigma of perspective in outline pictures. Bull. la Cl. des Lettres l’Academie R. Belgique 34, 268–288.

Google Scholar

Milner, A. D., and Goodale, M. A. (2008). Two visual systems re-viewed. Neuropsychologia 46, 774–785. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Minini, L., Parker, A. J., and Bridge, H. (2010). Neural modulation by binocular disparity greatest in human dorsal visual stream. J. Neurophysiol. 104, 169–178. doi: 10.1152/jn.00790.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murray, S. O., Olshausen, B. A., and Woods, D. L. (2003). Processing shape, motion and three-dimensional shape-from-motion in the human cortex. Cereb. Cortex 13, 508–516. doi: 10.1093/cercor/13.5.508

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Naganuma, T., Nose, I., Inoue, K., Takemoto, A., Katsuyama, N., and Taira, M. (2005). Information processing of geometrical features of a surface based on binocular disparity cues: an fMRI study. Neurosci. Res. 51, 147–155. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2004.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neri, P., Bridge, H., and Heeger, D. J. (2004). Stereoscopic processing of absolute and relative disparity in human visual cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 92, 1880–1891. doi: 10.1152/jn.01042.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nichols, T., Brett, M., Andersson, J., Wager, T., and Poline, J.-B. (2005). Valid conjunction inference with the minimum statistic. Neuroimage 25, 653–660. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nichols, T. E. (2012). Multiple testing corrections, nonparametric methods, and random field theory. Neuroimage 62, 811–815. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.04.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Norman, K. A., Polyn, S. M., Detre, G. J., and Haxby, J. V. (2006). Beyond mind-reading: multi-voxel pattern analysis of fMRI data. Trends Cogn. Sci. 10, 424–430. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.07.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ohzawa, I., DeAngelis, G., and Freeman, R. (1990). Stereoscopic depth discrimination in the visual cortex: neurons ideally suited as disparity detectors. Science 249, 1037–1041. doi: 10.1126/science.2396096

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Orban, G. A. (2011). The extraction of 3D shape in the visual system of human and nonhuman primates. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 34, 361–388. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113819

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Orban, G. A., Sunaert, S., Todd, J. T., Van Hecke, P., and Marchal, G. (1999). Human cortical regions involved in extracting depth from motion. Neuron 24, 929–940. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)81040-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Orban, G. A., Van Essen, D., and Vanduffel, W. (2004). Comparative mapping of higher visual areas in monkeys and humans. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 315–324. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.05.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parker, A. J. (2007). Binocular depth perception and the cerebral cortex. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 379–391. doi: 10.1038/nrn2131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poggio, G. F., Motter, B. C., Squatrito, S., and Trotter, Y. (1985). Responses of neurons in visual cortex (V1 and V2) of the alert macaque to dynamic random-dot stereograms. Vision Res. 25, 397–406. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(85)90065-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Preston, T. J., Li, S., Kourtzi, Z., and Welchman, A. E. (2008). Multivoxel pattern selectivity for perceptually relevant binocular disparities in the human brain. J. Neurosci. 28, 11315–11327. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2728-08.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenberg, A., and Angelaki, D. E. (2014). Reliability-dependent contributions of visual orientation cues in parietal cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 111, 18043–18048. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1421131111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenberg, A., Cowan, N. J., and Angelaki, D. E. (2013). The visual representation of 3D object orientation in parietal cortex. J. Neurosci. 33, 19352–19361. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3174-13.2013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sakata, H., Tsutsui, K.-I., and Taira, M. (2005). Toward an understanding of the neural processing for 3D shape perception. Neuropsychologia 43, 151–161. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schlosberg, H. (1941). Stereoscopic depth from single pictures. Am. J. Psychol. 54, 601–605. doi: 10.2307/1417214

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schubotz, R. I., and Von Cramon, D. Y. (2002). Predicting perceptual events activates corresponding motor schemes in lateral premotor cortex: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 15, 787–796. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.1043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shikata, E., Hamzei, F., Glauche, V., Knab, R., Dettmers, C., Weiller, C., et al. (2001). Surface orientation discrimination activates caudal and anterior intraparietal sulcus in humans: an event-related fMRI study. J. Neurophysiol. 85, 1309–1314. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.85.3.1309

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, S. M., and Nichols, T. E. (2009). Threshold-free cluster enhancement: addressing problems of smoothing, threshold dependence and localisation in cluster inference. Neuroimage 44, 83–98. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Snow, J. C., Pettypiece, C. E., McAdam, T. D., McLean, A. D., Stroman, P. W., Goodale, M. A., et al. (2011). Bringing the real world into the fMRI scanner: repetition effects for pictures versus real objects. Sci. Rep. 1:130. doi: 10.1038/srep00130

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taira, M., Nose, I., Inoue, K., and Tsutsui, K. (2001). Cortical areas related to attention to 3D surface structures based on shading: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 14, 959–966. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0895

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taira, M., Tsutsui, K.-I., Jiang, M., Yara, K., and Sakata, H. (2000). Parietal neurons represent surface orientation from the gradient of binocular disparity. J. Neurophysiol. 83, 3140–3146. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.83.5.3140

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsao, D. Y., Vanduffel, W., Sasaki, Y., Fize, D., Knutsen, T. A., Mandeville, J. B., et al. (2003). Stereopsis activates V3A and caudal intraparietal areas in macaques and humans. Neuron 39, 555–568. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00459-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsutsui, K. I., Sakata, H., Naganuma, T., and Taira, M. (2002). Neural correlates for perception of 3D surface orientation from texture gradient. Science 298, 409–412. doi: 10.1126/science.1074128

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tunik, E., Rice, N. J., Hamilton, A., and Grafton, S. T. (2007). Beyond grasping: representation of action in human anterior intraparietal sulcus. Neuroimage 36, T77–T86. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uji, M., Jentzsch, I., Redburn, J., and Vishwanath, D. (2019). Dissociating neural activity associated with the subjective phenomenology of monocular stereopsis: an EEG study. Neuropsychologia 129, 357–371. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.04.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uji, M., Wilson, R., Francis, S. T., Mullinger, K. J., and Mayhew, S. D. (2018). Exploring the advantages of multiband fMRI with simultaneous EEG to investigate coupling between gamma frequency neural activity and the BOLD response in humans. Hum. Brain Mapp. 39, 1673–1687. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23943

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Dromme, I. C., Premereur, E., Verhoef, B.-E., Vanduffel, W., and Janssen, P. (2016). Posterior parietal cortex drives inferotemporal activations during three-dimensional object vision. PLoS Biol. 14:e1002445. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002445

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Dromme, I. C. L., Vanduffel, W., and Janssen, P. (2015). The relation between functional magnetic resonance imaging activations and single-cell selectivity in the macaque intraparietal sulcus. Neuroimage 113, 86–100. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.03.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Ee, R., and Richards, W. (2002). A planar and a volumetric test for stereoanomaly. Perception 31, 51–64. doi: 10.1068/p3303

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verhoef, B. E., Bohon, K. S., and Conway, B. R. (2015). Functional architecture for disparity in macaque inferior temporal cortex and its relationship to the architecture for faces, color, scenes, and visual field. J. Neurosci. 35, 6952–6968. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5079-14.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verhoef, B. E., Vogels, R., and Janssen, P. (2010). Contribution of inferior temporal and posterior parietal activity to three-dimensional shape perception. Curr. Biol. 20, 909–913. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.03.058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verhoef, B.-E., Vogels, R., and Janssen, P. (2011). Synchronization between the end stages of the dorsal and the ventral visual stream. J. Neurophysiol. 105, 2030–2042. doi: 10.1152/jn.00924.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vingerhoets, G. (2014). Contribution of the posterior parietal cortex in reaching, grasping, and using objects and tools. Front. Psychol. 5:151. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vishwanath, D. (2014). Toward a new theory of stereopsis. Psychol. Rev. 121, 151–178. doi: 10.1037/a0035233

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vishwanath, D., and Hibbard, P. B. (2013). Seeing in 3-D with just one eye: stereopsis in the absence of binocular disparities. Psychol. Sci. 24, 1673–1685. doi: 10.1177/0956797613477867

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wade, N. J., Ono, H., and Lillakas, L. (2001). Leonardo da vinci’s struggles with representations of reality. Leonardo 34, 231–235. doi: 10.1162/002409401750286994

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watt, S. J., and Bradshaw, M. F. (2003). The visual control of reaching and grasping: binocular disparity and motion parallax. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 29, 404–415. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.29.2.404

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Welchman, A. E. (2016). The human brain in depth: how we see in 3D. Annu. Rev. Vis. Sci. 2, 345–376. doi: 10.1146/annurev-vision-111815-114605

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Westheimer, G. (2011). Three-dimensional displays and stereo vision. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 278, 2241–2248. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.2777

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wheatstone, C. (1838). Contributions to the physiology of vision. part the first. on some remarkable, and hitherto unobserved, phenomena of binocular vision. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. 128, 371–394. doi: 10.1098/rstl.1838.0019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wijntjes, M. W. A. (2017). Ways of viewing pictorial plasticity. Iperception 8, 1–10. doi: 10.1177/2041669517699220

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winkler, A. M., Ridgway, G. R., Webster, M. A., Smith, S. M., and Nichols, T. E. (2014). Permutation inference for the general linear model. Neuroimage 92, 381–397. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.01.060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woo, C.-W., Krishnan, A., and Wager, T. D. (2014). Cluster-extent based thresholding in fMRI analyses: pitfalls and recommendations. Neuroimage 91, 412–419. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.12.058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woolrich, M. W., Behrens, T. E., Beckmann, C. F., Jenkinson, M., and Smith, S. M. (2004). Multilevel linear modelling for FMRI group analysis using bayesian inference. Neuroimage 21, 1732–1747.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Zhang, Y., Brady, M., and Smith, S. (2001). Segmentation of brain MR images through a hidden markov random field model and the expectation-maximization algorithm. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging 20, 45–57. doi: 10.1109/42.906424

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zlatkina, V., and Petrides, M. (2014). Morphological patterns of the intraparietal sulcus and the anterior intermediate parietal sulcus of Jensen in the human brain. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 281:20141493. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2014.1493

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: realness, stereopsis, fMRI, parietal cortex, intraparietal sulcus, 3D perception, depth perception

Citation: Uji M, Lingnau A, Cavin I and Vishwanath D (2019) Identifying Cortical Substrates Underlying the Phenomenology of Stereopsis and Realness: A Pilot fMRI Study. Front. Neurosci. 13:646. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00646

Received: 21 February 2019; Accepted: 05 June 2019;
Published: 11 July 2019.

Edited by:

Lars Muckli, University of Glasgow, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Paul Barry Hibbard, University of Essex, United Kingdom
Raymond van Ee, Radboud University Nijmegen, Netherlands

Copyright © 2019 Uji, Lingnau, Cavin and Vishwanath. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Makoto Uji, bXU2QHN0LWFuZHJld3MuYWMudWs=; Dhanraj Vishwanath, ZHYxMEBzdC1hbmRyZXdzLmFjLnVr

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.