Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 19 March 2019
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Cognitive Neuroscience Editor's Pick 2021 View all 10 articles

A Review of Psychophysiological Measures to Assess Cognitive States in Real-World Driving

\r\nMonika Lohani*Monika Lohani1*Brennan R. PayneBrennan R. Payne2David L. StrayerDavid L. Strayer2
  • 1Department of Educational Psychology, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, United States
  • 2Department of Psychology, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, United States

As driving functions become increasingly automated, motorists run the risk of becoming cognitively removed from the driving process. Psychophysiological measures may provide added value not captured through behavioral or self-report measures alone. This paper provides a selective review of the psychophysiological measures that can be utilized to assess cognitive states in real-world driving environments. First, the importance of psychophysiological measures within the context of traffic safety is discussed. Next, the most commonly used physiology-based indices of cognitive states are considered as potential candidates relevant for driving research. These include: electroencephalography and event-related potentials, optical imaging, heart rate and heart rate variability, blood pressure, skin conductance, electromyography, thermal imaging, and pupillometry. For each of these measures, an overview is provided, followed by a discussion of the methods for measuring it in a driving context. Drawing from recent empirical driving and psychophysiology research, the relative strengths and limitations of each measure are discussed to highlight each measures' unique value. Challenges and recommendations for valid and reliable quantification from lab to (less predictable) real-world driving settings are considered. Finally, we discuss measures that may be better candidates for a near real-time assessment of motorists' cognitive states that can be utilized in applied settings outside the lab. This review synthesizes the literature on in-vehicle psychophysiological measures to advance the development of effective human-machine driving interfaces and driver support systems.

The Importance of Psychophysiological Measures in Traffic Safety

Suboptimal level of cognitive functioning (e.g., inattention, drowsiness) is a key cause of traffic accidents and poor driving performance. According to Traffic Safety Culture Index, 87.5% of drivers identify distracted driving to be a greater concern today than in past years and 87.9% perceive drowsiness as a threat to their safety (AAA Foundation for Traffic Safety, 2018). Traffic safety researchers are constantly working on methods to improve driving performance by assessing cognitive states, such as drivers' workload, inattention, and fatigue. One way to improve the assessment of covert cognitive states is to adopt a multi-method approach to measure changes in central and peripheral nervous system functioning in order to sense near-real time information about cognitive states of motorists. Such assessments of internal states can also promote the development of Advanced Driver Assistance Systems (ADAS) that can predict and augment risky driving behavior.

Why Adopt Psychophysiological Measures?

Cognitive states can be assessed using subjective, behavioral, and physiological measures (Mauss and Robinson, 2009; Strayer et al., 2015; Lohani et al., 2018). Subjective measures can be limiting if the assessment is disruptive to the real-time task (i.e., primary task intrusion, see O'Donnell and Eggemeier, 1986). More importantly, humans may not always be accurate in making judgements about their cognitive states (Schmidt et al., 2009). Motorists can be inaccurate in making judgments about their internal and cognitive states (such as their attention, workload, and drowsiness levels). For instance, motorists were inaccurate at self-assessments of vigilance (Schmidt et al., 2009); even though objective physiological indicators (e.g., heart rate, EEG, and ERPs) suggested poor vigilance levels at the end of a 3-h drive, participants self-reported improved vigilance instead (Schmidt et al., 2009). Such misjudgments in assessment of cognitive states suggest that objective measures are required to assess and augment human behavior in order to reduce risk for traffic safety. While behavioral measures (such as head movement detection to assess distraction) are also useful, given the intent of this review, we will focus on physiological measures. Accuracy in detecting cognitive workload has been found to significantly increase when physiological data was utilized (Lenneman and Backs, 2009, 2010; Solovey et al., 2014; Borghini et al., 2015; Yang et al., 2016). Some work has also found that physiological measures were sensitive to variations in cognitive load during secondary tasks while behavioral driving measures like steering wheel reversals and velocity (Belyusar et al., 2015) and lane-keeping measures (Lenneman and Backs, 2009) were not. Unlike behavioral measures (e.g., verbal and facial behavior), many physiological measures are not under voluntary control of motorists. Moreover, cognitive states such as mental workload are a multi-faceted and dynamic concept and self-report alone cannot be used to operationalize it, but multiple measures (e.g., performance and physiology) are warranted (de Waard and Lewis-Evans, 2014). Thus, inclusion of physiological data can complement and extend behavioral metrics and improve assessments of motorists' state-level changes in cognition (Brookhuis and de Waard, 1993; Mehler et al., 2012).

As automation is likely to become more prevalent over time, real-time monitoring behaviors required by motorists may decline as they are less involved in the driving process. This is a critical reason why non-behavior-based metrics will become more relevant to incorporate into our understanding of the motorists' cognitive states. Moreover, distracted motorists of a self-driving vehicle compared to manually driving motorists take longer to gain control of the driving task once automation deactivates (Vogelpohl et al., 2018). Intelligent driving assistance systems should be capable of reliably sensing and assessing distraction and drowsiness levels of motorists to be able to augment safe-driving conditions. Building reliable systems to be able to predict decreased levels of vigilance or dangerous levels of fatigue, drowsiness, or workload could help augment them in a timely manner (Balters et al., 2018).

Cognition in Dynamic Real-World Driving Contexts

In general, psychophysiological measures can be used to assess degree of arousal or activation (Mauss and Robinson, 2009). Importantly, multiple psychological constructs can influence variations in psychophysiological measures. For instance, heart rate, skin conductance, and electrical activity of the brain are sensitive to many psychological constructs experienced by motorists, such as workload, drowsiness, stress, etc. In the past years, important contributions have reviewed the literature on specific cognitive states, such as workload (Borghini et al., 2014; Costa et al., 2017), distraction (Matthews et al., 2019), drowsiness (Sahayadhas et al., 2012; Borghini et al., 2014), and stress (Rastgoo et al., 2018) in driving research. These reviews provide an understanding of physiological outcomes that can explain variations in specific constructs based on carefully manipulated and well-controlled designs. Unlike highly controlled lab-based settings, where a single construct (e.g., workload) can be successfully manipulated and its effect on psychophysiological measures examined, real-world settings are more dynamic and complex.

In a real-world setting, the net resulting cognitive state of a motorist is a combination of variation among several interrelated constructs (e.g., attention allocation, stress, workload, fatigue). Broadly speaking, the net cognitive state of a motorist, composed of variation among these many dimensions, can be classified along an arousal-spectrum ranging from lower-arousal and passive states, to a state of optimal performance, to a hyper-aroused or over-active state. Indeed, this concept is not new; Yerkes and Dodson (1908) established strong non-linear relationships between arousal-level and performance, and such relationships have since been well-established across many human performance domains (Hebb, 1955; Broadhurst, 1959; Wekselblatt and Niell, 2015). Although these ideas are not new, there has been a recent resurgence in a formal understanding of arousal-performance relationships, including an expanded understanding of the underlying neuromodulatory systems involved in regulating task engagement and optimal performance (e.g., the adaptive-gain control theory, Aston-Jones and Cohen, 2005). Given the recent increase in understanding of the mapping between physiological indices of arousal and human performance in the lab, such models serve as a clear starting point in delineating the predictive capacity of psychophysiological measures for understanding cognitive states and human performance in the vehicle.

For instance, low-arousal states relevant to the driving task can be driven by a combination of psychological constructs including low workload, reduced stress, and high drowsiness. On the other hand, an over-aroused state could be due to a combination of high workload and high stress in the presence of low drowsiness. Similarly, other combinations of constructs can also lead to changes in general arousal states as well. Given the likely dynamic interplay among these interrelated constructs in applied settings, the current review focuses on psychophysiological measures that can be utilized to capture motorists' states in real-world driving settings. Indeed, one major applied goal of this work is to be able to accurately capture the dynamic and highly variable changes in arousal that occur in ecologically valid driving settings, a goal that is critical for building accurate predictive models (Yarkoni and Westfall, 2017) of individual motorist's states and future driving performance.

Specifically, there are two novel contributions of this review. First, instead of focusing on a selective construct and related measures of interest, the goal of this current review is to focus on psychophysiological measures that may have the potential to be adopted in real-world and applied settings to measure state level variations in motorists. The paper provides a broad but selective review of a number of psychophysiological measures that we believe show the greatest promise in their utilization to assess low-arousal vs. over-arousal (passive vs. over-active) states in real-world driving environments. The most commonly used physiology-based measures of cognitive states are considered as potential candidates relevant for driving research. The following physiological measures are reviewed (see section “Psychophysiological Measures to Assess Cognitive States” and Tables 1, 2) in assessing arousal state in real-world driving research: electroencephalography and event-related potentials, optical imaging, heart rate, and heart rate variability, blood pressure, skin conductance, electromyography, thermal imaging, and pupillometry. As reviewed in classical contributions by Cacioppo et al. (Cacioppo and Tassinary, 1990; Cacioppo et al., 2007), inference of unique psychological constructs based on physiological indices (one-to-one relation) is still unresolved and is not the aim of this review (see further discussion in section “Research Applicability in Real-World Settings”). However, we discuss how multiple measures (that are sensitive to several interrelated internal states) may be combined to delineate net resulting changes across multiple inter-related cognitive state-level variations. Second, for each measure, we make the distinction between useful research measures and practical measures for real-world application (see section Research Applicability in Real-World Settings and Table 2). Throughout, we have tried to highlight the practical relevance of measures in the driving context. Although this review focuses primarily on on-road and simulated driving contexts, when relevant, we have also drawn research from related contexts (traffic operators, pilots, or ship navigators) to more thoroughly characterize each measure.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Overview of relationships between arousal state and physiological indices in real-world driving.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Tentative framework for considering the research applicability of these measures in lab and real-world settings.

Psychophysiological Measures to Assess Cognitive States

Electroencephalogram (EEG) and Event-Related Potentials (ERP)

EEG Quantification

The EEG is a record of both oscillatory and aperiodic brain electrical activity. Neural activity (largely post-synaptic potentials) from multiple simultaneous generators propagate throughout the brain and skull and summate at a distance, where voltages can be measured relatively non-invasively via electrodes placed on the scalp. The dominant sources of scalp-recorded EEG come from cortical pyramidal cells arranged in the columnar organization of the cortex (Nunez and Srinivasan, 2006). Pyramidal cells are the most numerous cortical excitatory cell type and play a critical role in advanced cognitive functions (Spruston, 2008). The laminar organization of the cortex results in cortical pyramidal cells following an open-field alignment with a consistent orientation that is perpendicular to the skull, such that their post-synaptic potentials can summate at a distance. Importantly, EEG allows for a high temporal resolution (millisecond) and direct record of neural activity. This detailed temporal resolution also allows for a decomposition of the time-domain EEG signal into spectral information via Fourier analysis, allowing for an examination of oscillatory activity in canonical frequency bands (e.g., alpha, ~8–12 Hz; theta, ~4–7 Hz), which have been related to specific neurocognitive functions. For instance, mental workload increases theta power and reduce alpha power activity (Mun et al., 2017), whereas fatigue increases alpha power (Käthner et al., 2014). Moreover, the development of novel computational techniques for analyzing spectral activity has promoted a wide range of new tools for probing ongoing neural dynamics during human cognition via EEG; such as cross-frequency coupling, phase coupling (Cohen, 2011), independent component analysis (Dasari et al., 2017), and neighborhood component analysis (Lim et al., 2018). In addition, more traditional analyses of transient neural activity that is tied to specific perceptual, motor, or cognitive events can be gleaned from continuous EEG, via the calculation of event-related brain potentials.

ERP Quantification

ERPs are electrophysiological responses that are consistently linked in time with specific sensory, cognitive, or motor events. They are derived from the continuously recorded EEG by time-aligning epochs of EEG relative to an event of interest, such as a stimulus onset or a participant's response and averaging many of these similar EEG segments to reveal activity that is time and phase locked to the event. Such discrete events can be added in the experimental design, e.g., every time a participant responded to a secondary task while driving. The logic of this approach is that systematic activity that is locked in time and space to some specific activity will remain in the averaged ERP waveform, whereas activity that is not time- and phase-locked will average to zero with a large enough number of trials (Luck and Kappenman, 2012). The resulting ERP waveform is plotted as voltage over time at a given set of electrodes. ERP topography can also be examined, showing the distribution of activity over the entire space within a particular time-window. A major benefit of ERPs is that the waveform has characteristic components, stereotyped features of the ERP with specific eliciting conditions. ERP components are defined empirically by a combination of their polarity, timing, scalp distribution, and sensitivity to task manipulations.

Extensive work has characterized and validated specific ERP components with respect to their associations with specific cognitive and neural processes (e.g., Fabiani et al., 2007; Luck and Kappenman, 2012; Mun et al., 2017). Cognitive demands can modulate several ERP components, such as P3 (discussed below; Käthner et al., 2014), mismatch negativity (MMN is a negative ERP component sensitive to pre-attentive information processing; Wanyan et al., 2018), and late positive potentials amplitude (a later ERP component like P6 that is related to attentional allocation similar to P3; Mun et al., 2017). The P3 component is associated with attentional and memory processes required to detect any changes in incoming stimuli-related information (Polich, 2007). The canonical P3 has two distinct but related components – the P3a and P3b (see Polich, 2007 for a review). The P3a, with an anterior distribution, is associated with novel stimulus-driven attentional processing or orienting responses. The P3b, with a centro-posterior distribution, is associated with task-relevant stimulus-driven attentional, decision making, and subsequent memory processing (Polich, 2007). Both components have been used in driving research. Recent work has also examined how neural indices (as measured by both P3 ERP components) are associated with subjective workload (as measured by NASA-TLX) and how this covariation is influenced by cognitive effort (Yakobi, 2018). Novel techniques (such as intra-block averaging of ERP amplitudes; Horat et al., 2016) can enable robust electrophysiological measurement of cognitive demands over time. Thus, ERPs are an attractive measure for studying cognitive states and performance in driving contexts.

EEG/ERP in Driving Context

EEG and ERPs have a long history in the study of the neural indices of cognitive effort and attention allocation in both laboratory and applied settings. EEG is perhaps one of the most widely used neurophysiological methods to study driving behavior. Several frequencies (e.g., power in alpha frequency band) and time (e.g., P3) domain indices can reliably measure changes in cognitive demands (Käthner et al., 2014). This makes EEG is viable measure for applied driving settings.

Over-arousal in driving context

Over-aroused states, such as increased workload while driving can be indexed by decreases in alpha power and increases in theta power (Borghini et al., 2014; Käthner et al., 2014). A recent study found alpha band power to be higher during the relaxed condition compared with the engaged condition in an autonomous driving setting (Zander et al., 2017). This highlights the sensitivity of alpha power to internal factors such as attentional engagement. In addition to internal factors, external factors (such as task load and time on task) can also influence alpha and theta power bands in opposite directions (Wascher et al., 2018). For instance, a decrease in task load and time on task led to an increase in relative alpha power, but a decrease in theta power (Getzmann et al., 2018; Wascher et al., 2018). To account for both power bands, past work has also used a ratio of frontal theta and parietal alpha power spectral density to operationalize workload in pilots (Borghini et al., 2015). This ratio approach may be relevant for driving research as well, however this has been a point of debate, as discussed shortly.

The application of known ERP indicators of attentional workload (and their eliciting tasks) can be successfully translated into the driving domain as well. One of the most commonly adopted components in driving research is the P3b (Brookhuis and de Waard, 2010; Solís-Marcos and Kircher, 2018). Mental workload can be indexed by increases in P3b latencies (Ying et al., 2011) and amplitude (Strayer and Drews, 2007). For example, Strayer and Drews (2007) examined the amplitude of the P3b time-locked to the onset of a pace break light under single-task driving conditions or dual-tasking via cell-phone–induced distraction. Drawing on basic experimental work that has shown that the P3b is sensitive to the degree of attention allocated to a task (e.g., Sirevaag et al., 1989), they also showed that cell-phone induced distraction resulted in reduced P3b amplitudes to brake lights. Similar effects have been observed in comparing the workload of “single-task” driving in laboratory simulator vs. real-life driving contexts, where for example, the diversion of attention to other concurrent activities in the vehicle result in additional attentional demands in real-world driving (Strayer et al., 2015).

A recent study compared mental workload due to increased information processing demands consumed by in-vehicle information systems (Solís-Marcos and Kircher, 2018). They found both P3b and N1 latencies and amplitudes to be sensitive to cognitive demands of processing additional in-vehicle information systems. For instance, P3b amplitudes decreased with additional information processing related tasks (Solís-Marcos and Kircher, 2018). P3a amplitude was also found to decrease with additional task-related load (Getzmann et al., 2018). High mental workload has been associated with increased latencies in MMN during driving (Ying et al., 2011) and also increased frontal MMN in flight simulation tasks (Wanyan et al., 2018), however a recent study did not find workload to influence MMN amplitudes (Getzmann et al., 2018). Future work will help clarify sensitivity of MMN in driving research.

Under-arousal in driving context

Extensive work has focused on electrophysiological indicators of under-arousal via EEG. A substantial number of papers have implicated changes in alpha amplitude during fatigued driving (e.g., Schier, 2000; Jensen and Mazaheri, 2010; Simon et al., 2011; Zhao et al., 2012; Borghini et al., 2014; Jagannath and Balasubramanian, 2014; Arnau et al., 2017; Brouwer et al., 2017), such that fatigued driving is associated with increased alpha activity. However, other work has challenged these alpha power links with fatigue and claim that alpha power changes may be due to the decreases in task-demands and visual input during monotonous driving tasks and not due to decline in cognitive processing abilities (Wascher et al., 2014). Increases in relative alpha band power with increased time on task, easier driving route, and lower control of driving situations, which suggested that relative alpha power increases imply attentional withdrawal and not fatigue (Wascher et al., 2014, 2018). Wascher et al. (2014, 2018) have argued that mid-frontal theta activity may be a more appropriate neural marker of cognitive-control related processes in driving than occipital alpha activity. Low task load is associated with relatively reduced theta activity, which suggests that theta activity is sensitive to declines in cognitive processing ability. Instead of alpha activity, Wascher et al. (2014, 2018) recommend that indices of oscillatory synchronization (e.g., inter-trial phase clustering) and ERPs (such as P3a) are more reliable and valid indices of changes in cognitive state associated with mental fatigue. For instance, time on task (Wascher et al., 2014), fatigue (Massar et al., 2010), and decreases in vigilance over time (Schmidt et al., 2009) were found to reduce P3a amplitude while driving. Similarly, mind-wandering during driving is associated with a reduction in P3a amplitude (Baldwin et al., 2017). One other study found both P3a and P3b components' amplitudes were reduced due to driving-related fatigue (Guoping and Zhang, 2009). These findings show that ERP components could be utilized to detect variations in neurophysiological arousal due to interrelated cognitive constructs in driving contexts.

Some researchers have argued that LF/HF ratios (e.g., frontal theta/beta) are potential biomarkers for attentional control, and have established some evidence that such measures have good psychometric properties, for e.g., test-retest reliability (Putman et al., 2014; Angelidis et al., 2016). Decreases in beta power (e.g., Zhao et al., 2012; Jagannath and Balasubramanian, 2014) have been found, along with changes in theta and delta activity as markers related to transition to fatigue. This has led some researchers to propose spectral ratio indices (e.g., alpha/beta; Eoh et al., 2005; Wang et al., 2018), as biomarkers of alertness. However, ratio indices have also been criticized for being an inadequate method because it combines frequency bands with distinct topographic specificity that change differently over time (Wascher et al., 2014). There is existing criticism of this ratio approach, especially in driving research (Wascher et al., 2018), and more broadly, researchers in cognitive electrophysiology have been moving away from such highly constrained “band-based” approaches given their lack of replicability across studies. Alternatively, researchers have increasingly endorsed methods that allow for broad-band assessment of spectral dynamics (e.g., 1/f scaling, Voytek and Knight, 2015) and methods that can address narrow-band dynamics without a priori selection of frequency (e.g., cluster-based permutation testing in time-frequency data; Maris and Oostenveld, 2007). Other recent work has used EEG-based detection algorithms to detect fatigue and drowsiness (Li et al., 2017; Morales et al., 2017; Belakhdar et al., 2018; Gao et al., 2018; Wei et al., 2018). However, other work reported no additional benefit of utilizing EEG measures in drowsiness and fatigue detection in sleep deprivation contexts (Perrier et al., 2016; Liang et al., 2017). Another line of work has aimed to apply machine-learning techniques to brain computing interfaces in order to classify states of drowsiness and fatigue in real-time (e.g., Lin et al., 2005; Correa et al., 2014). Recent work has also shown data filtering and processing techniques such as artifact subspace reconstruction and independent component analysis could be utilized for “online” processing of EEG data collected while driving in order to attenuate movement-and noise-related artifacts (Krol et al., 2017). Together, these findings suggest that EEG and ERPs can be utilized as objective techniques to assess state-level variations in cognitive demands.

Practical Considerations

There are a number of important considerations when applying EEG indices to real-world driving environments. Typical EEG artifacts arising from muscle-and-eye movements (de Waard, 1996; Zander et al., 2017), impedance shifts, environmental line (60 Hz) noise, and other complications are potentially amplified in real-world environments. As such, real-time monitoring of good quality EEG signals is critical for effective data collection. The commercial introduction of high-impedance systems with active electrodes and small electrically shielded mobile EEG amplifiers has spawned a large increase in real-world EEG applications. Many of these systems are capable of high density (<128 channel) recording, but it is critical for the researcher to decide whether and to what degree an increase in the number of channels may result in a decrease in the quality of the recorded EEG (Luck and Kappenman, 2012). Importantly, the well-understood limitations of the spatial resolution of EEG limit the utility of high-density recording in ecologically valid environments (e.g., where measurement of EEG sensors co-localized in 3D space on a single-subject basis may be unfeasible). Moreover, with increasing channel density comes increases in the likelihood for poorly recorded or poorly monitored channels during recording. As such, if source-localization of underlying EEG/ERP generators is not a primary aim of the methodology (and we expect, in most applied cases it would not be), researchers may wish to record from a smaller density (e.g., 32 channels or fewer), at the benefit of better monitoring of data quality throughout the experiment.

On the theoretical side—researchers in human factors automotive research should carefully consider the linking hypotheses between specific electrophysiological indicators (e.g., P3b ERP amplitude, alpha power increases) and their purported cognitive interpretations. The ERP literature has a massive basic literature in which specific components have been very well-characterized relative to their eliciting conditions and underlying cognitive interpretations (Luck and Kappenman, 2012). One such example was reviewed earlier on characterizing the P3b under different states of distraction during driving. Limited work (e.g., Strayer et al., 2015) has attempted to examine ERP components in naturalistic settings. In future work, inventive approaches can be validated to use task-related responses or behaviors (such as eye-blink potentials or frequent vs. infrequent vehicle cues) as discrete events that can be recorded to estimate ERP components in real-world settings. At the same time, such characterizations in the spectral domain are not as clearly developed to date. However, this is changing, as basic research in cognitive electrophysiology shifts toward a more complete understanding of oscillatory mechanisms underlying human perception and cognition (e.g., Kahana, 2006), involving development in standardized analysis methods (Cohen, 2011), careful experimental characterization of specific oscillatory markers (e.g., alpha phase and perception, Mathewson et al., 2009; midline frontal theta and conflict resolution; Cavanagh and Frank, 2014), and the development of neurophysiologically guided models (Jensen and Mazaheri, 2010; Voytek and Knight, 2015). We expect that such development of basic research findings in cognitive electrophysiology will be a great asset in future applied research in contexts such as driving.

Optical Imaging for Cerebral Blood Flow

Optical Imaging Quantification

Optical imaging methods allow for the visualization of the interaction of photons with tissues (Villringer et al., 1993). In recent years, there has been a rapid advancement in the application of non-invasive optical imaging methods such as functional near infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) to study human brain and cognitive functioning. fNIRS is a neuroimaging method based on the principles of near-infrared spectroscopy, which was originally developed in humans for investigating clinical features of brain functioning (e.g., cerebral oxygenation; Jobsis, 1977). These principles have been extended to measure local changes in cerebral hemodynamic activity that can be used to infer information on the underlying neural activity due to neurovascular coupling, following similar logic to the Blood Oxygen Level Dependent (BOLD) signal in functional magnetic resonance imaging. NIR (700–1,000 nm) light is able to penetrate several centimeters through the skull and into brain tissue, allowing for non-invasive measurement of certain optical properties of cortical tissue. For example, changes in the concentration of oxy- and deoxy-hemoglobin can be measured via NIRS because oxy- and deoxy-hemoglobin have distinct absorption spectra that correspond to the different coloration of arterial and venous blood (Grinvald et al., 1986). These absorption characteristics make it possible to use a spectroscopic approach to measure changes in the concentration of oxy- and deoxy- hemoglobin as a function of neural activity, for example during cognitive task performance. In typical optical imaging systems, optical fibers, called optodes or sources, carry NIR light to the scalp while other optical fibers, called detectors, collect the photons as they emerge from the scalp. Each source–detector pair is a single channel. Multi-channel and wearable fNIRS systems have become commercially available with diverse montages capable of measuring brain activity across the entire scalp.

Optical Imaging for Cerebral Blood Flow in Driving Context

The application of fNIRS in driving research is in its infancy. Nevertheless, a number of interesting demonstrations of the utility of fNIRS for studying over-arousal states such as driver workload have emerged (e.g., Tsunashima and Yanagisawa, 2009; Liu et al., 2012, 2016; Sibi et al., 2016). For example, increases in oxygenated hemoglobin have been reported during simulated driving tasks under cognitive load compared to control conditions (Liu et al., 2012). A recent study (Unni et al., 2017) utilized fNIRS in a naturalistic driving simulator while doing a secondary task (modified version of 0–4 back). They found systematic increases in bilateral inferior frontal and temporo-occipital brain regions with increments in workload. Another study reported that fNIRS could be used to differentiate between low vs. high workload (n-back task) related hemodynamic activity in the prefrontal cortex while motorists drove in a realistic driving simulator (Herff et al., 2017). Furthermore, fNIRS have been used to monitor pilot's task engagement and working memory load in real-time (Gateau et al., 2015). On a related note, fNIRS have been found sensitive to increase in task difficulty in flight simulators (Causse et al., 2017) as indicated by an increased concentration of oxygenated hemoglobin and a decreased deoxygenated hemoglobin.

Other work has investigated effects of under-arousal related states with fNIRS. Research has related decreases in hemodynamic measures of cerebral oxygenation with fatigue in simulated driving (Li et al., 2009), and findings have been extended into actual highway driving (Yoshino et al., 2013). An increase in fatigue can be indexed by a decrease in cerebral oxygenation and mental stress can be indexed by an increase in cerebral oxygenation. Tsunashima and Yanagisawa (2009) examined changes in prefrontal activity via multi-channel frontal fNIRS systems in driving with and without adaptive cruise control. Their findings revealed substantial decreases in prefrontal activity when participants drove with adaptive cruise control relative to without, which was correlated with perceived workload (via the NASA-TLX). Similar decreases in activation of prefrontal cortex (lower cognitive load associated with drowsiness) were reported while participants monitored a simulated autonomous car driving task relative to higher prefrontal cortex activation during manual driving task (Sibi et al., 2016). Such findings indicate that optical imaging for cerebral blood flow is a valuable tool for assessing performance and neural efficiency in well-controlled realistic driving contexts.

Practical Considerations

One important limitation of fNIRS is that, because it relies on the measurement of absorption properties of light as a function of vascular changes in the brain, its temporal resolution is limited by the time-course of hemodynamic activity (on the order of seconds). In contrast, the development of recent ‘fast’ optical imaging methods, such as the event-related optical signal (EROS; Gratton and Fabiani, 2001, 2003), which measures scattering properties of light as a function of changes in neural activity, have a much higher temporal resolution (on the order of milliseconds). Although applications of this method in human factors research is sparse, fast optical imaging methods have growing promise. While the spatial resolution of optical imaging methods is higher than EEG, such spatial inference is constrained by the penetration depth of NIR light, which reaches only a few cm from the scalp surface. Therefore, imaging of activity from deep cortical and subcortical sources (beyond the outer cortical mantle) is limited. Recent work has also employed wearable fNIRS systems (Piper et al., 2014; McKendrick et al., 2016; Le et al., 2018) and simultaneous collection of fNIRS and EEG (Kassab et al., 2018), which can enable real-world monitoring in ecologically valid settings.

Heart Rate (HR) and Heart Rate Variability (HRV)

Heart Activity Quantification

Heart rate (in beats per minute or bpm) is the number of heartbeats in 1 min (Jennings et al., 1981). Electrocardiography (ECG) is a well-established method to record the electrical activity of the heart. In psychophysiology, a lead II configuration (i.e., placing the negative electrode in the region of right collar bone, the ground near the left collar bone, and the positive lead over the lower left ribcage, or functionally similar variant) is commonly used to be able to record electrical activity of the heart via research grade equipment. A single heart beat wave in an ECG signal shows changes in electrical potentials (referred to as the P, Q, R, S, & T components and together they are referred to as the QRS complex, for review please see Berntson et al., 2007). The R component (one for each heart beat) is due to ventricular depolarization and for a lead II configuration, it has a larger magnitude and a sharper inflection than the rest of the components making it easily detectable. While heart rate is a count of beat per minute, heart period (also called inter-beat-interval) is the time in milliseconds between successive R spikes (Berntson et al., 2007). Heart rate is generally derived by converting mean heart period (in milliseconds) to heart rate (in beats per minute), see Berntson et al. (2007).

Heart data can also be collected via other technique including photoelectric plethysmography (PPG) and photoplethysmography imaging (PPGI). PPG technique includes use of a photocell (such as an infrared light-emitting diode) placed over an area of tissue with blood capillaries that is easily accessible (e.g., finger or ear lobe). Energy emitted from an infrared source passes through the tissue and reflects off the tissue. Changes in blood volume (due to heart beats) in an area can thus be assessed by the amount of light that was reflected back to the photodetector, and thus forms the basis of estimating heart beats (Berntson et al., 2007; Laborde et al., 2017). A similar concept is used in “wearables” which have photo-emitters and detectors placed on a convenient location (e.g., wrists and earlobes) making them easy to wear and collect data from them (Byrom et al., 2018; van Gent et al., 2018). This idea is used in vehicles with photo-emitters and detectors placed on the steering wheels, which allow collecting heart data (heart rate, HRV, and blood volume pulse) while driving. Another advancement in PPG is a contactless measurement technique called PPGI that detects color changes (e.g., the forehead area) in a video due to blood perfusions (Blöcher et al., 2017). Instead of photodiodes used in PPG, PPGI uses detector arrays in cameras to collect image sequences that contain information about bio-signals (e.g., blood volume pulse and respiration). Image and signal processing methods are utilized for beat-to-beat heart rate estimation (Blöcher et al., 2017; Madan et al., 2018).

On a related note, established guidelines for heart beat detection processing, with recommended parameters to derive heart rate and heart rate variability are provided in Jennings et al. (1981), Berntson et al. (2007), and Shaffer and Ginsberg (2017). Custom and open-source software has also been developed to automatically detect R peaks to calculate heart beats. As is true for most physiological measures, data should be visually checked to inspect the ECG data for artifacts and irregularities. Artifacts can be introduced in these data due to numerous reasons (such as motorists' excessive motion, sneezing and coughing, and irregular heartbeats) any of which can disrupt the ECG measurement or directly impact normal heart-beat patterns. Visual inspection helps insure that the heart beats are correctly marked by the detection software and physiologically improbable values are detected and then corrected.

HRV is variability in the time intervals of adjacent heartbeats (Berntson et al., 2007; Shaffer and Ginsberg, 2017). HRV can be derived from ECG data over a period of time ranging from short intervals (~1–5 min) up to longer intervals (~24 h). HRV metrics can be roughly categorized as falling under time-domain, frequency-domain, or non-linear measures of HRV (for a review see Shaffer and Ginsberg, 2017). Time domain-based parameters calculate the variations in heart beat intervals, such as standard deviation of R-R intervals (SDRR), percentage of successive R-R intervals that differ by more than 50 ms (pNN50), and root mean square of successive R-R intervals (RMSSD). A few time-domain parameters also represent geometric shape of R-R interval distributions, such as the HRV triangular index (i.e., plotting the integral of the ratio of RR interval density histogram by its height) and the baseline width of the RR intervals histogram (TINN), for details see Shaffer and Ginsberg (2017). Frequency-domain based measures transform the beat-to-beat variations in heart beat (R-R intervals) into frequency power bands via Fourier analysis (Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology, 1996). The most commonly used frequency-domain methods are low- and high-frequency power. A low-frequency (LF) power is the energy of heart rate oscillations in a lower-frequency (0.04–0.15 Hz) band. Similarly, high-frequency (HF) power is the energy of heart rate oscillations in a higher-frequency (0.15–0.4 Hz) band (Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology, 1996; Shaffer and Ginsberg, 2017). A peak in these frequency bands can also be calculated, which is an estimate of the peak frequency in the specific frequency band. Non-linear measures of HRV are useful in capturing the unpredictability and dynamic nature of heart rate time-series data (Shaffer and Ginsberg, 2017). Common measures include fitting an elliptical-shape to represent non-linear HRV and calculating approximate entropy (ApEn) and sample entropy (SmpEn), which characterize the complex pattern of time-series heart data (Shaffer and Ginsberg, 2017). Detailed discussions can be found elsewhere (Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology, 1996; Berntson et al., 2007; Laborde et al., 2017; Shaffer and Ginsberg, 2017).

HR/HRV in Driving Context

Over-arousal in driving context

Heart rate is a commonly measured index of physiological arousal in response to changes in driving demands. One of the most studied over-aroused cognitive states is workload. Numerous studies have examined changes in heart rate as a function of workload (Lenneman and Backs, 2009, 2010; Mehler et al., 2012; Heine et al., 2017). Heart rate was also found to increase while performing visual and auditory dual-tasks relative to single-task of driving in a simulator (Lenneman and Backs, 2009). Similarly, heart rate has been shown to be incrementally higher for systematically more difficult auditory dual-tasks while driving in a simulator (Mehler et al., 2009) as well as while driving on-road (Reimer et al., 2009). These findings of an incremental change in heart have been replicated in younger-aged (20–29 years old), middle-aged (40–49 years old), and older-aged (60–69 years old) adults (Mehler et al., 2012). Thus, heart rate increases with workload due to cognitive demand (Lenneman and Backs, 2009; Mehler et al., 2012; Ruscio et al., 2017; Hidalgo-Muñoz et al., 2018; c.f., Engström et al., 2005). Other efforts have also been made to utilize rhythmic and morphological parameters of a heart activity to explore mental workload. A recent study examined the influence of mental workload (due to a secondary task) on morphological parameters from ECG while completing a lane change task (Heine et al., 2017). They found that a combination of derived HR and HRV features (such as mean HR, RMSSD, pNN50, etc.) could be extracted from ECG data that could distinguish between workload levels and suggest that a combination of ECG features can be used to detect mental workload (for details see Heine et al., 2017).

Relative to HR, a fewer number of studies have examined HRV, especially in a systematic manner. HRV decreases with increasing task demands (Luque-Casado et al., 2016). HRV has been found to be sensitive to variations in attention levels while driving that may not be necessarily evident in driving performance (Lenneman and Backs, 2009) and thus HRV can have more sensitivity than behavioral measures. LF- and HF-HRV power bands are influenced by driving task (Zhao et al., 2012; Tozman et al., 2015; Wang et al., 2018). A study (Tozman et al., 2015) compared effect of demand levels (boredom, average demand, and high demand) on HRV in a driving simulator. Both LF- and HF-HRV varied for all the three conditions. High task demands reduced both LF-HRV and HF-HRV (Tozman et al., 2015). Some work has indicated that stress-inducing real-world driving tasks lead to increased heart rate and decreased SDNN, RMSSD, pNN50 (Lee et al., 2007). HRV also varies with workload experienced by drivers during simulated driving (Zhao et al., 2012; Heine et al., 2017; Hidalgo-Muñoz et al., 2018) and on-road driving (Lee et al., 2007). In addition, HRV variations due to cognitive workload have also been found in city traffic operators (Fallahi et al., 2016) and unmanned aerial vehicles operators (Jasper et al., 2016). HRV is sensitive to workload increases due to vigilance and situational awareness demands of the task (Saus et al., 2001; Stuiver et al., 2014; Jasper et al., 2016). However, at least one study (Shakouri et al., 2018) found no variation in heart rate variability metrics (RMSSD, LF, HF, and LF/HF ratio) as a function of higher traffic density while driving in a simulator, even though variations in subjective workload were found.

Under-arousal in driving context

HR and HRV are also sensitive to low-arousal states, such as vigilance and drowsiness. Decreases in vigilance over the course of a 3-h continuous driving task were indexed by a significant drop in heart rate over time (Schmidt et al., 2009). Drowsiness experienced in car drivers and aircraft pilots can also be associated with decreases in HR (Borghini et al., 2014). A recent on-road study (Biondi et al., 2018) found that driving a Tesla in semi-automated mode (e.g., autopilot) led to a lower heart rate relative to manual driving on a freeway. Another study found heart rate was sensitive to activity of the Adaptive Cruise Control (ACC) technology (Brouwer et al., 2017). Heart rate increased when ACC decelerated more suddenly compared to instances when the car decelerated more gradually (Brouwer et al., 2017). These findings suggest that heart rate is a sensitive measure that can assess cognitive processing pertaining to advanced technology in semi-autonomous vehicles.

Other studies have found that LF-HRV and HF-HRV vary with fatigue (Liang et al., 2009; Sugie et al., 2016). A recent study (Wang et al., 2018) found that changes in fatigue levels while driving can be represented by non-linear measures of HRV (e.g., sample entropy). Variations in drowsiness levels can also impact HRV (Noda et al., 2015; Piotrowski and Szypulska, 2017). Another recent study found that variations in HRV (TINN and RMSSD) was higher when participants drove a vehicle in automated mode relative to the manual mode (Biondi et al., 2018). Perhaps, drowsiness and a lack of engagement in the driving task during automated mode may have led to a higher HRV. HRV and blink rates have also been shown to assess sleep onset (Noda et al., 2015). HRV-based assessment algorithms can be used for early detection of fatigue and drowsiness to augment attention and performance (Patel et al., 2011; Zhao et al., 2012; Abe et al., 2016; Vicente et al., 2016).

Practical Considerations

Heart rate and its variability are inexpensive and reliable measures that are relatively easy to record with research-quality equipment that meets recommended guidelines (Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology, 1996). It has good signal to noise ratio as well (R-R peaks can be detected even in very noisy environments). Consequently, it is also not difficult to collect in lab as well as in unpredictable field studies, especially with the availability of mobile data recording systems. However, these advantages can also lead to misuse of this methodology. Great attention to the data collection and processing are required to have meaningful data. Skin preparation (e.g., cleaning with alcohol wipes) before electrode placement and signal monitoring to collect good quality data can drastically reduce post-processing (e.g., Berntson et al., 2007). Participants should be comfortably positioned to avoid physiologically induced changes in heart rate such as altered breathing rate due to postural adjustments. Body movements should be minimized and accounted for as such movements can add noise and also add movement-related heart rate changes. Effective data cleaning to remove artifacts and noise are a must, otherwise heart data will be uninterpretable.

Some recording devices do not utilize the traditional QRS complex from an ECG to calculate HR and HRV. For example, PPG uses a photoelectric sensor that estimates changes in blood volume to calculate HR. There are a few methodological challenges that should be considered before adopting such PPG-based systems. PPG records a lagged cardiac response further away from the heart (e.g., from fingers and earlobes). Unlike ECG based estimates that have a sharp spike for the R component, PPG-based methods instead show a less pronounced curved peak of the blood volume pulse signal, which makes accurate and automatic detection of heart period relatively more difficult (Laborde et al., 2017). Moreover, ECG-based estimates of HR and HRV are recommended for more reliable results because it allows visual inspection and artifact correction of heart data. Such methodological differences between PPG and ECG can explain why PPG and ECG findings are comparable during rest, but are not comparable during stress, for example (Schäfer and Vagedes, 2013).

On a related note, commercialized equipment meant for exercise and fitness tracking fail to meet established guidelines for heart data collection and processing (e.g., minimum sampling rate and access to raw data for necessary artifact correction methods), which are necessary to make meaningful interpretations (see Berntson et al., 2007; Quintana et al., 2016; Shaffer and Ginsberg, 2017). Similarly, smartphone camera-based assessments have methodological challenges, including very poor sampling rate, illumination variation (due to confounds like weather and time of day), poor signal-to-noise ratio, and motion-related artifacts that can lead to inaccurate interpretations (Laborde et al., 2017; cf., Nowara et al., 2018; van Gent et al., 2018). Ensuring the validity and inter-device variability of wearables (which utilize a PPG-based or camera-based HR system) with an established ECG-based equipment is a necessary step to be able to validate data collected from wearables. However, most commercialized equipment has not been validated in such a manner (Quintana et al., 2016). Without this critical validation step, data collected from commercialized non-research grade equipment does not have convergent validity and should be discouraged by the scientific community until such standards are met. While innovation is critical to be able to collect psychophysiological data in real-world settings, careful adoption and cross-checks with existing gold standards are necessary to make meaningful progress in the adoption of these technologies in real-world driving research.

Moreover, HF-HRV has been found to be impacted by parasympathetic nervous system, however, LF-HRV is influenced by both sympathetic and parasympathetic nervous systems (Berntson et al., 2007; Laborde et al., 2017). Thus, LF-HRV should not be described as a metric of sympathetic activity, but instead be interpreted as a mixture of sympathetic and parasympathetic influences. On a related note, the LF/HF ratio has been a controversial metric as it assumes that LF is due to sympathetic activity while HF is due to parasympathetic (Billman, 2013). The LF/HF ratio was originally based on 24 h recordings, while shorter duration recordings (even 5 min long) have also been calculated. The duration of recording (e.g., 5 min vs. 24 h) can also lead to uncorrelated findings and some metrics are better for short term recordings than others (Shaffer and Ginsberg, 2017).

Another metric we would like to highlight is heart period. Heart rate and heart period have been used interchangeably, however in some instances heart period may be a better choice. Even though, heart rate is more commonly used metric, use of heart period instead of heart rate is recommended measure of autonomic activity because heart period changes more linearly over time (Quigley and Berntson, 1996; Berntson et al., 2007). Heart period should specially be used when comparing changes in heart activity due to experimental manipulation or due to between group differences for short time periods. Further information on heart activity related metrics can be found in detailed reviews (Jennings et al., 1981; Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology, 1996; Berntson et al., 2007; Laborde et al., 2017; Shaffer and Ginsberg, 2017).

Not all heart-based metrices may be sensitive to the variations in cognitive state during driving task. For instance, a study compared several commonly used metrices for HR and HRV cognitive workload during highway driving (Mehler et al., 2011). While HR was robust in differentiating between cognitive workload in single vs. dual tasks, HRV indices were less robust (e.g., smaller effect sizes). A few HRV indices varied with workload (RMSSD, SDSD, and LF power), however others (SDNN, NN50, pNN50, HF power, and LF/HF) did not significantly differ with workload (Mehler et al., 2011). These findings suggest that depending upon the task, certain indices may be more sensitive to variation in cognitive state than other indices that may be less robust.

In addition, researchers should consider other contextual factors that may vary across participants and may confound study interpretations. A confounding factor that can potentially bias HF-HRV comparisons between conditions of interest is differences in respiration (Grossman, 1992; Berntson et al., 2007; Laborde et al., 2017). Respiration related-parameters should be accounted for by using them as covariates with such HRV indices (for a detailed discussion, see Berntson et al., 2007; Laborde et al., 2017). Similarly, other factors may impact HR/HRV, including task characteristics and motorists' state (relaxation, engagement, and motivation) and activities (smoking and posture). For instance, HRV may increase over time if the task becomes less difficult over time, which may put motorists in a more relaxed state (Jasper et al., 2016). Similarly, HRV may also increase over time with disengagement or demotivation to perform a difficult task (Jasper et al., 2016). Careful consideration of contextual factors will afford accurate and reliable measurement of HR/HRV indices in applied driving settings.

Blood Pressure (BP)

BP Quantification

BP (in millimeters of mercury, also written as mmHg) is the force exerted against the walls of the blood vessels (Shapiro et al., 1996; Berntson et al., 2007). Depending upon the stage of the dynamic cardiac cycle, BP differs from lowest to highest levels. During a single cardiac cycle, diastolic BP is the lowest level of arterial pressure when the heart is filled with blood and systolic BP is relatively the highest level of arterial pressure (Shapiro et al., 1996; Berntson et al., 2007). As invasive methods to record BP require additional safeguards and equipment, most psychophysiology research studies focus on non-invasive approaches to record blood pressure. Three relatively non-invasive methods are auscultatory or oscillometric methods, arterial tonometry, or the volume-clamp methods (see for details, Berntson et al., 2007). The most common method is auscultatory measurement, which records the sounds of blood flow by placing a cuff on the upper arm and a stethoscope placed over the brachial artery to identify the systolic and diastolic blood pressure (Shapiro et al., 1996; Berntson et al., 2007). Physiological arousal during mentally effortful situations leads to greater vasoconstriction and cardiovascular reactivity evidenced by increased heart rate and blood pressure and decreased heart rate variability (Lundberg et al., 1994; Ottaviani et al., 2016). BP increases with psychological stress (Ottaviani et al., 2016) and is correlated with self-reported stress (Lundberg et al., 1994). However, cognitive workload may not reliably influence BP (ElKomy et al., 2017).

BP in Driving Context

Limited research has examined over- and under-arousal via BP in driving contexts. Systolic BP and BP variability have been found to increase while driving in simulated high traffic conditions that had high workload demands (Stuiver et al., 2014). Fatigue was also associated with a decrease in systolic BP and HR (Liang et al., 2009). However, other studies have not found a reliable effect of stress on BP (Simonson et al., 1968; Littler et al., 1973; Lee et al., 2007). One study found no significant change in BP from beginning to end of the drive with a short period of arterial pressure changes during events such as overtaking that returned to baseline (Littler et al., 1973). BP was also not found to vary in an on-road stressful driving task speed in a simulator even though HRV parameters were significantly impacted (Lee et al., 2007).

Nevertheless, BP is a very useful measure to understand the factors that impact driving performance. One clear example of this comes from a simulator-based study investigating aggressive driving behavior in irregular traffic flow and under time pressure (Drews et al., 2012). Irregular traffic patterns were not found to impact BP. However, male drivers who were under time pressure to drive faster in order to receive a monetary incentive, had elevated systolic BP compared to females under time pressure or compared to male drivers who were not under time pressure. In fact, females did not show any elevated blood pressure under time pressure (Drews et al., 2012). These findings suggest that individual difference factors such as sex differences and motivation to drive aggressively may impact driving behavior and associated physiological signals. Other studies have shown that trait-level variation in BP (such as a history of high BP i.e., hypertension) is an important measure to capture health and age-related impact on driving performance in vulnerable older populations (Lyman et al., 2001; Siren et al., 2004). A 5-year longitudinal study that examined the effect of urban bus driving on BP found that the number of hours driven per week predicted higher diastolic BP (Johansson et al., 2012), suggesting that there are cumulative effects of cognitive demands and stress of continuous driving.

Practical Considerations

While heart-rate was reported to rapidly change in response to car racing, BP was “less responsive” (Simonson et al., 1968). Other studies have found that BP does not change significantly during on-road driving (Littler et al., 1973; Lee et al., 2007). A few BP recording-related reasons could play a role. BP can rapidly change over time so multiple readings are recommended for a more accurate estimate. However, a limiting factor is the BP equipment. The pressure from a cuff worn by the responder can become uncomfortable and disruptive within a few minutes. Continuous reliable BP measurement (especially via volume-clamp) is uncomfortable, distracting, and potentially disruptive to driving. This limits the frequency of samples that could be collected, which are about 1 reading per minute. Also, the BP recordings are sensitive to movement so in an on-road study, it is less feasible to accurately record multiple BP reading from participants while drivers are actively involved in the driving process. While some alternative methods to record blood pressure (e.g., plethysmography) may be available, methodological issues similar to those discussed in recording heart activity apply to BP as well and it is crucial to evade poor quality unreliable equipment. In sum, BP provides valuable insights about vulnerable states of the drivers, however, in a real-world driving context, methodological concerns can limit reliable data collection. Much future work is required to be able to measure reliable and non-invasive BP activity.

Electrodermal Activity (EDA)

EDA Quantification

EDA, previously known as galvanic skin response, is a change in electrical potentials of the skin that can be used to make interpretations about the psychological phenomena of the responder (Boucsein et al., 2012). EDA can be measured via exosomatic or endosomatic techniques. Exosomatic techniques—a more commonly used method used in applied research—apply a small current through a pair of electrodes and then measure electrical resistance (or its reciprocal, i.e., conductance) from the skin. Because the current is kept constant, it is possible to measure changes in the voltage between the electrodes that will vary directly with changes in skin resistance, following Ohm's lab (see Dawson et al., 2007 for a technical review). Endosomatic techniques measure passive changes in intrinsic electrical activity without application of an external current. For details on EDA recording techniques, see Fowles (1986), Dawson et al. (2007), and Boucsein et al. (2012). Higher EDA is indicative of physiological arousal due to increased sympathetic autonomic nervous activity (Dawson et al., 2007; Lohani and Isaacowitz, 2014). EDA is sensitive to physiological reactivity and many other factors, such as respiration and mental effort (Dawson et al., 2007). Commonly derived EDA metrics (Dawson et al., 2007; Boucsein et al., 2012) include slowly varying tonic level of electrical conductivity (skin conductance level; SCL) and phasic increase in magnitude electrical conductance in response to an unexpected or relevant event (skin conductance response; SCR). Non-linear EDA metrics that can differentiate between increased cognitive load vs. recovery phases of stressors have been identified as well (Visnovcova et al., 2016).

EDA in Driving Context

In driving research, systematic variation in several arousal-related constructs can impact EDA. Most commonly investigated is cognitive workload. SCL is higher during increased workload in dual-task relative to single-task driving (Mehler et al., 2012). A systematic investigation of workload increments in one on-road driving study (Mehler et al., 2012) found a systematic increase in SCL as a function of three levels of auditory workload secondary tasks relative to single driving task for young, middle, and older age groups. These findings suggest that SCL can be used to index workload levels in driving context. High SCR has also been found to increase with workload experienced by motorists while driving on difficult road types that required avoiding more traffic and making more decisions (Schneegass et al., 2013). A recent study reported SCR amplitude increased with cognitive load due to dual-task driving (Ruscio et al., 2017). Additional workload experienced due to texting and navigation (Seo et al., 2017) and speeding (Kajiwara, 2014) while simulated driving was also found to increase EDA.

EDA also varies with other physiological arousal-related constructs. EDA based indices can be used to detect stressful events during driving (Affanni et al., 2018). A recent study utilized feature extraction and discrimination processing techniques to classify EDA data into low, medium, vs. high stress levels with about 82% recognition rate (Liu and Du, 2018). Another recent study found higher SCLs when participants drove a simulated vehicle in autonomous mode compared to manual mode (Morris et al., 2017). Higher skin conductance levels could be indicative of lower levels of trust in the autonomous mode than manual mode. State anxiety during simulated driving was also found to be associated with SCL (Barnard and Chapman, 2018). Another recent study found that relative to sleepiness, higher skin conductance levels are found during wakefulness, effects which are indicative of comparatively higher sympathetic activity (Schmidt et al., 2017).

Practical Considerations

In driving contexts, EDA is shown to vary due to many cognitive states, such as workload, stress, anxiety, sleepiness, all of which are influenced by sympathetic nervous system activity. This allows the use of EDA in assessment of various psychological phenomena (Dawson et al., 2007). Therefore, caution should be exercised while interpreting changes in EDA in an applied and less-controlled setting as it is sensitive to not one, but many psychological variables. In the driving context, careful choice of filters to remove artifacts (Affanni et al., 2018) and identification of cognition-related features (Chen et al., 2017; Liu and Du, 2018) that have been successfully implemented could be utilized to improve accuracy and detection. One disadvantage of EDA is that it has a slower response (lag of 1–3 s) after the stimulus has occurred (Dawson et al., 2007). In instances when near-real time physiological responses need to be detected, EDA may be relatively slower (than cardiovascular measures). Another point to consider is that, similar to other physiological measures, not all individuals have the expected skin conductance response (Dawson et al., 2007). This is another reason to avoid reliance on a single measure, but multiple channels, to capture the psychological phenomena of interest.

Electromyography (EMG)

EMG Quantification

EMG is used to measure the electrical activity generated by muscle fibers (Fridlund and Cacioppo, 1986; van Boxtel, 2001). Surface EMG is captured by placing small surface electrodes on specific muscles of interest, which is then digitized and amplified to record muscle activity (Fridlund and Cacioppo, 1986). Numerous features can be extracted from the EMG signals. Root mean square of the signal (in microvolts) is a recommended and commonly reported EMG signal amplitude (Fridlund and Cacioppo, 1986). Other commonly assessed statistical features are peak spectral density, peak amplitude, and peak frequency. A specific muscle's activity can provide insights into the psychological processes underplay. For instance, the smile muscle (or zygomaticus major) and the frown muscle (or corrugator supercilii) have been used a lot in emotion research to identify positive and negative behavioral expressions. For example, more frown muscle activation can be an index of negative behavioral expressions (Lohani and Isaacowitz, 2014; Lohani et al., 2018). Psychological processes (e.g., stress) can lead to sympathetic nervous system activity (Lundberg et al., 1994), which can elicit muscular tension. Researchers have studied muscular activations under controlled conditions to index mental processes (Lundberg et al., 1994; Wijsman et al., 2013; Luijcks et al., 2014). Applied driving research has successfully assessed psychological processes by assessing EMG (Healey et al., 1999; Fu et al., 2016; cf., Morris et al., 2017; Ma et al., 2018).

EMG in Driving Context

In driving contexts, surface EMG has been utilized to study psychological and physiological stress (Jonsson and Jonsson, 1975; Wikström, 1993; Balasubramanian and Adalarasu, 2007; Ahlström et al., 2018). Stress and fatigue have been studied by recording electrical activity from relevant muscles. For instance, variations in the trapezius muscle (a major back muscle that extends from the neck to shoulder blades and lower spine) and deltoid (triangular muscle located on uppermost part of an arm and the top of shoulder) are influenced by mental stress (Wikström, 1993; Balasubramanian and Adalarasu, 2007; Hirao et al., 2007; Wijsman et al., 2013; Luijcks et al., 2014; cf., Morris et al., 2017). A recent study (Lee et al., 2017a) recorded trapezius muscle activity to detect stress in a driving simulator under relaxed and stressed conditions. A continuous increase over time in muscular tension was associated with greater stress experienced due to driving task (Lee et al., 2017a). Muscular tension can thus be a useful metric of stress level that can be utilized in driving research.

It is worth noting that muscular fatigue and discomfort are not isolated issues (Leinonen et al., 2005) and they cause psychological distress and disrupt cognitive performance while driving. Muscle fatigue while driving has been studied by examining changes in muscular tension in shoulder and neck muscles (Sheridan et al., 1991; Wikström, 1993; Balasubramanian and Adalarasu, 2007; Hirao et al., 2007). Compared to the beginning of the drive, continuous driving can lead to reduced back muscles (e.g., trapezius and deltoid) activity and fatigue. Muscular fatigue (measured by EMG of back muscles) is associated with decreases in power of EMG activity-related frequency band (Hostens and Ramon, 2005; Balasubramanian and Adalarasu, 2007; Hirao et al., 2007). Surface EMG is a helpful way of identifying discomfort in fatigued and weak muscles and targeting rehabilitation for skeletomuscular problems specially in professional or long-distance drivers (Balasubramanian and Adalarasu, 2007). A recent study (Artanto et al., 2017) has also used a low-cost EMG system to detect drowsiness. An EMG sensor attached to muscles around eyelid region captured the duration of eyelid closure as an indicator of drowsiness (Artanto et al., 2017). Another recent study has proposed a system that can detect real-time changes in EMG (Mazzetta et al., 2018). Further research is needed to validate EMG's applicability in real-world settings.

Practical Considerations

EMG measurement enable recording continuous data from the specific muscle of interest without obstructing the driving task. Such objective information can be helpful in learning about muscular activity (and relevant cognitive states) that may not be necessarily visible to the researchers or under the awareness of the responder. However, it is essential to pay attention to any outliers or irrelevant events that may add noise to the EMG signal and impact signal interpretation. Irrelevant events can include muscular activity due to driving-unrelated (e.g., continuous posture change, scratching skin, or touching the electrodes) and driving-related (e.g., functional steering activity) movement and yet unrelated to the cognitive state (e.g., mental workload) of the driver (Mehler et al., 2009). In real-world settings, it can be tedious to tease apart muscular activity due to other confounding reasons from activity relevant to changes in cognitive states. Furthermore, the task under investigation is also of importance. For instance, a study that compared muscular tension while driving car autonomously vs. manually found no differences in EMG signals, but significant differences were found for SCL (Morris et al., 2017). This suggests that for some tasks the muscular activity may not significantly differ, but may still be psychologically different in other modalities. This also highlights the importance of multiple measures.

Thermal Imaging

Thermal Imaging Quantification

The measurement of changes in skin temperature is a useful technique to detect and track attributes of a responder, such as body posture and emotional expression (Gade and Moeslund, 2014; Rai et al., 2017). A special merit of this technology is that it enables sensing the real-time state of motorists non-invasively without disrupting driving related tasks. In addition, unlike RGB cameras, thermal cameras do not depend on an external illumination (Gade and Moeslund, 2014; Rai et al., 2017). Objects that emit radiations in the mid-to-long wavelength infrared spectrum (3–14 μm), such as the human body (but not inanimate objects) can be detected via thermal imaging (Gade and Moeslund, 2014; Rai et al., 2017). Changes in temperature distribution, as captured by the thermal cameras, are utilized to make meaningful interpretations. For instance, facial thermography can be used to capture the heat distribution in facial locations known to vary with sympathetic activity as a metric of the varying psychological phenomena. Most commonly investigated facial locations include the forehead and nasal temperature changes.

Sympathetic autonomous nervous system activation may lead to constrictions of blood vessels, thereby decreasing temperature in extremities, such as the nose (Or and Duffy, 2007; Gade and Moeslund, 2014). For example, mental workload changes lead to temperature variations in the forehead, nose, cheeks, and chin regions (Stemberger et al., 2010; Marinescu et al., 2018). A recent study examined the validity and sensitivity of thermal imaging in assessing variation in cognitive load (Abdelrahman et al., 2017). Increased cognitive task difficulty led to significant increases in the forehead temperature and decreases in nose temperature (Abdelrahman et al., 2017). The largest effect sizes were found when the difference in forehead and nose temperature was estimated. Higher task difficulty led to an increase in forehead and nose temperature differences (Abdelrahman et al., 2017). Additional work has also examined real-time sensitivity of thermal imaging and found that specialized thermal cameras can detect changes in cognitive load with a latency of 0.7 s post eliciting event (Abdelrahman et al., 2017). This finding suggests that this methodology has a high relevance for real-time assessments of cognitive load in applied settings like driving.

Thermography in Driving Context

In driving contexts, facial thermography was found to be useful in assessing over-arousal constructs such as mental workload (Or and Duffy, 2007; Murai et al., 2008). Performing a secondary workload task (mental arithmetic) while driving in a simulator as well as an on-road car led to a decrease in nasal temperature with stable forehead temperatures (Or and Duffy, 2007). Drop in nasal temperature also correlated with self-reported workload (Or and Duffy, 2007). Another study found increases in the difference between nose and forehead temperature increased with mental workload (Kajiwara, 2014). Participants' nasal temperature varied as a function of mental workload in simulated driving (Kajiwara, 2014). Workload variation indexed by changes in nasal temperature were also reported during ship navigation using a simulator (Murai et al., 2008), highlighting its utility in applied settings.

Furthermore, facial thermography can be useful to examine and infer heat distribution in faces during emotional states. This method could be promising and may provide a non-invasive approach to capture emotional states because current methods of emotion recognition using facial features detection software have limitations. One study used an infrared thermal camera to non-invasively detect face regions and recognize emotional states of motorists (Kolli et al., 2011). This study suggests that thermography can improve face detection algorithm for in-vehicle settings thereby facilitating ADAS.

In another line of work (Cheng et al., 2007), a combination of thermal infrared and color cameras have shown to be effective in sensing body movements in real-time on-road driving. Similarly, infrared streaming has been used to develop posture and occupancy sensory systems (Kato et al., 2004; Trivedi et al., 2004). Another recent study reported successful use of near-infrared light and thermal camera sensors to identify aggressive driving behavior (Lee et al., 2018) and were able to categorize aggressive driving from relaxed driving. The above studies suggest that thermography has the potential to be a useful non-invasive technique that can be validated to capture cognition-relevant states and improve traffic safety.

Practical Considerations

Thermal cameras are used in numerous industrial, agricultural, and military settings (Gade and Moeslund, 2014). They can be extremely useful in vehicular technology because they are non-contact sensors and can work regardless of external illumination. Nevertheless, further testing is needed to better understand how this technology would improve our understanding of cognitive states in traffic safety. Further systematic investigation and replication of thermography as a function of cognitive workload, stress, and drowsiness after controlling for confounding factors, such as environmental factors (e.g., weather conditions and air conditioning), are needed to be able to make confident assessments of cognitive states. The results so far look promising.

Pupillometry

Pupil Quantification

Pupillometry is the measurement of pupil size and reactivity. Modern pupillometry is measured via optical eye-trackers that use some combination of monitoring infrared light reflections from the cornea, the back of the lens, and the pupil, as well as absorption of light by the pupil (e.g., dark-pupil tracking). Most modern eye-tracking devices can monitor pupil location (and eye-fixation location) with very high resolution (>1,000 Hz) non-invasively and at a substantial distance from a participant. Thus, measurement can occur in highly ecologically valid environments, without participants having to make any overt responses. Since the 1960's it has been shown that pupil dilation changes as a result of mental activity—for example, increases in arousal and cognitive workload (e.g., Hess and Polt, 1964). In a classic study demonstrating the sensitivity of pupillometry to cognitive demands, Kahneman and Beatty (1966) showed that pupil dilation increases parametrically with an increasing number of words to recall in a simple word list memory task. Moreover, they showed that this increase in workload persists over a maintenance interval, and reduces parametrically as each word is retrieved (and released) from memory. These findings, along with a number of other demonstrations of pupillary sensitivity to cognitive workload, for example in math problem solving (Sirois and Brisson, 2014), working memory and individual differences in intelligence (Tsukahara et al., 2016), aging and verbal memory load (Piquado et al., 2010), has led to wide interest in this measure as a physiological marker of arousal and cognitive effort.

Janisse (1977) remarked that the eye is the only “visible part of the brain.” Indeed, detailed models of the neurophysiology of pupillomotor functioning are developed and growing, including an understanding of the innervation of the sphincter and dilator muscles by the autonomic nervous system (Miller et al., 2005), as well as the neuromodulatory relationship between pupil dilation, activity in the locus-coeruleus (LC; a neuromodulatory nucleus in the dorsal pons of the brainstem strongly linked to phasic and tonic arousal, cognitive control, and monitoring functions), and norepinephrine (Gilzenrat, 2006). For instance, a high correlation (0.6) between spike frequency and pupil diameter has been found, whereby large pupil diameter equates to high LC activity (Rajkowski et al., 1994). Demberg (2013) have also recently reported changes in pupillometry due to linguistically induced cognitive load (e.g., comprehending syntactically demanding sentences). Other recent work has also examined user state related changes in pupil diameter in lab-settings such as variations in valence and arousal (Kassem et al., 2017) and interest in real-time (Jacob et al., 2018).

Pupillometry in Driving Context

Eye-tracking has been used extensively in studying visual perception and attention in driving contexts, however the unique use of pupillometry as an index of real-time physiological indicator of cognitive workload is only lately growing in popularity (Schwalm et al., 2008). For example, Cegovnik et al. (2018) recently validated a low-cost eye-tracker and showed that pupil dilation increases with increments in cognitive load due to a secondary memory task (n-back) (see also Recarte and Nunes, 2000 for similar results). Pupillometry has also been adopted in driving research while motorists drove in a simulated driving context. Pupil diameter was found to reliably increase with increases in cognitive load (Palinko et al., 2010; Faure et al., 2016). Other work has use machine learning algorithms to detect cognitive load while driving from pupillometry data (Yoshida et al., 2014). A recent study found that during simulated driving, pupil dilation could detect increases in cognitive load imposed by a secondary task within a lag of 1 s (Prabhakar et al., 2018). This suggests that pupillometry could be used as a near-real time index of cognitive load.

Pupillometry has also been used to differentiate between alertness and drowsiness (Soares et al., 2013). Alertness is associated with increased mean pupil diameter and decreases in standard deviation (i.e., stable), whereas drowsiness is associated with decreases in diameter, but increases in standard deviation (i.e., fluctuations) in pupil diameter (Morad et al., 2000; Wilhelm et al., 2009). Fluctuations in pupil size have been proposed to be a reliable index of drowsiness-related impairment while driving (Maccora et al., 2018). Pupil dilation was also found sensitive to fatigue levels while driving with a decrease in fatigue being associated with an increase in pupil diameter (Schmidt et al., 2017). Although early, these findings, along with others (for a recent review see Marquart et al., 2015; Maccora et al., 2018) suggest that pupillometry is an efficient, ecologically valid, and low-cost physiological reporter variable for indexing cognitive states in driving in highly-controlled environments like realistic driving simulators.

Practical Considerations

In lab settings, pupil diameter was found to be a reliable, non-invasive, and real-time measure of workload (Marinescu et al., 2018). However, in on-road settings, it is quite challenging to capture interpretable pupil information due to large variations in luminance that are hard to control across conditions and participants. Indeed, photopupillary reflex is massive in magnitude relative to changes in pupil size related to cognitive and attentional factors. As such, if there are considerable changes in lighting conditions (e.g., sunny vs. cloudy days), this can create considerable noise in the pupillary signal. Moreover, if specific conditions of interest are confounded with respect to overall luminance (e.g., driving during the day vs. driving at night), this overall pupillary light reflex-related shift should be taken into consideration. Furthermore, if investigating event-related pupillary responses in driving, one should be careful to determine that differences in pupil dilation are not only due to differences in visual stimulation (e.g., presenting a luminant STOP sign). Modeling techniques have also developed methods to infer cognitive workload after accounting for some variations in lighting conditions (Pfleging et al., 2016; Reilly et al., 2018).

Marshall (2002) have developed a signal processing method for extracting high-frequency changes in pupil dilation that they argue is uniquely related to cognitive components (Index of Cognitive Activity or ICA). However, this method is a commercially available “black box” system, and should be interpreted with caution given that the exact algorithm used to calculate ICA from raw pupillometry is not open source. Other work has estimated an Index of Pupillary Activity (IPA) inspired by ICA, that uses wavelet-based algorithms to decompose pupil data (Duchowski et al., 2018). IPA was found to differentiate between low vs. high mental workload (Duchowski et al., 2018). Another important feature to consider is that measurement of pupil dilation is affected by eye-movements and relative gaze position (e.g., Gagl et al., 2011). When gaze position changes from central to peripheral locations, the recorded pupil shifts from a circular to an elliptical shape from the point of view of fixed camera location. This change in the recorded geometry of the pupil is accompanied by changes in overall pupil size, irrespective of actual changes in dilation or constriction. Gagl et al. (2011) have developed methods for the measurement and removal of such systematic influences. Nevertheless, researchers should be careful to measure gaze position and to design studies such that likely visual target locations are not confounded across conditions of interest.

Challenges and Recommendations

Psychophysiological research has made tremendous progress in developing methods to quantify cognitive processes. Most of this research has been conducted in carefully controlled environments to be able to interpret with certainty what changes in a physiological signal may imply about the psychological phenomena under investigation. Physiological signals are valuable to understand how people interact in real-world contexts. Driving research is an excellent application of psychophysiological methods to understand and interpret how people interact with automation in natural settings, which in turn can inform intelligent systems to improve driving performance and safety. As evidenced by much of the growing research base discussed above, psychophysiological measures can be successfully adopted to meet these goals. At the same time, lack of adherence to research protocols and guidelines can seriously jeopardize meaningful use of these methodologies. Here we highlight a few general challenges and recommendations that cut across all psychophysiological measures in driving research when collecting data from real-world driving settings—which are less predictable than lab settings— to improve data-quality and aid in effective interpretation.

Valid and Reliable Quantification of Construct

Depending upon the task and setting (lab-based simulator or field study), some physiological measures will be more suitable and feasible than others. For example, in a simulator with very controlled body movement, continuous blood pressure using the volume clamp method can be collected. However, while on-road, this equipment may compromise drivers' safety and thus is not feasible. Other measures like ECG and thermal cameras are highly mobile and feasible. Careful observations can allow interpretation of cognitive processes while driving. One important concern is the possibility of misinterpreting the relationship between physiological signals and cognitive processes (Cacioppo and Tassinary, 1990; Cacioppo et al., 2007). Often, physiological measures (such as HR, EDA, EMG) are impacted by multiple processes, such as drowsiness, stress, and workload, which can lead to interpretive caveats. Systematic variations in different experimental conditions can help tease apart the underlying mechanism causing autonomic activations to be able to draw clear inferences. However, in an applied setting like driving a car in unpredictable traffic, control over the experimental task is largely out of the control of the researcher. Confirmatory independent measures are important to validate the construct of interest in the study. Similarly, it is helpful to ensure that the construct of interest reliably varies across conditions and that the experimental manipulation was effective.

Individual Differences

A combination of factors may influence physiological signals, including trait-level variables such as demographic factors (age, gender), task experience (professional, experienced, inexperienced), anxiety, and certain health conditions and medications (e.g., cardiovascular health). State-level variations such as stress-levels unrelated to task, caffeine intake (which may change autonomic activity), and engagement/motivation and frustration during the task can also interact with individual differences in ways that may not be readily apparent. Combining data from participants after considering such trait- and state-level variables can help in proper interpretation of study findings.

On a related note, a critical challenge in multi-modal recordings is that individuals may be highly reactive as assessed by one measure but not necessarily, according to another. There is considerable variability across individuals in how closely physiological, behavioral, and subjective measures covary over time with one another (Lohani et al., 2018). Furthermore, it is possible that only some individuals may be sensitive to the experimental manipulation (Drews et al., 2012). Such individual differences may lead to variations in psychophysiological assessments and may also explain to some extent lack of significant differences across experimental conditions. Many, if not all, of these measures are currently utilized within paradigms where we are studying relative changes in the outcome across conditions (e.g., P3b amplitude is a difference wave, HRV% change, %signal change in BOLD response, etc.), for which these measures do not have currently well-understood absolute thresholds for making strong absolute judgements. While there isn't a fixed threshold for physiological measures that can be used across individuals to define high and low arousal levels, relative changes from baseline can be a useful way of assessing variations in arousal levels from optimal levels for the individual. If the system can be calibrated on what is a “normal” range for an individual, then significant variations from this calibrated range can be a way to detect sub-optimal arousal levels.

Baseline Assessments

Baseline assessments provide insights about the physiological state of the responder when the experimental condition was absent. It also allows to control for physiological activity due to any prior conditions, so that the change in the experimental condition of interest is interpreted relative to the state right before the condition started. A single baseline is generally not enough, especially when there are multiple conditions. It is a good practice to capture as many baseline assessments and as close to the experimental condition as possible. Another alternate design to consider (for measures with high temporal resolution) is an event-related design, where activity is time-locked to specific events of interest. In this design, pre-event activity in the measure is subtracted from the overall physiological time series, resulting in a strong baseline control for each trial (e.g., ERPs).

Sampling Rate, Filtering, and Signal Quality

Nearly all physiological signals discussed above are analog signals, which have to be digitized for further processing. Choice of optimal sampling rate and filtering helps avoid signal distortions (Jennings and Allen, 2016), and as such, knowledge of signal processing characteristics of the target physiological measures is necessary for researchers to effectively use these tools. Optimal sampling rate differs by the physiological signal's frequency characteristics, and poor sampling rate can distort waveform characteristics, and induce artificial oscillatory characteristics that are not part of the true analog signal (i.e., aliasing). For example, for HRV analysis, the recommended sampling rate is at least 250 Hz (Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology, 1996). Some commercial wearables (e.g., fitness-related wrist watch sensors) have sampling rate as low as 60 Hz, which will lead to signal aliasing (Jennings and Allen, 2016) and inaccurate and uninterpretable HRV values. The sampling rate needs to be at least above the Nyquist frequency (2x the sampling rate of the highest frequency), and current standards suggest a sample rate 3–4 times the highest frequency component of physiological signal. Advancements in modern computing allow for research-grade equipment to sample far above Nyquist for most of the measures discussed (>2,000 Hz) during data acquisition. Of course, data can always be down-sampled post data collection. As discussed in sections “Heart Activity Quantification” and “Practical Considerations” on heart activity, quantification using wearables can lead to inaccurate assessments (Laborde et al., 2017) due to poor sampling rates, lagged responses, and noisier signals to name a few, which would lead to inaccurate interpretations.

Filters are helpful in getting rid of artifacts and noise not relevant for the physiological signal being processed. For instance, muscle and electrical noise (around 60 Hz) are not meaningful while interpreting EEG and ERP data, and thus data outside the range of interest (typically not higher than 40–50 Hz) can be bandpass filtered. However, if EMG activity, which has a much higher frequency content, is of interest, then bandpass filtering with allow low-pass cutoff at 500 Hz and high-pass cutoff at 20 Hz, is often suitable (van Boxtel, 2001). Visual inspection pre- and post-filtering process can help determine how filtering is affecting a signal. Note that all filters distort the waveform and spectral characteristics, so unnecessary filtering should be avoided and researchers should take care to understand exactly how filters are impacting their data in time and frequency domains.

For each psychophysiological measure discussed, researchers have a growing number of indices that can be examined (for example, for HRV, time-based, frequency-based, and non-linear measures can be derived). Choice of metrics should be carefully evaluated, as some metrics may be more suitable to meet the goals of the study, while others may not be suitable. For instance, some metrics require minimum duration of data and falling short of such requirements will lead to misrepresentative findings (e.g., standard deviation of R-R heart beats or SDRR is considered more accurate when calculated over 24 h vs. 5 min or shorter intervals; Shaffer and Ginsberg, 2017). Such choices should be made a priori, based on the research question of interest and links between a measure and its purported psychological interpretation based on prior research. Such flexibility in multi-modal recording comes at the cost of an increasing number of “experimenter degrees of freedom,” that can lead to inflated Type-I error rates, if a consistent analysis pipeline is not followed. It is also important to use comparable durations of physiological signals across conditions and participants for appropriate interpretation. Finally, great attention to accurate event markers is critical for valid interpretation within and across participants in event-related designs. This can be an issue when using commercial products that are not designed for research purposes.

Innovation

A limitation of most current psychophysiological research-grade measures is the need for using contact sensors (placed on skin). Non-contact sensors are beginning to be tested in applied settings, which can make physiological data collection even less invasive. For instance, ECG data can be derived from high-quality RGB cameras, or sensors could be placed on the steering wheel and driving seats (but should meet the recommended requirements). While these can potentially be a great approach to counter the limitations of contact sensors, caution is advised while considering them because new limitations or inaccuracies in assessment are possible and further research and testing is required to adopt them in research. Commercial products may not meet the requirements recommended by the scientific community, which can lead to poor data quality and invalid interpretations. For example, smartphone camera-based PPG sensing estimates have poor sampling rate and can lead to inaccurate assessments (Laborde et al., 2017). It is essential to ensure that the guidelines for measures are met before investing time and resources to avoid technical issues in data collection and interpretation. For instance, as discussed earlier, it is critical to collect physiological data with recommended frequency sampling to avoid aliasing (Jennings and Allen, 2016). Only equipment that have been or can be validated against research-grade devices should be adopted for research purposes.

Classification

Reliable and valid assessment of cognitive states is the groundwork to develop inputs to advance state detection-workload managers and “aware” systems. For instance, a recent study reported a reliable method to elicit stress in naturalistic driving scenarios (Baltodano et al., 2018). Given that one measure may not be enough to reliably measure subtle changes in cognitive state, a multi-method approach is critical to capture state-level variations that may not be apparent through a single measure alone. Research has shown that multi-modal approaches provide a reliable (Schmidt et al., 2011; Borghini et al., 2014; Chen et al., 2017) way to sense and assess cognitive states of motorists in real-world settings. Notably, due to the dynamic nature of the physiological signals, conventional linear approaches are not always appropriate in modeling and predicting cognitive state (Chen et al., 2015). The discussed physiological signals are often non-stationary overall but for the briefest periods of time. As such, innovative methods of combining temporal and spectral resolution (time-frequency analysis) have been developed in some domains (e.g., EEG), but their application to other physiological signals is only in its infancy.

Once data have been processed to remove artifacts or irrelevant noise, machine learning techniques could be trained on these data to identify “risky” sub-optimal levels of cognitive states, such as low-arousal states of drowsiness and fatigue associated with unsafe driving performance. During the training phase, multimodal features extracted from physiological training data could be used to train models to classify observations into high-arousal states (e.g., due to high stress and workload), optimal-arousal state, vs. low-arousal state (e.g., due to drowsiness and fatigue). During the test phase, the fully-specified machine learning algorithm can be tested in terms of its capacity to accurately classify observations into respective arousal states. Indeed, cognitive state detection based on multimodal feature analysis and classifiers have been also used to detect stress (Yang et al., 2016; Chen et al., 2017; Lee et al., 2017b), alertness and drowsiness (Forsman et al., 2013; Correa et al., 2014; Chen et al., 2015; Wang and Chuan, 2016), fatigue (Fu and Wang, 2014; Wang, 2015; Fu et al., 2016; Li et al., 2017; Wang et al., 2017), and workload (Borghini et al., 2014; Yang et al., 2016) in real-time. Such studies have integrated data from more than one measure by conducting multi-modal analysis to extract the relevant features to capture the psychological phenomena at hand. A comparison of multiple classifiers to train & optimize machine learning algorithms can help determine the best fitting model to represent changes in cognitive states that can explain driving performance (Nadeau and Bengio, 2000; Fairclough et al., 2015; Balters and Steinert, 2017; Tran et al., 2017). Thus, utilizing multi-modal physiological signals, models could be trained to learn and predict motorists' sub-optimal cognitive states associated with unsafe-driving behavior.

The optimized machine learning algorithms could accordingly inform advanced state detection managers to trigger warnings or otherwise intervene when sub-optimal cognitive states associated with risky driving behavior are detected (Aidman et al., 2015). The ability to predict unsafe levels of physiological arousal will enable targeted augmentation to modify motorists' cognitive state to promote safer driving behavior (Schmidt and Bullinger, 2017; Schmidt et al., 2017; Aricò et al., 2018). For instance, countermeasures to augment cognitive states, such as thermal stimulation (Schmidt and Bullinger, 2017; Schmidt et al., 2017) and warning signs or verbal communication (Schmidt et al., 2011; Aidman et al., 2015) can be used by an automated system to modify drivers' cognitive state. This may especially benefit vulnerable groups such as inexperienced drivers (Noordzij et al., 2017; Yan et al., 2017) and older (Costa et al., 2017) drivers who may be more susceptible to cognitive overload. Furthermore, a person-centered approach can account for individual differences, such as the role of age, driving profile, trust, and reliance on automation. For instance, a recent study used discriminant analysis to account for motorists' driving-styles and individual difference factors (e.g., gender, age, anxiety, anger) and also identify motorists' EEG and EDA response features to classify motorists' safe vs. risky driving tendencies (Liang and Lin, 2018). This study shows that individual differences can explain variations in driving performance and a customized approach may also help improve model prediction over time by accounting for motorists' characteristics and preferences. For example, the low, normal, and high physiological arousal ranges will vary depending on attributes such as anxious, risky, and distress reduction driving styles of an individual (Liang and Lin, 2018) and prediction of cognitive state-level variations may be more accurate when predictions account for such individual-level variations. Thus, a person-centered approach will improve reliable predictions of cognitive states in real-world contexts by intelligent driving systems.

Research Applicability in Real-World Settings

As the reviewed literature in section, “Psychophysiological Measures to Assess Cognitive States” suggests, many interrelated states could lead to a similar pattern of findings on a physiological measure (e.g., mental fatigue, drowsiness, lower vigilance, and mind wandering are all sensitive to similar EEG/ERP indices). After considering the overlap across findings from interrelated constructs, in Table 1 we have summarized the expected pattern that each physiological measure will have during a low vs. high arousal state in an applied driving context. There are a few points to consider. First, changes in several related cognitive states can lead to similar changes in arousal. For example, increases in driver workload, stress, or vigilance may occur under different contexts, but may similarly lead to heightened arousal. Second, even though arousal is continuous, we chose to classify driver states into categories of low and high arousal because both extremes are sub-optimal for driving performance. Third, cognitive states are complex and change across time. For instance, in the current review, we have placed mind wandering in a low-arousal state based on similar patterns of findings as drowsiness. However, mind wandering is a convenient short-hand for a more complex constellation of non-externally directed cognitive states (see Smallwood and Schooler, 2006 for a review) and depending on the context, such mind-wandering states can yield states of heightened-arousal as well. Similarly, fatigue can be categorized as high-arousal due to prolonged cognitive overload or it can be passive, because of underload due to monotonous driving conditions, for example (Saxby et al., 2008; Matthews et al., 2019). With further empirical evidence in naturalistic environments, a better characterization of complex cognitive states could be developed.

It is still an open question if interrelated cognitive states could be successfully differentiated from other similar states in naturalistic environments (see Cacioppo et al., 2007 for challenges with psychological inference). However, physiological measures could be used to assess sub-optimal levels of general arousal in real-world settings and intelligent systems can use this information to trigger augmentation strategies even if we cannot fully differentiate between specific cognitive states besides along their arousal axis. We have reviewed how physiological responses across multiple measures can provide a rich array of response data relevant to domains that are of interest to driving researchers (e.g., attention, fatigue, workload, etc.). These measures provide unique information and unique sensitivity to experimental manipulations beyond behavioral responses alone. Thus, their current and future utility in real-world driving research is important. This does not mean that measuring one or even a large number of these measures alone will provide us with a direct interpretation of a covert state (e.g., becoming increasingly frustrated about an aggressive driver behind you). Before the state of the research matures to be able to address such a lofty goal as predicting specific cognitive states (Yarkoni and Westfall, 2017), we first need careful on-road experimental work to understand the sensitivity and specificity of these measures to specific changes in driver-relevant states in observational and experimental research in real-world settings. Thus, the focus of the current review is not to claim that measurement of multiple physiological measures in real-world driving could accurately predict motorists' specific cognitive state. Rather, our goal is to summarize the feasibility of each of these measures for integrating high-quality psychophysiological methodology into real-world driving research. Table 1 presents the current working predictions that are expected based on the available literature, but more work is needed to be able to use physiological signals to infer psychological processes. The current review represents a summary of initial steps in that direction.

In Table 2, we have summarized the research applicability of the reviewed psychophysiological measures. Although all of these measures can provide valuable insights in the controlled settings of a lab, some measures are more feasible to use and interpret than others in real-world driving contexts. A few factors that may play a role in determining the practical use of physiological measures in applied settings are: the degree of coupling between the measure and subtle changes in cognitive states, temporal resolution, psychometric reliability, ease of data collection (e.g., setup time), sensitivity to artifacts, and the degree of invasiveness and disruption to normal driving. After considering the available evidence, we have categorized each measure's real-world research applicability into low, medium, or high levels. Moreover, certain measures may be better candidates than others for a near real-time assessment in applied settings. We review the real-world applicability and feasibility of each of the measures in Table 2.

Some promising work suggests that cardiovascular measures may be robust in detecting near real-time changes across multiple domains. Studies have shown that cardiovascular data can reliably detect changes in workload (Mehler et al., 2009, 2012; Lenneman and Backs, 2010; Stuiver et al., 2014), fatigue (Patel et al., 2011; Matthews et al., 2019), and drowsiness (Vicente et al., 2016; Kurosawa et al., 2017). Like any physiological signal, cardiovascular data is susceptible to artifacts that could otherwise lead to inaccurate estimations. However, recent analytical advances have led to an improved use in real-world settings even in the presence of substantial recording artifact. For instance, an analysis approach using short segments of cardiovascular data (e.g., a moving window of 30 s; Stuiver et al., 2012) can be used to detect workload demands during driving (Stuiver et al., 2014). Use of smaller temporal windows of data allow for an investigation of the short-term effects of cognitive state without being overly susceptible to artifacts. Recent work has shown that frequency analysis techniques on ECG data can also be utilized to detect early onset of fatigue (Matthews et al., 2019). While the limitations of PPG discussed earlier still apply, recent preliminary work using near-infrared illumination PPG (which overcomes confounds of illumination and motion-related inaccuracies) while driving seems a promising direction for future practical applications (Nowara et al., 2018). Another recent work has developed a noise-resistant algorithm specifically designed to analyze PPG waveforms (van Gent et al., 2018), which can provide researchers an open-source and validated heart rate analysis software to overcome some existing limitations of PPG data processing, making it more feasible for applied driving research.

EDA has been found to be a robust measure of sympathetic arousal in driving contexts in real-world settings (Mehler et al., 2012; Schneegass et al., 2013; Ruscio et al., 2017). EDA is also easy to set up and collect from a motorist without obstructing the driving process. Even though it has a slower response time and provides only a broad sense of arousal (a combination of workload, stress, fatigue, etc.), EDA in an applied uncontrolled environment can estimate relative changes and periods of stability in sympathetic activity of a motorist with an upper temporal resolution of approximately 3–5 s. For example, recent work found EDA to be suitable in capturing stress-level variations in a real-time unconstrained setting (ElKomy et al., 2017). Feature extraction and pattern recognition algorithms have also shown reasonable success recently in detecting changes in cognitive states (Chen et al., 2017; Liu and Du, 2018). Moreover, adaptive filters have been successfully used to remove motion-related artifacts for automatic and accurate detection (up to 95% sensitivity) of state-level variations in cognition (Affanni et al., 2018). Such recent processing and analytic advances with EDA data has shown its high relevance in applied intelligent automation. For example, a development approach proposed for monitoring driver's fatigue levels and functional state utilizes automated analysis of EDA indices in their detection module to improve intelligent vehicular systems (Liu and Du, 2018; Savchenko and Poddubko, 2018).

EEG, a direct measure of brain's electrical activity, can provide robust measures of cognitive state variations while driving, including levels of drowsiness (Liang et al., 2006; Wei et al., 2018), fatigue (Liu et al., 2015; Fu et al., 2016; Hung et al., 2017), and workload (Dasari et al., 2017; Zander et al., 2017). EEG has high temporal resolution and is a direct measure of brain activity. However, data collection (e.g., longer setup time) and processing in real-world setting (e.g., movement artifacts) can be quite challenging to implement into a real-world driving research protocol (Popescu et al., 2008). At the same time, there have been innovative technological and analytical developments in EEG acquisition. For instance, efforts in brain computer interface applications have utilized a single electrode to classify relaxed vs. cognitive workload phases (Shirazi et al., 2014) and monitor fatigue levels (Morales et al., 2017). Recent work extracted features from a 6-channel EEG dataset to classify mental tasks with up to 83% accuracy rate (Neshov et al., 2018). Other recent work has reported detection algorithms that can be used to accurately classify fatigue (Li et al., 2017; Gao et al., 2018). In other work, a novel approach to detect drowsiness has been proposed which reduces calibration time for a new user by 90% using a hierarchical clustering method, which accounts for inter- and intra-subject variability (Wei et al., 2018). Automatic drowsiness detection algorithms based on only a single target channel can allow real-time neural assessments of cognitive states (Belakhdar et al., 2018). With increasing advancements in sensor development and data processing, we hold an optimistic view of adopting EEG-based measures in driving research, albeit after considerable validation (Kosiachenko and Si, 2017; Krol et al., 2017; Zander et al., 2017; Byrom et al., 2018). Recent work has also shown the applicability of specific ERP components (such as the P300), some of which show good psychometric properties (e.g., Cassidy et al., 2012), and can be adopted to brain-computer interfaces (Piña-Ramírez et al., 2018). Future work and reliable replication of studies are required to ensure EEG and ERPs could be assimilated in human-machine automation interface.

Traditional fNIRS has lower temporal resolution and may additionally be difficult to collect in applied settings. However, recently, mobile-friendly systems have been developed and used in applied domains (von Lühmann et al., 2015) including exercise physiology (Byun et al., 2014), clinical monitoring (Kassab et al., 2018), and infant developmental research (Quaresima et al., 2012). Importantly, these advancements mean that fNIRS measurements can be performed in naturalistic environments without considerable restraint. As the development of ultra-portable systems grows (e.g., battery powered mobile systems, McKendrick et al., 2016), fNIRS will likely form a novel complement to the many other physiological measures discussed here, in part because of its unique capability to image neural hemodynamics and reveal changes in brain activity with improved spatial resolution compared to other portable and non-invasive neurophysiological methods (e.g., EEG; Ahn and Jun, 2017). For instance, a recent study adopted a wearable fNIRS system (with sensors placed on a baseball cap making it less intrusive) to measure cognitive distraction while driving (Le et al., 2018). Thus, while these methods are still in their infancy compared to many of the other methods discussed here, the ability to reveal neural mechanisms of cognitive states in real-world domains such as driving is promising.

Similar to fNIRS, thermal imaging also shows some early promise. It is a non-contact technology that has high relevance in applied settings, including driving (Lee et al., 2018). For example, recent work has shown the validity of thermal imaging in indexing cognitive load. In these studies, changes in nasal and forehead temperatures were observed as a function of task difficulty in a non-driving context (Abdelrahman et al., 2017; Marinescu et al., 2018). However, research in real-world settings is currently limited. Existing preliminary work has focused primarily on understanding the sensitivity of this measure in well-controlled environments. Future work will help qualify the utility and validity of thermal imaging in real-world conditions.

On the other hand, several measures, despite clear utility in a lab environment, may be currently of less use in real-world settings. For example, pupillometry in well-controlled lab settings can provide helpful information in interpreting user state (e.g., Pfleging et al., 2016; Cegovnik et al., 2018). Moreover, with the development of desktop-mounted eye trackers, pupil dilation and constriction can be measured non-invasively and remotely with high spatial and temporal resolution. In lab settings, where features such as luminance can be controlled and measured, recent work has shown success in using pupillometry to examine mental workload in an unconstrained setting (e.g., Lego construction; Bækgaard et al., 2019). In driving, some researchers have suggested that pupil-based measurements are highly relevant for assessment of drowsiness (Maccora et al., 2018). However, detection of pupil diameter in real-world settings with rapidly changing and uncontrollable variations in luminance is a critical confounding factor in the utility of pupillometry in driving (Kassem et al., 2017).

Similarly, EMG can be utilized in lab settings to understand psychological processes. For example, EMG in combination with other psychophysiological measures was recently utilized in detecting fatigue in drivers (Fu et al., 2016; Ma et al., 2018). Preliminary research has also proposed the use of EMG to detect drowsiness (Artanto et al., 2017) and real-time monitoring of muscle activity (Mazzetta et al., 2018). However, in applied settings such as driving, EMG may have only low utility, in part because the necessary motor activity needed to engage in the task (e.g., turning the steering wheel and actuation of break) can cause uncontrolled changes in muscle activity that can be confounded with the psychological variance in EMG, which is an order of magnitude smaller than these artifacts.

At the same time, ongoing methodological developments are resulting in more efficient systems, improved signal-to-noise ratio, and improved signal-processing methods, all of which culminate in rapidly improving the reliability and validity of acquisition across these multiple methodologies. Some attempts to assess cognitive states using multiple methods have been integrated in non-driving domains (ElKomy et al., 2017; Ko et al., 2017; Moghaddam and Lowe, 2019) and multi-method work in real-world driving contexts are already underway (Fu et al., 2016; Brouwer et al., 2017; Zander et al., 2017; Aricò et al., 2018; Belakhdar et al., 2018; Haouij et al., 2018; Paredes et al., 2018; Rastgoo et al., 2018).

Taken together, we have reviewed a growing body of empirical evidence suggesting that physiological measures can be used to sense and assess changes in the cognitive states of motorists during real-world driving. Through this selective review, we believe that the strengths and limitations of adopting physiological measures in driving can clearly extend to other domains such as the use of aircraft, trains, and ships. Furthermore, we see growing promise for the application of covert monitoring methods like those reviewed above with the increasing rise in semi-automated technology, where motorists will become less directly involved in the driving process. As such, the development of intelligent driving assistance systems will need to utilize non-behavior-based measures to index covert cognitive states of a motorist in the absence of any overt behavior. The physiological measures reviewed above have the potential to detect sub-optimal arousal levels associated with risky driving behavior and inform state detection-workload managers and “aware” systems to trigger warnings or intervene, resulting in a closed-loop system in the absence of any overt-driving behaviors. Before we reach such a future however, the field needs to adopt rigorous standards for the use of psychophysiological measurement in real-world settings. We hope to see a future of increased collaboration and integration of basic psychophysiology, human factors, and traffic safety research. Such integration is necessary to advance the development of effective human-machine driving interfaces and driver support systems, with the ultimate goal of improving traffic safety.

Author Contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Funding

Support for this paper was provided by a grant from AAA Foundation for Traffic Safety.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

AAA Foundation for Traffic Safety (2018). 2017 Traffic Safety Culture Index. Washington, DC: AAA Foundation for Traffic Safety.

Abdelrahman, Y., Velloso, E., Dingler, T., Schmidt, A., and Vetere, F. (2017). “Cognitive heat: exploring the usage of thermal imaging to unobtrusively estimate cognitive load,” in Proceedings of the ACM on Interactive, Mobile, Wearable and Ubiquitous Technologies, (New York, NY). doi: 10.1145/3130898

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abe, E., Fujiwara, K., Hiraoka, T., Yamakawa, T., and Kano, M. (2016). Development of drowsiness detection method by integrating heart rate variability analysis and multivariate statistical process control. SICE J. Control Meas. Syst. Integr. 9, 10–17. doi: 10.9746/jcmsi.9.10

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Affanni, A., Bernardini, R., Piras, A., Rinaldo, R., and Zontone, P. (2018). Driver's stress detection using skin potential response signals. Measurement 122, 264–274. doi: 10.1016/j.measurement.2018.03.040

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ahlström, C., Gink Lövgren, M., Nilsson, M., Dukic Willstrand, T., and Anund, A. (2018). The effect of an active steering system on city bus drivers muscle activity. Int. J. Occup. Saf. Ergonom. 2018, 1–9. doi: 10.1080/10803548.2018.1445465

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ahn, S., and Jun, S. C. (2017). Multi-modal integration of EEG-fNIRS for brain-computer interfaces–current limitations and future directions. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:503. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00503

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aidman, E., Chadunow, C., Johnson, K., and Reece, J. (2015). Real-time driver drowsiness feedback improves driver alertness and self-reported driving performance. Accid. Anal. Prev. 81, 8–13. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2015.03.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Angelidis, A., van der Does, W., Schakel, L., and Putman, P. (2016). Frontal EEG theta/beta ratio as an electrophysiological marker for attentional control and its test-retest reliability. Biol. Psychol. 121, 49–52. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aricò, P., Borghini, G., Di Flumeri, G., Sciaraffa, N., and Babiloni, F. (2018). Passive BCI beyond the lab: current trends and future directions. Physiol. Meas. 39:08TR02. doi: 10.1088/1361-6579/aad57e

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arnau, S., Möckel, T., Rinkenauer, G., and Wascher, E. (2017). The interconnection of mental fatigue and aging: an EEG study. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 117, 17–25. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2017.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Artanto, D., Sulistyanto, M. P., Pranowo, I. D., and Pramesta, E. E. (2017). “Drowsiness detection system based on eye-closure using a low-cost EMG and ESP8266,” in Information Technology, Information Systems and Electrical Engineering (ICITISEE), 2017 2nd International Conferences (Yogyakarta), 235–238. doi: 10.1109/ICITISEE.2017.8285502

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aston-Jones, G., and Cohen, J. D. (2005). An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: adaptive gain and optimal performance. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 28, 403–450. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135709

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bækgaard, P., Jalaliniya, S., and Hansen, J. P. (2019). Pupillary measurement during an assembly task. Appl. Ergon. 75, 99–107. doi: 10.1016/j.apergo.2018.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balasubramanian, V., and Adalarasu, K. (2007). EMG-based analysis of change in muscle activity during simulated driving. J. Bodywork Mov. Ther. 11, 151–158. doi: 10.1016/j.jbmt.2006.12.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baldwin, C. L., Roberts, D. M., Barragan, D., Lee, J. D., Lerner, N., and Higgins, J. S. (2017). Detecting and quantifying mind wandering during simulated driving. Front. Human Neurosci. 11:406. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00406

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balters, S., Murnane, E. L., Landay, J. A., and Paredes, P. E. (2018). “Breath booster!: exploring in-car, fast-paced breathing interventions to enhance driver arousal state,” in Proceedings of the 12th EAI International Conference on Pervasive Computing Technologies for Healthcare (New York, NY). doi: 10.1145/3240925.3240939

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balters, S., and Steinert, M. (2017). Capturing emotion reactivity through physiology measurement as a foundation for affective engineering in engineering design science and engineering practices. J. Intell. Manuf. 28, 1585–1607. doi: 10.1007/s10845-015-1145-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baltodano, S., Garcia-Mancilla, J., and Ju, W. (2018). “Eliciting driver stress using naturalistic driving scenarios on real roads,” in Proceedings of the 10th International Conference on Automotive User Interfaces and Interactive Vehicular Applications (Toronto, ON). doi: 10.1145/3239060.3239090

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barnard, M. P., and Chapman, P. (2018). The effects of instruction and environmental demand on state anxiety, driving performance and autonomic activity: are ego-threatening manipulations effective? Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 55, 123–135. doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2018.02.040

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belakhdar, I., Kaaniche, W., Djemal, R., and Ouni, B. (2018). Single-channel-based automatic drowsiness detection architecture with a reduced number of EEG features. Microprocess. Microsyt. 58, 13–23. doi: 10.1016/j.micpro.2018.02.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belyusar, D., Mehler, B., Solovey, E., and Reimer, B. (2015). Impact of repeated exposure to a multilevel working memory task on physiological arousal and driving performance. Transp. Res. Rec. J. Transp. Res. Board 2518, 46–53. doi: 10.3141/2518-06

CrossRef Full Text

Berntson, G. G., Quigley, K. S., and Lozano, D. (2007). Cardiovascular psychophysiology. Handb. Psychophysiol. 3, 182–210. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511546396.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Billman, G. E. (2013). The LF/HF ratio does not accurately measure cardiac sympatho-vagal balance. Front. Physiol. 4:26. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2013.00026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biondi, F. N., Lohani, M., Hopman, R., Mills, S., Cooper, J. M., and Strayer, D. L. (2018). “80 MPH and out-of-the-loop: effects of real-world semi-automated driving on driver workload and arousal,” in Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting (Sage, CA; Los Angeles, CA: SAGE Publications).

Google Scholar

Blöcher, T., Schneider, J., Schinle, M., and Stork, W. (2017). “An online PPGI approach for camera based heart rate monitoring using beat-to-beat detection,” in Sensors Applications Symposium (SAS), 2017 IEEE. doi: 10.1109/SAS.2017.7894052

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borghini, G., Aricò, P., Di Flumeri, G., Salinari, S., Colosimo, A., Bonelli, S., et al. (2015). “Avionic technology testing by using a cognitive neurometric index: a study with professional helicopter pilots,” in Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC), 2015 37th Annual International Conference of the IEEE (Milan). doi: 10.1109/EMBC.2015.7319804

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borghini, G., Astolfi, L., Vecchiato, G., Mattia, D., and Babiloni, F. (2014). Measuring neurophysiological signals in aircraft pilots and car drivers for the assessment of mental workload, fatigue and drowsiness. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 44, 58–75. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boucsein, W., Fowles, D. C., Grimnes, S., Ben-Shakhar, G., Roth, W. T., Dawson, M. E., et al. (2012). Publication recommendations for electrodermal measurements. Psychophysiology 49, 1017–1034. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01384.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Broadhurst, P. L. (1959). The interaction of task difficulty and motivation: the Yerkes Dodson Law revived. Acta Psychol. 16, 321–338. doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(59)90105-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brookhuis, K. A., and de Waard, D. (1993). The use of psychophysiology to assess driver status. Ergonomics 36, 1099–1110. doi: 10.1080/00140139308967981

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brookhuis, K. A., and de Waard, D. (2010). Monitoring drivers' mental workload in driving simulators using physiological measures. Accid. Anal. Prev. 42, 898–903. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2009.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brouwer, A., Snelting, A., Jaswa, M., Flascher, O., Krol, L., and Zander, T. (2017). “Physiological effects of adaptive cruise control behaviour in real driving,” in Proceedings of the 2017 ACM Workshop on An Application-oriented Approach to BCI out of the Laboratory (Limassol). doi: 10.1145/3038439.3038441

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Byrom, B., McCarthy, M., Schueler, P., and Muehlhausen, W. (2018). Brain monitoring devices in neuroscience clinical research: the potential of remote monitoring using sensors, wearables, and mobile devices. Clin. Pharmacol. Ther. 104, 59–71. doi: 10.1002/cpt.1077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Byun, K., Hyodo, K., Suwabe, K., Ochi, G., Sakairi, Y., Kato, M., et al. (2014). Positive effect of acute mild exercise on executive function via arousal-related prefrontal activations: an fNIRS study. Neuroimage 98, 336–345. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.04.067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cacioppo, J. T., and Tassinary, L. G. (1990). Inferring psychological significance from physiological signals. Am. Psychol. 45:16. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.45.1.16

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cacioppo, J. T., Tassinary, L. G., and Berntson, G. G. (2007). “Psychophysiological science: interdisciplinary approaches to classic questions about the mind,” in Handbook of Psychophysiology, eds J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, and G. G. Berntson (NewYork, NY: Cambridge University Press), 1–16. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511546396.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cassidy, S. M., Robertson, I. H., and O'Connell, R. G. (2012). Retest reliability of event-related potentials: evidence from a variety of paradigms. Psychophysiology 49, 659–664. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01349.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Causse, M., Chua, Z., Peysakhovich, V., Campo, N., and Matton, N. (2017). Mental workload and neural efficiency quantified in the prefrontal cortex using fNIRS. Sci. Rep. 7:5222. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-05378-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cavanagh, J. F., and Frank, M. J. (2014). Frontal theta as a mechanism for cognitive control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 414–421. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.04.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cegovnik, T., Stojmenova, K., Jakus, G., and Sodnik, J. (2018). An analysis of the suitability of a low-cost eye tracker for assessing the cognitive load of drivers. Appl. Ergon. 68, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.apergo.2017.10.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, L. L., Zhao, Y., Ye, P. F., Zhang, J., and Zou, J. Z. (2017). Detecting driving stress in physiological signals based on multimodal feature analysis and kernel classifiers. Expert Syst. Appl. 85, 279–291. doi: 10.1016/j.eswa.2017.01.040

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, L. L., Zhao, Y., Zhang, J., and Zou, J. Z. (2015). Automatic detection of alertness/drowsiness from physiological signals using wavelet-based nonlinear features and machine learning. Expert Systems Appl. 42, 7344–7355. doi: 10.1016/j.eswa.2015.05.028

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheng, S. Y., Park, S., and Trivedi, M. M. (2007). Multi-spectral and multi-perspective video arrays for driver body tracking and activity analysis. Comput. Vis. Image Underst. 106, 245–257. doi: 10.1016/j.cviu.2006.08.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, M. X. (2011). It's about time. Front. Human Neurosci. 5:2. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Correa, A. G., Orosco, L., and Laciar, E. (2014). Automatic detection of drowsiness in EEG records based on multimodal analysis. Med. Eng. Phys. 36, 244–249. doi: 10.1016/j.medengphy.2013.07.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Costa, N., Simões, P., Costa, S., and Arezes, P. (2017). “Driving workload indicators: the case of senior drivers,” in International Conference on Applied Human Factors and Ergonomics (Cham: Springer), 604–615.

Google Scholar

Dasari, D., Shou, G., and Ding, L. (2017). ICA-Derived EEG correlates to mental fatigue, effort, and workload in a realistically simulated air traffic control task. Front. Neurosci. 11:297. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00297

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dawson, M. E., Schell, A. M., and Filion, D. L. (2007). The electrodermal system. Handb. Psychophysiol. 2, 200–223. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511546396.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Waard, D. (1996). The Measurement of Drivers' Mental Workload. Netherlands: Groningen University, Traffic Research Center.

Google Scholar

de Waard, D., and Lewis-Evans, B. (2014). Self-report scales alone cannot capture mental workload. Cogn. Technol. Work 16, 303–305. doi: 10.1007/s10111-014-0277-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Demberg, V. (2013). “Pupillometry: the index of cognitive activity in a dual-task study,” in Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Vol. 35. Available online at: https://escholarship.org/uc/item/4vf5w6bn#author

Google Scholar

Drews, F. A., Strayer, D. L., Uchino, B., and Smith, T. W. (2012). “The road to road rage,” in Vision in Vehicles X, eds A. G. Gale, S. P. Taylor, and C. Castro (Elsevier), 346–352.

Duchowski, A. T., Krejtz, K., Krejtz, I., Biele, C., Niedzielska, A., Kiefer, P., et al. (2018). “The index of pupillary activity: measuring cognitive load vis-à-vis task difficulty with pupil oscillation,” in Proceedings of the 2018 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (Montreal, QC). doi: 10.1145/3173574.3173856

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

ElKomy, M., Abdelrahman, Y., Funk, M., Dingler, T., Schmidt, A., and Abdennadher, S. (2017). “ABBAS: an adaptive bio-sensors based assistive system,” Proceedings of the 2017 CHI Conference Extended Abstracts on Human Factors in Computing Systems, CHI EA '17 (Denver, CO; New York, NY: ACM), 2543–2550. doi: 10.1145/3027063.3053179

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engström, J., Johansson, E., and Östlund, J. (2005). Effects of visual and cognitive load in real and simulated motorway driving. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 8, 97–120. doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2005.04.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eoh, H. J., Chung, M. K., and Kim, S. H. (2005). Electroencephalographic study of drowsiness in simulated driving with sleep deprivation. Int. J. Indus. Ergon. 35, 307–320. doi: 10.1016/j.ergon.2004.09.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fabiani, M., Gratton, G., and Federmeier, K. D. (2007). “Event-related brain potentials: methods, theory, and applications,” in Handbook of Psychophysiology, eds J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, and G. G. Berntson (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 85–119. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511546396.004

CrossRef Full Text

Fairclough, S. H., Karran, A. J., and Gilleade, K. (2015). “Classification accuracy from the perspective of the user: real-time interaction with physiological computing,” in Proceedings of the 33rd Annual ACM Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (Seoul), 3029–3038. doi: 10.1145/2702123.2702454

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fallahi, M., Motamedzade, M., Heidarimoghadam, R., Soltanian, A. R., and Miyake, S. (2016). Effects of mental workload on physiological and subjective responses during traffic density monitoring: a field study. Appl. Ergon. 52, 95–103. doi: 10.1016/j.apergo.2015.07.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faure, V., Lobjois, R., and Benguigui, N. (2016). The effects of driving environment complexity and dual tasking on drivers' mental workload and eye blink behavior. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 40, 78–90. doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2016.04.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forsman, P. M., Vila, B. J., Short, R. A., Mott, C. G., and van Dongen, H. P. A. (2013). Efficient driver drowsiness detection at moderate levels of drowsiness. Accid. Anal. Prev. 50, 341–350. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2012.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fowles, D. C. (1986). The eccrine system and electrodermal activity. Psychophysiology 1, 51–96. doi: 10.4236/ojpsych.2013.31007

CrossRef Full Text

Fridlund, A. J., and Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). Guidelines for human electromyographic research. Psychophysiology 23, 567–589. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1986.tb00676.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fu, R., and Wang, H. (2014). Detection of driving fatigue by using noncontact EMG and ECG signals measurement system. Int. J. Neural Syst. 24:1450006. doi: 10.1142/S0129065714500063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fu, R., Wang, H., and Zhao, W. (2016). Dynamic driver fatigue detection using hidden Markov model in real driving condition. Expert Syst. Appl. 63, 397–411. doi: 10.1016/j.eswa.2016.06.042

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gade, R., and Moeslund, T. B. (2014). Thermal cameras and applications: a survey. Mach. Vis. Appl. 25, 245–262. doi: 10.1007/s00138-013-0570-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gagl, B., Hawelka, S., and Hutzler, F. (2011). Systematic influence of gaze position on pupil size measurement: analysis and correction. Behav. Res. Methods 43, 1171–1181. doi: 10.3758/s13428-011-0109-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gao, Z., Li, S., Cai, Q., Dang, W., Yang, Y., Mu, C., et al. (2018). Relative wavelet entropy complex network for improving EEG-based fatigue driving classification. IEEE Trans. Instrum. Meas. 99, 1–7. doi: 10.1109/TIM.2018.2865842

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gateau, T., Durantin, G., Lancelot, F., Scannella, S., and Dehais, F. (2015). Real-time state estimation in a flight simulator using fNIRS. PLoS ONE 10:e0121279. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0121279

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Getzmann, S., Arnau, S., Karthaus, M., Reiser, J. E., and Wascher, E. (2018). Age-related differences in pro-active driving behaviour revealed by EEG measures. Front. Human Neurosci. 12:321. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00321

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilzenrat, M. S. (2006). The Role of the Locus Coeruleus in Cognitive Control: Pupillometric Measures and Computational Mechanisms. 0724–0724. Ph.D. thesis, Princeton University.

Gratton, G., and Fabiani, M. (2001). Shedding light on brain function: the event-related optical signal. Trends Cogn. Sci. 5, 357–363. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01701-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gratton, G., and Fabiani, M. (2003). The event-related optical signal (EROS) in visual cortex: replicability, consistency, localization, and resolution. Psychophysiology 40, 561–571. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.00058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grinvald, A., Lieke, E., Frostig, R. D., Gilbert, C. D., and Wiesel, T. N. (1986). Functional architecture of cortex revealed by optical imaging of intrinsic signals. Nature 324, 361–364. doi: 10.1038/324361a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grossman, P. (1992). Respiratory and cardiac rhythms as windows to central and autonomic biobehavioral regulation: selection of window frames, keeping the panes clean and viewing the neural topography. Biol. Psychol. 34, 131–161. doi: 10.1016/0301-0511(92)90013-K

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guoping, S., and Zhang, K. (2009). An ERP study of effects of driving fatigue on auditory attention. Psychol. Sci. 32, 517–520. Available online at: https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2009-12822-001

Google Scholar

Haouij, N. E., Poggi, J.-M., Sevestre-Ghalila, S., Ghozi, R., and Jaïdane, M. (2018). “AffectiveROAD system and database to assess driver's attention,” in Proceedings of the 33rd Annual ACM Symposium on Applied Computing (Pau). doi: 10.1145/3167132.3167395

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Healey, J., Seger, J., and Picard, R. (1999). Quantifying driver stress: developing a system for collecting and processing bio-metric signals in natural situations. Biomed. Sci. Instrum. 35, 193–198.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Hebb, D. O. (1955). Drives and the CNS (conceptual nervous system). Psychol. Rev. 62:243. doi: 10.1037/h0041823

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heine, T., Lenis, G., Reichensperger, P., Beran, T., Doessel, O., and Deml, B. (2017). Electrocardiographic features for the measurement of drivers' mental workload. Appl. Ergon. 61, 31–43. doi: 10.1016/j.apergo.2016.12.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herff, C., Putze, F., and Schultz, T. (2017). “Evaluating fNIR-based workload discrimination in a realistic driving scenario,” in The First Biannual Neuroadaptive Technology, Conference (Berlin).

Hess, E. H., and Polt, J. M. (1964). Pupil size in relation to mental activity during simple problem-solving. Science 143, 1190–1192. doi: 10.1126/science.143.3611.1190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hidalgo-Muñoz, A. R., Béquet, A. J., Astier-Juvenon, M., Pépin, G., Fort, A., Jallais, C., et al. (2018). Respiration and heart rate modulation due to competing cognitive tasks while driving. Front. Human Neurosci. 12:525. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00525

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hirao, A., Kato, K., Kitazaki, S., and Yamazaki, N. (2007). Evaluations of Physical Fatigue During Long-Term Driving with a New Driving Posture. SAE Technical Paper, SAE World Congress & Exhibition (No. 2007-01-0348). doi: 10.4271/2007-01-0348

CrossRef Full Text

Horat, S. K., Herrmann, F. R., Favre, G., Terzis, J., Debatisse, D., Merlo, M. C., et al. (2016). Assessment of mental workload: a new electrophysiological method based on intra-block averaging of ERP amplitudes. Neuropsychologia 82, 11–17. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.12.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hostens, I., and Ramon, H. (2005). Assessment of muscle fatigue in low level monotonous task performance during car driving. J. Electromyogr. Kinesiol. 15, 266–274. doi: 10.1016/j.jelekin.2004.08.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hung, Y. C., Wang, Y. K., Prasad, M., and Lin, C. T. (2017). “Brain dynamic states analysis based on 3D convolutional neural network,” in Systems, Man, and Cybernetics (SMC), 2017 IEEE International Conference (Banff, AB), 222–227. doi: 10.1109/SMC.2017.8122606

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacob, S., Bukhari, S. S., Ishimaru, S., and Dengel, A. (2018). “Gaze-based interest detection on newspaper articles,” in Proceedings of the 7th Workshop on Pervasive Eye Tracking and Mobile Eye-Based Interaction (Warsaw), 4. doi: 10.1145/3208031.3208034

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jagannath, M., and Balasubramanian, V. (2014). Assessment of early onset of driver fatigue using multimodal fatigue measures in a static simulator. Appl. Ergon. 45, 1140–1147. doi: 10.1016/j.apergo.2014.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Janisse, M. P. (1977). Pupillometry: The Psychology of the Pupillary Response. Halsted Press.

Jasper, P., Sibley, C., and Coyne, J. (2016). “Using heart rate variability to assess operator mental workload in a command and control simulation of multiple unmanned aerial vehicles,” in Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting, Vol. 60 (Sage CA; Los Angeles, CA: SAGE Publications), 1125–1129. doi: 10.1177/1541931213601264

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jennings, J. R., and Allen, B. (2016). “Methodology,” Chapter. in Handbook of Psychophysiology, 4th Edn. eds J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, and G. G. Berntson, 583–611. Cambridge Handbooks in Psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/9781107415782.027

CrossRef Full Text

Jennings, J. R., Bberg, W. K., Hutcheson, J. S., Obrist, P., Porges, S., and Turpin, G. (1981). Publication guidelines for heart rate studies in man. Psychophysiology 18, 226–231. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1981.tb03023.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jensen, O., and Mazaheri, A. (2010). Shaping functional architecture by oscillatory alpha activity: gating by inhibition. Front. Human Neurosci. 4:186. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00186

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jobsis, F. F. (1977). Noninvasive, infrared monitoring of cerebral and myocardial oxygen sufficiency and circulatory parameters. Science 198, 1264–1267. doi: 10.1126/science.929199

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johansson, G., Evans, G. W., Cederström, C., Rydstedt, L. W., Fuller-Rowell, T., and Ong, A. D. (2012). The effects of urban bus driving on blood pressure and musculoskeletal problems: a quasi-experimental study. Psychosom. Med. 74, 89–92. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31823ba88f

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jonsson, S., and Jonsson, B. (1975). Function of the muscles of the upper limb in car driving. Ergonomics 18, 375–388. doi: 10.1080/00140137508931471

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kahana, M. J. (2006). The cognitive correlates of human brain oscillations. J. Neurosci. 26, 1669–1672. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3737-05c.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kahneman, D., and Beatty, J. (1966). Pupil diameter and load on memory. Science 154, 1583–1585. doi: 10.1126/science.154.3756.1583

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kajiwara, S. (2014). Evaluation of driver's mental workload by facial temperature and electrodermal activity under simulated driving conditions. Int. J. Automot. Technol. 15, 65–70.

Google Scholar

Kassab, A., Le Lan, J., Tremblay, J., Vannasing, P., Dehbozorgi, M., Pouliot, P., et al. (2018). Multichannel wearable fNIRS-EEG system for long-term clinical monitoring. Human Brain Mapp. 39, 7–23. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23849

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kassem, K., Salah, J., Abdrabou, Y., Morsy, M., El-Gendy, R., Abdelrahman, Y., et al. (2017). “DiVA: exploring the usage of pupil diameter to elicit valence and arousal,” in Proceedings of the 16th International Conference on Mobile and Ubiquitous Multimedia (Stuttgart). doi: 10.1145/3152832.3152836

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Käthner, I., Wriessnegger, S. C., Müller-Putz, G. R., Kübler, A., and Halder, S. (2014). Effects of mental workload and fatigue on the P300, alpha and theta band power during operation of an ERP (P300) brain–computer interface. Biol. Psychol. 102, 118–129. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.07.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kato, T., Fujii, T., and Tanimoto, M. (2004). “Detection of driver's posture in the car by using far infrared camera,” in Intelligent Vehicles Symposium, 2004 (Parma). doi: 10.1109/IVS.2004.1336406

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ko, L. W., Komarov, O., Hairston, W. D., Jung, T. P., and Lin, C. T. (2017). Sustained attention in real classroom settings: an eeg study. Front. Human Neurosci. 11:388. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00388

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kolli, A., Fasih, A., Al Machot, F., and Kyamakya, K. (2011). “Non-intrusive car driver's emotion recognition using thermal camera,” in Nonlinear Dynamics and Synchronization (INDS) & 16th Int'l Symposium on Theoretical Electrical Engineering (Klagenfurt). doi: 10.1109/INDS.2011.6024802

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kosiachenko, E., and Si, D. (2017). Classification of brain activity using brainwaves recorded by consumer-grade EEG devices. Adv. Mass Data, 14.

Google Scholar

Krol, L. R., Zander, T. O., Jaswa, M., Flascher, O., Snelting, A., and Brouwer, A.-M. (2017). “Online-capable cleaning of highly artefactual EEG data recorded during real driving,” in Proceedings of the 7th Graz Brain-Computer Interface Conference. doi: 10.3217/978-3-85125-533-1-47

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kurosawa, K., Takezawa, N., Sano, T., Miyamoto, S., Yasushi, M., and Hashimoto, H. (2017). “Drowsiness prediction system for vehicle using capacity coupled electrode type non-invasive ECG measurement,” in System Integration (SII), 2017 IEEE/SICE International Symposium. doi: 10.1109/SII.2017.8279230

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laborde, S., Mosley, E., and Thayer, J. F. (2017). Heart rate variability and cardiac vagal tone in psychophysiological research–recommendations for experiment planning, data analysis, and data reporting. Front. Psychol. 8:213. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Le, A. S., Aoki, H., Murase, F., and Ishida, K. (2018). A novel method for classifying driver cognitive distraction under naturalistic conditions with information from near-infrared spectroscopy. Front. Human Neurosci. 12:431. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00431

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, B. G., Chong, T. W., Lee, B. L., Park, H. J., Kim, Y. N., and Kim, B. (2017a). Wearable mobile-based emotional response-monitoring system for drivers. IEEE Trans. Human Mach. Syst. 47, 636–649. doi: 10.1109/THMS.2017.2658442

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, D. S., Chong, T. W., and Lee, B. G. (2017b). Stress events detection of driver by wearable glove system. IEEE Sens. J. 17, 194–204. doi: 10.1109/JSEN.2016.2625323

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, H. B., Kim, J. S., Kim, Y. S., Baek, H. J., Ryu, M. S., and Park, K. S. (2007). “The relationship between HRV parameters and stressful driving situation in the real road,” in 6th International Special Topic Conference on Information Technology Applications in Biomedicine, 2007 (Tokyo). doi: 10.1109/ITAB.2007.4407380

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, K. W., Yoon, H. S., Song, J. M., and Park, K. R. (2018). Convolutional neural network-based classification of driver's emotion during aggressive and smooth driving using multi-modal camera sensors. Sensors 18:957. doi: 10.3390/s1804095

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leinonen, V., Kankaanpää, M., Vanharanta, H., Airaksinen, O., and Hänninen, O. (2005). Back and neck extensor loading and back pain provocation in urban bus drivers with and without low back pain. Pathophysiology 12, 249–255. doi: 10.1016/j.pathophys.2005.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lenneman, J. K., and Backs, R. W. (2009). Cardiac autonomic control during simulated driving with a concurrent verbal working memory task. Human Fact. 51, 404–418. doi: 10.1177/0018720809337716

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lenneman, J. K., and Backs, R. W. (2010). “Enhancing assessment of in-vehicle technology attention demands with cardiac measures,” in Proceedings of the 2nd International Conference on Automotive User Interfaces and Interactive Vehicular Applications. doi: 10.1145/1969773.1969777

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, Z., Chen, L., Peng, J., and Wu, Y. (2017). Automatic detection of driver fatigue using driving operation information for transportation safety. Sensors 17:1212. doi: 10.3390/s17061212

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, Z., Zhang, M., Zhang, X., Dai, S., Yu, X., and Wang, Y. (2009). Assessment of cerebral oxygenation during prolonged simulated driving using near infrared spectroscopy: its implications for fatigue development. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 107, 281–287. doi: 10.1007/s00421-009-1122-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liang, B., and Lin, Y. (2018). Using physiological and behavioral measurements in a picture-based road hazard perception experiment to classify risky and safe drivers. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 58, 93–105. doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2018.05.024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liang, S. F., Lin, C. T., Wu, R. C., Chen, Y. C., Huang, T. Y., and Jung, T. P. (2006). “Monitoring driver's alertness based on the driving performance estimation and the EEG power spectrum analysis,” in 2005 IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology 27th Annual Conference (Shanghai). doi: 10.1109/IEMBS.2005.1615791

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liang, W. C., Yuan, J., Sun, D. C., and Lin, M. H. (2009). Changes in physiological parameters induced by indoor simulated driving: effect of lower body exercise at mid-term break. Sensors 9, 6913–6933. doi: 10.3390/s90906913

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liang, Y., Horrey, W. J., Howard, M. E., Lee, M. L., Anderson, C., Shreeve, M. S., et al. (2017). Prediction of drowsiness events in night shift workers during morning driving. Accid. Anal. Prev. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2017.11.004. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lim, W. L., Sourina, O., and Wang, L. P. (2018). STEW: Simultaneous task EEG workload dataset. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng. 26, 2106–2114. doi: 10.1109/TNSRE.2018.2872924

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, C. T., Wu, R. C., Liang, S. F., Chao, W. H., Chen, Y. J., and Jung, T. P. (2005). EEG-based drowsiness estimation for safety driving using independent component analysis. IEEE Trans. Circuits Syst. I Regular Papers 52, 2726–2738. doi: 10.1109/TCSI.2005.857555

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Littler, W. A., Honour, A. J., and Sleight, P. (1973). Direct arterial pressure and electrocardiogram during motor car driving. Br. Med. J. 2, 273–277. doi: 10.1136/bmj.2.5861.273

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, T., Pelowski, M., Pang, C., Zhou, Y., and Cai, J. (2016). Near-infrared spectroscopy as a tool for driving research. Ergonomics 59, 368–379. doi: 10.1080/00140139.2015.1076057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, T., Saito, H., and Oi, M. (2012). Distinctive activation patterns under intrinsically versus extrinsically driven cognitive loads in prefrontal cortex: a near-infrared spectroscopy study using a driving video game. Neurosci. Lett. 506, 220–224. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2011.11.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, Y., and Du, S. (2018). Psychological stress level detection based on electrodermal activity. Behav. Brain Res. 341, 50–53. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.12.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, Y.-T., Lin, Y.-T., Wu, S.-L., Hsieh, T.-Y., and Lin, C.-T. (2015). “Assessment of mental fatigue: an EEG-based forecasting system for driving safety,” in 2015 IEEE International Conference on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics (Kowloon). doi: 10.1109/SMC.2015.561

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lohani, M., and Isaacowitz, D. M. (2014). Age differences in managing response to sadness elicitors using attentional deployment, positive reappraisal and suppression. Cogn. Emot. 28, 678–697. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2013.853648

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lohani, M., Payne, B. R., and Isaacowitz, D. M. (2018). Emotional coherence in early and later adulthood during sadness reactivity and regulation. Emotion 18, 789–804. doi: 10.1037/emo0000345

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luck, S. J., and Kappenman, E. S. (2012). Oxford Handbook of ERP Components. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Luijcks, R., Hermens, H. J., Bodar, L., Vossen, C. J., van Os, J., and Lousberg, R. (2014). Experimentally induced stress validated by EMG activity. PLoS ONE 9:e95215. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0095215

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lundberg, U., Kadefors, R., Melin, B., Palmerud, G., Hassmén, P., Engström, M., et al. (1994). Psychophysiological stress and EMG activity of the trapezius muscle. Int. J. Behav. Med. 354–370. doi: 10.1207/s15327558ijbm0104_5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luque-Casado, A., Perales, J. C., Cárdenas, D., and Sanabria, D. (2016). Heart rate variability and cognitive processing: the autonomic response to task demands. Biol. Psychol. 113, 83–90. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.11.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lyman, J. M., McGwin, G. Jr., and Sims, R. V. (2001). Factors related to driving difficulty and habits in older drivers. Accid. Anal. Prev. 33, 413–421. doi: 10.1016/S0001-4575(00)00055-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, Z., Yao, S., and Li, J. (2018). “A study of integrated driver fatigue judging algorithm based on principal component analysis,” in MATEC Web of Conferences, (EDP Sciences) (Nanjing). doi: 10.1051/matecconf/201817302011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maccora, J., Manousakis, J. E., and Anderson, C. (2018). Pupillary instability as an accurate, objective marker of alertness failure and performance impairment. J. Sleep Res. e12739. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12739

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Madan, C. R., Harrison, T., and Mathewson, K. E. (2018). Noncontact measurement of emotional and physiological changes in heart rate from a webcam. Psychophysiology 55:e13005. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marinescu, A. C., Sharples, S., Ritchie, A. C., Sánchez López, T., McDowell, M., and Morvan, H. P. (2018). Physiological parameter response to variation of mental workload. Hum. Factors 60, 31–56. doi: 10.1177/0018720817733101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maris, E., and Oostenveld, R. (2007). Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG-and MEG-data. J. Neurosci. Methods 164, 177–190. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.03.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marquart, G., Cabrall, C., and de Winter, J. (2015). Review of eye-related measures of drivers' mental workload. Procedia Manuf. 3, 2854–2861. doi: 10.1016/j.promfg.2015.07.783

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marshall, S. P. (2002). “The index of cognitive activity: measuring cognitive workload,” in Human Factors and Power Plants, 2002. Proceedings of the 2002 IEEE 7th Conference (Scottsdale, AZ). doi: 10.1109/HFPP.2002.1042860

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Massar, S. A. A., Wester, A. E., Volkerts, E. R., and Kenemans, J. L. (2010). Manipulation specific effects of mental fatigue: evidence from novelty processing and simulated driving. Psychophysiology 47, 1119–1126. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01028.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mathewson, K. E., Gratton, G., Fabiani, M., Beck, D. M., and Ro, T. (2009). To see or not to see: prestimulus α phase predicts visual awareness. J. Neurosci. 29, 2725–2732. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3963-08.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matthews, G., Wohleber, R., Lin, J., Funke, G., and Neubauer, C. (2019). “Monitoring task fatigue in contemporary and future vehicles: a review,” in Advances in Human Factors in Simulation and Modeling. AHFE 2018. Advances in Intelligent Systems and Computing, Vol. 780, ed D. Cassenti (Cham: Springer) doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-94223-0_10

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mauss, I. B., and Robinson, M. D. (2009). Measures of emotion: a review. Cogn. Emot. 23, 209–237. doi: 10.1080/02699930802204677

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mazzetta, I., Gentile, P., Pessione, M., Suppa, A., Zampogna, A., Bianchini, E., et al. (2018). Stand-alone wearable system for ubiquitous real-time monitoring of muscle activation potentials. Sensors 18:6. doi: 10.3390/s18061748

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McKendrick, R., Parasuraman, R., Murtza, R., Formwalt, A., Baccus, W., Paczynski, M., et al. (2016). Into the wild: neuroergonomic differentiation of hand-held and augmented reality wearable displays during outdoor navigation with functional near infrared spectroscopy. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:216. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00216

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mehler, B., Reimer, B., and Coughlin, J. F. (2012). Sensitivity of physiological measures for detecting systematic variations in cognitive demand from a working memory task: an on-road study across three age groups. Hum. Factors 54, 396–412. doi: 10.1177/0018720812442086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mehler, B., Reimer, B., Coughlin, J. F., and Dusek, J. A. (2009). The impact of incremental increases in cognitive workload on physiological arousal and performance in young adult drivers. Transport. Res. Rec. 2138, 6–12. doi: 10.3141/2138-02

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mehler, B., Reimer, B., and Wang, Y. (2011). “A comparison of heart rate and heart rate variability indices in distinguishing single-task driving and driving under secondary cognitive workload,” in Proceedings of the 6th International Driving Symposium on Human Factors in Driver Assessment, Training, and Vehicle Design: Driving Assessment, 2011 (Lake Tahoe, CA), 590–597. doi: 10.17077/drivingassessment.1451

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, N. R., Newman, N. J., Biousse, V., and Kerrison, J. B. (2005). Walsh and Hoyt's Clinical-Neuroophthalmology, 6th Edn. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins

Google Scholar

Moghaddam, G. K., Lowe, C. R., (eds.). (2019). “Ex vivo biosignatures,” in Health and Wellness Measurement Approaches for Mobile Healthcare. (Cham: Springer), 51–104. doi: 10.1007/978-3-030-01557-2_3

CrossRef Full Text

Morad, Y., Lemberg, H., Yofe, N., and Dagan, Y. (2000). Pupillography as an objective indicator of fatigue. Curr. Eye Res. 21, 535–542. doi: 10.1076/0271-3683(200007)2111-ZFT535

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morales, J. M., Diaz-Piedra, C., Rieiro, H., Roca-González, J., Romero, S., Catena, A., et al. (2017). Monitoring driver fatigue using a single-channel electroencephalographic device: a validation study by gaze-based, driving performance, and subjective data. Accid. Anal. Prev. 109, 62–69. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2017.09.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, D. M., Erno, J. M., and Pilcher, J. J. (2017). “Electrodermal response and automation trust during simulated self-driving car use,” in Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting, (Los Angeles, CA: SAGE Publications), 61, 1759–1762. doi: 10.1177/1541931213601921

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mun, S., Whang, M., Park, S., and Park, M. (2017). Effects of mental workload on involuntary attention: a somatosensory ERP study. Neuropsychologia 106, 7–20. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.08.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murai, K., Hayashi, Y., Okazaki, T., Stone, L. C., and Mitomo, N. (2008). “Evaluation of ship navigator's mental workload using nasal temperature and heart rate variability,” in Systems, Man and Cybernetics, 2008. SMC 2008. IEEE International Conference (Singapore). doi: 10.1109/ICSMC.2008.4811503

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nadeau, C., and Bengio, Y. (2000). “Inference for the generalization error,” in Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems, 307–313. doi: 10.1023/A:1024068626366

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neshov, N. N., Manolova, A. H., Draganov, I. R., Tonschev, K. T., and Boumbarov, O. L. (2018). Classification of Mental Tasks from EEG Signals Using Spectral Analysis, PCA and SVM. Cybern. Inf. Technol. 18, 81–92. doi: 10.2478/cait-2018-0007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noda, A., Miyaji, M., Wakuda, Y., Hara, Y., and Yasuma, F. (2015). Simultaneous measurement of heart rate variability and blinking duration to predict sleep onset and drowsiness in drivers. J. Sleep Disord Ther 4, 2167–0277. doi: 10.4172/2167-0277.1000213

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noordzij, M. L., Dorrestijn, S. M., and van den Berg, I. A. (2017). “An idiographic study into the physiology and self-reported mental workload of learning to drive a car,” in Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Europe Chapter 2016 Annual Conference (Prague). Available online at: https://research.utwente.nl/en/publications/an-idiographic-study-into-the-physiology-and-selfreported-mental-

Google Scholar

Nowara, E. M., Marks, T. K., Mansour, H., and Veeraraghavan, A. (2018). “SparsePPG: towards driver monitoring using camera-based vital signs estimation in near-infrared,” in Proceedings of the IEEE Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition Workshops (Salt Lake City, UT). doi: 10.1109/CVPRW.2018.00174

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nunez, P. L., and Srinivasan, R. (2006). Electric Fields of the Brain: The Neurophysics of EEG. Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195050387.001.0001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O'Donnell, R. D., and Eggemeier, F. T. (1986). “Workload assessment methodology,” in Handbook of perception and human performance. Vol. 2, Cognitive Processes and Performance, eds K. R. Boff, L. Kaufman, and J. P. Thomas (New York, NY: Wiley), 1–49.

Google Scholar

Or, C. K. L., and Duffy, V. G. (2007). Development of a facial skin temperature-based methodology for non-intrusive mental workload measurement. Occupational Ergonomics 7, 83–94. Available online at: https://content.iospress.com/articles/occupational-ergonomics/oer00140

Google Scholar

Ottaviani, C., Brosschot, J. F., Lonigro, A., Medea, B., Van Diest, I., and Thayer, J. F. (2016). Hemodynamic profiles of functional and dysfunctional forms of repetitive thinking. Ann. Behav. Med. 51, 261–271. doi: 10.1007/s12160-016-9851-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palinko, O., Kun, A. L., Shyrokov, A., and Heeman, P. (2010). “Estimating cognitive load using remote eye tracking in a driving simulator,” in Proceedings of the 2010 Symposium on Eye-Tracking Research & Applications (Austin, TX). doi: 10.1145/1743666.1743701

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paredes, P. E., Ordonez, F., Ju, W., and Landay, J. A. (2018). “Fast & furious: detecting stress with a car steering wheel,” in Proceedings of the 2018 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (Montreal, QC). doi: 10.1145/3173574.3174239

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patel, M., Lal, S. K., Kavanagh, D., and Rossiter, P. (2011). Applying neural network analysis on heart rate variability data to assess driver fatigue. Expert Syst. Appl. 38, 7235–7242. doi: 10.1016/j.eswa.2010.12.028

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perrier, J., Jongen, S., Vuurman, E., Bocca, M. L., Ramaekers, J. G., and Vermeeren, A. (2016). Driving performance and EEG fluctuations during on-the-road driving following sleep deprivation. Biol. Psychol. 121, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.09.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfleging, B., Fekety, D. K., Schmidt, A., and Kun, A. L. (2016). “A model relating pupil diameter to mental workload and lighting conditions,” in Proceedings of the 2016 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (San Jose, CA). doi: 10.1145/2858036.2858117

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piña-Ramírez, O., Valdés-Cristerna, R., Medina-Bañuelos, V., and Yañez-Suárez, O. (2018). “Chapter 7 - P300-based brain-computer interfaces,” in Smart Wheelchairs and Brain-Computer Interfaces, ed P. Diez (Academic Press), 131–170. Available online at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780128128923000078

Google Scholar

Piotrowski, Z., and Szypulska, M. (2017). Classification of falling asleep states using HRV analysis. Biocybern. Biomed. Eng. 37, 290–301. doi: 10.1016/j.bbe.2017.02.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piper, S. K., Krueger, A., Koch, S. P., Mehnert, J., Habermehl, C., Steinbrink, J., et al. (2014). A wearable multi-channel fNIRS system for brain imaging in freely moving subjects. Neuroimage 85, 64–71. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piquado, T., Isaacowitz, D. M., and Wingfield, A. (2010). Pupillometry as a measure of cognitive effort in younger and older adults. Psychophysiology 47, 560–569. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00947.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Polich, J. (2007). Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clin. Neurophysiol. 118, 2128–2148. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.04.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Popescu, F., Blankertz, B., and Müller, K. R. (2008). Computational challenges for noninvasive brain computer interfaces. IEEE Intell. Syst. 23, 78–79. Available online at: http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/summary?doi=10.1.1.370.8282

Google Scholar

Prabhakar, G., Madhu, N., and Biswas, P. (2018). “Comparing pupil dilation, head movement, and eeg for distraction detection of drivers,” in Proceedings of the 32nd International BCS Human Computer Interaction Conference (Belfast, UK). doi: 10.14236/ewic/HCI2018.69

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Putman, P., Verkuil, B., Arias-Garcia, E., Pantazi, I., and van Schie, C. (2014). EEG theta/beta ratio as a potential biomarker for attentional control and resilience against deleterious effects of stress on attention. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 14, 782–791. doi: 10.3758/s13415-013-0238-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Quaresima, V., Bisconti, S., and Ferrari, M. (2012). A brief review on the use of functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) for language imaging studies in human newborns and adults. Brain Lang. 121, 79–89. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2011.03.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Quigley, K. S., and Berntson, G. G. (1996). Autonomic interactions and chronotropic control of the heart: heart period versus heart rate. Psychophysiology 33, 605–611. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1996.tb02438.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Quintana, D. S., Elstad, M., Kaufmann, T., Brandt, C. L., Haatveit, B., Haram, M., et al. (2016). Resting-state high-frequency heart rate variability is related to respiratory frequency in individuals with severe mental illness but not healthy controls. Sci. Rep. 6:37212 doi: 10.1038/srep37212

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rai, M., Maity, T., and Yadav, R. K. (2017). Thermal imaging system and its real time applications: a survey. J. Eng. Technol. 6, 290–303. Available online at: http://www.joetsite.com/wp-content/uploads/2017/07/Vol.-62-24-2017.pdf

Google Scholar

Rajkowski, J., Kubiak, P., and Aston-Jones, G. (1994). Locus coeruleus activity in monkey: phasic and tonic changes are associated with altered vigilance. Brain Res. Bull. 35, 607-616. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(94)90175-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rastgoo, M. N., Nakisa, B., Rakotonirainy, A., Chandran, V., and Tjondronegoro, D. (2018). A critical review of proactive detection of driver stress levels based on multimodal measurements. ACM Comput. Surv. 51:88. doi: 10.1145/3186585

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Recarte, M. A., and Nunes, L. M. (2000). Effects of verbal and spatial-imagery tasks on eye fixations while driving. J. Exp. Psychol 6, 31–43. doi: 10.1037/1076-898X.6.1.31

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reilly, J., Kelly, A., Kim, S. H., Jett, S., and Zuckerman, B. (2018). The human task-evoked pupillary response function is linear: implications for baseline response scaling in pupillometry. Behav. Res. Methods 1–14. doi: 10.3758/s13428-018-1134-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reimer, B., Mehler, B., Coughlin, J. F., Godfrey, K. M., and Chuanzhong, T. (2009). “An on-road assessment of the impact of cognitive workload on physiological arousal in young adult drivers,” in Proceedings of the 1st International Conference on Automotive User Interfaces and Interactive Vehicular Applications, (ACM), 115–118. doi: 10.1145/1620509.1620531

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruscio, D., Bos, A. J., and Ciceri, M. R. (2017). Distraction or cognitive overload? using modulations of the autonomic nervous system to discriminate the possible negative effects of advanced assistance system. Accid. Anal. Prev. 103, 105–111. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2017.03.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sahayadhas, A., Sundaraj, K., and Murugappan, M. (2012). Detecting driver drowsiness based on sensors: a review. Sensors 12, 16937–16953. doi: 10.3390/s121216937

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saus, E. R., Johnsen, B. H., Eid, J., and Thayer, J. F. (2001). Who benefits from simulator training: Personality and heart rate variability in relation to situation awareness during navigation training? Comput. Hum. Behav. 28, 1262–1268. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2012.02.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Savchenko, V. V., and Poddubko, S. N. (2018). “Development approach to a method for monitoring of driver's ability of resumption of control over the vehicle by on-board systems in automatic mode,” in IOP Conference Series: Materials Science and Engineering. doi: 10.1088/1757-899X/386/1/012007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saxby, D. J., Matthews, G., Hitchcock, E. M., Warm, J. S., Funke, G. J., and Gantzer, T. (2008). “Effect of active and passive fatigue on performance using a driving simulator,” in Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting, Vol. 52, (Los Angeles, CA: Sage Publications), 1751–1755. doi: 10.1177/154193120805202113

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schäfer, A., and Vagedes, J. (2013). How accurate is pulse rate variability as an estimate of heart rate variability? a review on studies comparing photoplethysmographic technology with an electrocardiogram. Int. J. Cardiol. 166, 15–29. doi: 10.1016/j.ijcard.2012.03.119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schier, M. A. (2000). Changes in EEG alpha power during simulated driving: a demonstration. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 37, 155–162. doi: 10.1016/S0167-8760(00)00079-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, E., and Bullinger, A. C. (2017). Mitigating passive fatigue during monotonous drives with thermal stimuli: Insights into the effect of different stimulation durations. Accid. Anal. Prev. 4575, 30443–30448. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2017.12.005.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, E., Decke, R., Rasshofer, R., and Bullinger, A. C. (2017). Psychophysiological responses to short-term cooling during a simulated monotonous driving task. Appl. Ergon. 62, 9–18. doi: 10.1016/j.apergo.2017.01.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, E. A., Schrauf, M., Simon, M., Buchner, A., and Kincses, W. E. (2011). The short-term effect of verbally assessing drivers' state on vigilance indices during monotonous daytime driving. Transport. Res. Part F 14, 251–260. doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2011.01.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, E. A., Schrauf, M., Simon, M., Fritzsche, M., Buchner, A., and Kincses, W. E. (2009). Drivers' misjudgement of vigilance state during prolonged monotonous daytime driving. Accid. Anal. Prev. 41, 1087–1093. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2009.06.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneegass, S., Pfleging, B., Broy, N., Heinrich, F., and Schmidt, A. (2013). “A data set of real world driving to assess driver workload,” in Proceedings of the 5th International Conference on Automotive User Interfaces and Interactive Vehicular Applications (Eindhoven). doi: 10.1145/2516540.2516561

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwalm, M., Keinath, A., and Zimmer, H. D. (2008). “Pupillometry as a method for measuring mental workload within a simulated driving task,” in Human Factors for assistance and automation, eds F. Flemisch, B. Lorenz, H. Oberheid, K. A. Brookhuis, and D. de Waard (Maastricht: Shaker Publishing).

Google Scholar

Seo, S. H., Kwak, S. H., Chung, S. C., Kim, H. S., and Min, B. C. (2017). Effect of distraction task on driving performance of experienced taxi drivers. Asian Biomed. 11, 315–322. doi: 10.1515/abm-2018-0003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaffer, F., and Ginsberg, J. P. (2017). An overview of heart rate variability metrics and norms. Front. Public Health 5:258. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2017.00258

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shakouri, M., Ikuma, L. H., Aghazadeh, F., and Nahmens, I. (2018). Analysis of the sensitivity of heart rate variability and subjective workload measures in a driving simulator: the case of highway work zones. Int. J. Ind. Ergon. 66, 136–145. doi: 10.1016/j.ergon.2018.02.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shapiro, D., Jamner, L. D., Lane, J. D., Light, K. C., Myrtek, M., Sawada, Y., et al. (1996). Blood pressure publication guidelines. Psychophysiology 33, 1–12. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1996.tb02103.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheridan, T., Meyer, J., Roy, S., and Decker, K. (1991). Physiological and Psychological Evaluations of Driver Fatigue During Long Term Driving. SAE Technical Paper. doi: 10.4271/910116

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shirazi, A. S., Hassib, M., Henze, N., Schmidt, A., and Kunze, K. (2014). “What's on your mind?: mental task awareness using single electrode brain computer interfaces,” in Proceedings of the 5th Augmented Human International Conference (Kobe). doi: 10.1145/2582051.2582096

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sibi, S., Ayaz, H., Kuhns, D. P., Sirkin, D. M., and Ju, W. (2016). “Monitoring driver cognitive load using functional near infrared spectroscopy in partially autonomous cars,” in Intelligent Vehicles Symposium (IV), 2016 IEEE (Gothenburg). doi: 10.1109/IVS.2016.7535420

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simon, M., Schmidt, E. A., Kincses, W. E., Fritzsche, M., Bruns, A., Aufmuth, C., et al. (2011). EEG alpha spindle measures as indicators of driver fatigue under real traffic conditions. Clin. Neurophysiol. 122, 1168–1178. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2010.10.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simonson, E., Baker, C., Burns, N., Keiper, C., Schmitt, O. H., and Stackhouse, S. (1968). Cardiovascular stress (electrocardiographic changes) produced by driving an automobile. Am. Heart J. 75, 125–135. doi: 10.1016/0002-8703(68)90123-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siren, A., Hakamies-Blomqvist, L., and Lindeman, M. (2004). Driving cessation and health in older women. J. Appl. Gerontol. 23, 58–69. doi: 10.1177/0733464804263129

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sirevaag, E. J., Kramer, A. F., Coles, M. G. H., and Donchin, E. (1989). Resource reciprocity: an event-related brain potentials analysis. Acta Psychol. 70, 77–97. doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(89)90061-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sirois, S., and Brisson, J. (2014). Pupillometry. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 5, 679–692. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1323

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smallwood, J., and Schooler, J. W. (2006). The restless mind. Psychol. Bull. 132, 946–958. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.6.946

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soares, V. C. G., Souza, J. K. S., Ginani, G. E., Pompéia, S., Tierra-Criollo, C. J., and Melges, D. B. (2013). “Identification of drowsiness and alertness conditions by means of spectral F-test applied to pupillometric signals,” in Journal of Physics: Conference Series (Gothenburg), Vol. 477. doi: 10.1088/1742-6596/477/1/012027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Solís-Marcos, I., and Kircher, K. (2018). Event-related potentials as indices of mental workload while using an in-vehicle information system. Cogn. Technol. Work 1–13. doi: 10.1007/s10111-018-0485-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Solovey, E. T., Zec, M., Garcia Perez, E. A., Reimer, B., and Mehler, B. (2014). “Classifying driver workload using physiological and driving performance data: two field studies,” in Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (Toronto, ON). doi: 10.1145/2556288.2557068

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spruston, N. (2008). Pyramidal neurons: dendritic structure and synaptic integration. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 206–221. doi: 10.1038/nrn2286

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stemberger, J., Allison, R. S., and Schnell, T. (2010). “Thermal imaging as a way to classify cognitive workload,” in 2010 Canadian Conference on Computer and Robot Vision, IEEE (Ottawa, ON). doi: 10.1109/CRV.2010.37

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strayer, D. L., and Drews, F. A. (2007). Multi-tasking in the automobile. Attention: From theory to practice 121–133.

Google Scholar

Strayer, D. L., Turrill, J., Cooper, J. M., Coleman, J. R., Medeiros-Ward, N., and Biondi, F. (2015). Assessing cognitive distraction in the automobile. Hum. Factors 57, 1300–1324. doi: 10.1177/0018720815575149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stuiver, A., Brookhuis, K. A., de Waard, D., and Mulder, B. (2014). Short-term cardiovascular measures for driver support: increasing sensitivity for detecting changes in mental workload. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 92, 35–41. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.01.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stuiver, A., de Waard, D., Brookhuis, K. A., Dijksterhuis, C., Lewis-Evans, B., and Mulder, L. J. M. (2012). Short-term cardiovascular responses to changing task demands. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 85, 153–160. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sugie, R., Arakawa, T., and Kozuka, K (2016). Detection of fatigue in long-distance driving by heart rate variability. Innovat. Comput. Inform. Control Exp. Lett. 10, 1553–1559. Available online at: https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Toshiya_Arakawa2/publication/304757070_Detection_of_Fatigue_in_Long-distance_Driving_by_Heart_Rate_Variability/links/5779da3608ae4645d611f3cb.pdf

Google Scholar

Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology (1996). Heart rate variability, standards of measurement, physiological interpretation, and clinical use. Circulation 93, 1043–1065. doi: 10.1161/01.CIR.93.5.1043

CrossRef Full Text

Tozman, T., Magdas, E. S., MacDougall, H. G., and Vollmeyer, R. (2015). Understanding the psychophysiology of flow: a driving simulator experiment to investigate the relationship between flow and heart rate variability. Comput. Human Behav. 52, 408–418. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2015.06.023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tran, C. C., Yan, S., Habiyaremye, J. L., and Wei, Y. (2017). “Predicting driver's work performance in driving simulator based on physiological indices,” in International Conference on Intelligent Human Computer Interaction, (Cham: Springer), 150–162.

Google Scholar

Trivedi, M. M., Cheng, S. Y., Childers, E. M., and Krotosky, S. J. (2004). Occupant posture analysis with stereo and thermal infrared video: algorithms and experimental evaluation. IEEE Trans. Vehicul. Technol. 53, 1698–1712. doi: 10.1109/TVT.2004.835526

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsukahara, J. S., Harrison, T. L., and Engle, R. W. (2016). The relationship between baseline pupil size and intelligence. Cogn. Psychol. 91, 109–123. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2016.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsunashima, H., and Yanagisawa, K. (2009). Measurement of brain function of car driver using functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS). Comput. Intell. Neurosci. 2009:164958. doi: 10.1155/2009/164958

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Unni, A., Ihme, K., Jipp, M., and Rieger, J. W. (2017). Assessing the driver's current level of working memory load with high density functional near-infrared spectroscopy: a realistic driving simulator study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:167. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00167

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Boxtel, A. (2001). Optimal signal bandwidth for the recording of surface EMG activity of facial, jaw, oral, and neck muscles. Psychophysiology 38, 22–34. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.3810022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Gent, P., Farah, H., van Nes, N., and van Arem, B. (2018). Analysing noisy driver physiology real-time using off-the-self sensors: heart rate analysis software from the taking the fast lane project. J. Open Res. Softw. doi: 10.13140/RG.2.2.24895.56485

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vicente, J., Laguna, P., Bartra, A., and Bailón, R. (2016). Drowsiness detection using heart rate variability. Med. Biol. Eng. Comput. 54, 927–937. doi: 10.1007/s11517-015-1448-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Villringer, A., Planck, J., Hock, C., Schleinkofer, L., and Dirnagl, U. (1993). Near infrared spectroscopy (NIRS): a new tool to study hemodynamic changes during activation of brain function in human adults. Neurosci. Lett. 154, 101–104. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(93)90181-J

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Visnovcova, Z., Mestanik, M., Gala, M., Mestanikova, A., and Tonhajzerova, I. (2016). The complexity of electrodermal activity is altered in mental cognitive stressors. Comput. Biol. Med. 79, 123–129. doi: 10.1016/j.compbiomed.2016.10.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vogelpohl, T., Kühn, M., Hummel, T., and Vollrath, M. (2018). Asleep at the automated wheel—Sleepiness and fatigue during highly automated driving. Accid. Anal. Prev. 4575, 30117–30119. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2018.03.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

von Lühmann, A., Herff, C., Heger, D., and Schultz, T. (2015). Toward a wireless open source instrument: functional near-infrared spectroscopy in mobile neuroergonomics and BCI applications. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:617. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00617

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voytek, B., and Knight, R. T. (2015). Dynamic network communication as a unifying neural basis for cognition, development, aging, and disease. Biol. Psychiatry 77, 1089–1097. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.04.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, F., Wang, H., and Fu, R. (2018). Real-Time ECG-based detection of fatigue driving using sample entropy. Entropy 20:196. doi: 10.3390/e20030196

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, H. (2015). “Detection and alleviation of driving fatigue based on EMG and EMS/EEG using wearable sensor,” in Proceedings of the 5th EAI International Conference on Wireless Mobile Communication and Healthcare. doi: 10.4108/eai.14-10-2015.2261628

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, L., Wang, H., and Jiang, X. (2017). A new method to detect driver fatigue based on EMG and ECG collected by portable non-contact sensors. PROMET-Traffic Transport. 29, 479–488. doi: 10.7307/ptt.v29i5.2244

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, X., and Chuan, X. (2016). Driver drowsiness detection based on non-intrusive metrics considering individual specifics. Accid. Anal. Prev. 95, 350–357. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2015.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wanyan, X., Zhuang, D., Lin, Y., Xiao, X., and Song, J. -W. (2018). Influence of mental workload on detecting information varieties revealed by mismatch negativity during flight simulation. Int. J. Ind. Ergon. 64, 1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.ergon.2017.08.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wascher, E., Arnau, S., Gutberlet, I., Karthaus, M., and Getzmann, S. (2018). Evaluating pro-and re-active driving behavior by means of the EEG. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 12:205. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00205

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wascher, E., Rasch, B., Sänger, J., Hoffmann, S., Schneider, D., Rinkenauer, G., et al. (2014). Frontal theta activity reflects distinct aspects of mental fatigue. Biol. Psychol. 96, 57–65. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.11.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wei, C. S., Lin, Y. P., Wang, Y. T., Lin, C. T., and Jung, T. P. (2018). A subject-transfer framework for obviating inter-and intra-subject variability in EEG-based drowsiness detection. Neuroimage 174, 407–419. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.03.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wekselblatt, J. B., and Niell, C. M. (2015). Behavioral state—getting in the zone. Neuron 87, 7–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.06.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wijsman, J., Grundlehner, B., Penders, J., and Hermens, H. (2013). Trapezius muscle EMG as predictor of mental stress. ACM Trans. Embedded Comput. Syst. 12:99. doi: 10.1145/2485984.2485987

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wikström, B.-O. (1993). Effects from twisted postures and whole-body vibration during driving. Int. J. Ind. Ergon. 12, 61–75. doi: 10.1016/0169-8141(93)90038-F

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilhelm, B. J., Widmann, A., Durst, W., Heine, C., and Otto, G. (2009). Objective and quantitative analysis of daytime sleepiness in physicians after night duties. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 72, 307–313. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2009.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yakobi, O. (2018). “Determinants of Association and Dissociation between Subjective and Objective Measures of Workload,” in Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting, (Los Angeles, CA: SAGE Publications).

Google Scholar

Yan, S., Tran, C. C., Wei, Y., and Habiyaremye, J. L. (2017). Driver's mental workload prediction model based on physiological indices. Int. J. Occup. Saf. Ergon. 15, 1–9. doi: 10.1080/10803548.2017.1368951

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, S., Hosseiny, S. A. R., Susindar, S., and Ferris, T. K. (2016). “Investigating driver sympathetic arousal under short-term loads and acute stress events,” in Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting, Vol. 60, (Los Angeles, CA: SAGE Publications).

Google Scholar

Yarkoni, T., and Westfall, J. (2017). Choosing prediction over explanation in psychology: lessons from machine learning. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 12, 1100–1122. doi: 10.1177/1745691617693393

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yerkes, R. M., and Dodson, J. D. (1908). The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit-formation. J. Comp. Neurol. Psychol. 18, 459–482. doi: 10.1002/cne.920180503

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ying, L., Fu, S., Qian, X., and Sun, X. (2011). Effects of mental workload on long-latency auditory-evoked-potential, salivary cortisol, and immunoglobulin A. Neurosci. Lett. 491, 31–34. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2011.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoshida, Y., Ohwada, H., Mizoguchi, F., and Iwasaki, H. (2014). Classifying cognitive load and driving situation with machine learning. Int. J. Mach. Learn. Comput. 4:210. doi: 10.7763/IJMLC.2014.V4.414

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoshino, K., Oka, N., Yamamoto, K., Takahashi, H., and Kato, T. (2013). Functional brain imaging using near-infrared spectroscopy during actual driving on an expressway. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:882. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00882

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zander, T. O., Andreessen, L. M., Berg, A., Bleuel, M., Pawlitzki, J., Zawallich, L., et al. (2017). Evaluation of a dry EEG system for application of passive brain-computer interfaces in autonomous driving. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:78. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00078

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao, C., Zhao, M., Liu, J., and Zheng, C. (2012). Electroencephalogram and electrocardiograph assessment of mental fatigue in a driving simulator. Accid. Anal. Prev. 45, 83–90. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2011.11.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: psychophysiology, cognition, driving, traffic safety, real-world

Citation: Lohani M, Payne BR and Strayer DL (2019) A Review of Psychophysiological Measures to Assess Cognitive States in Real-World Driving. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 13:57. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00057

Received: 01 May 2018; Accepted: 01 February 2019;
Published: 19 March 2019.

Edited by:

Bruce Mehler, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, United States

Reviewed by:

Joost De Winter, Delft University of Technology, Netherlands
Mickael Causse, National Higher School of Aeronautics and Space, France
Dick De Waard, University of Groningen, Netherlands
Edmund Wascher, Leibniz Research Centre for Working Environment and Human Factors (IfADo), Germany

Copyright © 2019 Lohani, Payne and Strayer. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Monika Lohani, bW9uaWthLmxvaGFuaUB1dGFoLmVkdQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.