Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurol., 04 June 2020
Sec. Applied Neuroimaging

Both Stationary and Dynamic Functional Interhemispheric Connectivity Are Strongly Associated With Performance on Cognitive Tests in Multiple Sclerosis

\nSue-Jin LinSue-Jin Lin1Shannon KolindShannon Kolind2Aiping LiuAiping Liu3Katrina McMullenKatrina McMullen2Irene VavasourIrene Vavasour4Z. Jane WangZ. Jane Wang3Anthony TraboulseeAnthony Traboulsee2Martin J. McKeown,
Martin J. McKeown1,2*
  • 1Graduate Program in Neuroscience, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
  • 2Division of Neurology, Department of Medicine, UBC Hospital, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
  • 3Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering Program, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
  • 4Department of Radiology, Faculty of Medicine, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada

Although functional connectivity has been extensively studied in MS, robust estimates of both stationary (static connectivity at the time) and dynamic (connectivity variation across time) functional connectivity has not been commonly evaluated and neither has its association to cognition. In this study, we focused on interhemispheric connections as previous research has shown links between anatomical homologous connections and cognition. We examined functional interhemispheric connectivity (IC) in MS during resting-state functional MRI using both stationary and dynamic strategies and related connectivity measures to processing speed performance. Twenty-five patients with relapsing-remitting MS and 41 controls were recruited. Stationary functional IC was assessed between homologous Regions of Interest (ROIs) using correlation. For dynamic IC, a sliding window approach was used to quantify changes between homologous ROIs across time. We related IC measures to cognitive performance with correlation and regression. Compared to control subjects, MS demonstrated increased IC across homologous regions, which accurately predicted performance on the symbol digit modalities test (SDMT) (R2 = 0.96) and paced auditory serial addition test (PASAT) (R2 = 0.59). Dynamic measures were not different between the 2 groups, but dynamic IC was related to PASAT scores. The associations between stationary/dynamic connectivity and cognitive tests demonstrated that different aspects of functional IC were associated with cognitive processes. Processing speed measured in SDMT was associated with static interhemispheric connections and better PASAT performance, which requires working memory, sustain attention, and processing speed, was more related to rigid IC, underlining the neurophysiological mechanism of cognition in MS.

Introduction

As multiple sclerosis (MS) is characterized by discrete lesions visible on standard T2-weighted MRI images, there has been traditional emphasis on establishing the clinical–pathologic correlation between lesion location and symptomatology, including cognitive functioning (1, 2). Resting-state functional MRI (rs-fMRI) has been used to explore the more spatially distributed aspects of neural correlates of cognitive dysfunction, which can affect up to 70% of patients with MS and contribute to an impaired quality of life (3). Among the myriad of cognitive deficits seen in MS, information processing speed is especially important, as it is commonly impaired and important for performance in many cognitive domains (4). Linking processing speed deficits and functional connectivity has been a trend to further understand cognitive disease sequelae.

The corpus callosum (CC) facilitates interhemispheric connections in humans and lesions in the CC damage such connections (5, 6), resulting in numerous clinical deficits (711). For example, structural neuroimaging suggests that white matter lesions involving callosal connections predict disability in MS (7). Structural changes in the CC such as callosal atrophy is related to lower processing speed abilities in MS, as measured by the Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT) at both baseline and follow-up (12). Abnormal callosal fractional anisotropy is correlated with impairment in several behavioral measures such as functional composite scores, processing speed, and performance of upper extremity function (13).

In addition to altered structural interhemispheric connectivity (IC), studies of functional IC provide insights into deficits seen in MS. Functional connectivity measures of homologous regions derived by electroencephalography are associated with CC atrophy in MS (14). Although no correlations between functional measures and behavior are reported, this study reveals the association between structural changes in CC and functional IC. Decreased magnetoencephalography synchronization between the two hemispheres has also been found in MS, suggesting that weakened functional IC could be a biomarker for cognitive impairment (15). Homotopic functional connectivity has been shown to be altered in visual, somatosensory, motor, and sensory processing regions in MS (16), and such alterations are correlated with microstructural damages.

The two most commonly used tests for detecting cognitive deficits in MS are the SDMT and the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT). PASAT was originally developed to test the speed of information processing; however, owing to its complexity, the performance also requires a great deal of sustained and divided attention, working memory, and processing speed, so it is sensitive to deficits (17). SDMT, on the other hand, also measures processing speed ability, but the task does not involve complicated components across multiple cognitive domains and thus has been frequently used as a neuropsychological screening test for processing speed (18). Both tests have been recommended as the standardized tools for cognitive assessment in MS (19) and have been adapted to task-driven fMRI research. Studies have investigated effective functional connectivity among the test-specific regions and shown that information flow between two hemispheres is required for both SDMT and PASAT performance (20, 21).

There is a growing interest in dynamic functional connectivity (dFC), that is, how functional connectivity fluctuates over time, which may be a strategy to coordinate information flow across time and adapt to stimuli (22, 23). Traditional analyses estimate mean functional connectivity over the entire time course without considering the changes of connectivity strength over time. However, the brain is obviously dynamic, and thus, connectivity patterns may change constantly (24). A standard approach for calculating dFC is to use a sliding window approach to calculate connectivity within each window with or without overlap between windows (24, 25), revealing the variations in connectivity over time. Connectivity fluctuations are associated with performance in several cognitive domains such as attention, memory, executive function, and cognitive flexibility (2632). A few studies have recently demonstrated that dFC is associated with cognitive performance in MS. Performance on processing speed, working memory, and executive function tests has been associated with higher variation of both global and specific (e.g., interhemispheric connections) resting-state functional connectivity (i.e., higher dFC) in MS (33). Another study has reported that greater variability of connectivity within the default mode network is related to better performance in a processing speed task (34).

In this study, we proposed that IC is of interest in MS and examined both stationary and dynamic aspects of IC and specifically related them to processing speed performance. First, we evaluated stationary functional IC in MS during resting-state fMRI. In addition, we applied a sliding window approach with calculations on connectivity differences to quantify the variation of connectivity in IC. We then applied regression and correlation analyses to investigate the relationships between stationary/dynamic functional IC, clinical data, and performance on processing speed tests. The overarching goal was to investigate whether functional stationary and dynamic IC are altered in MS and whether the IC measures are related to the subjects' processing speed performance in early stages of the disease.

Materials and Methods

Subjects

We recruited 25 patients with relapsing-remitting MS (mean age ± SD = 37.2 ± 9.5; 10 male and 15 female) in a Phase III randomized, double-blind, double-dummy trial to evaluate the efficacy and safety of ocrelizumab vs. interferon beta-1a in relapsing forms of MS (OPERA II; NCT01412333) (35). The data included in this study (clinical scores and imaging data) were from the baseline time point only. We also recruited 41 age-/gender-matched healthy control subjects (HC) (mean age ± SD = 34.9 ± 10.1; 14 male and 27 female) who were not part of the randomized study. Ethics approval was received from the university's Clinical Research Ethics Board; and all subjects provided written, informed consent. HC did not have psychiatric, medical, cognitive, or other conditions that caused an inability to participate in an MRI study. Patients had Expanded Disability Status Scale (EDSS) scores ranging between 0 and 4 (median EDSS = 2) (Table 1), indicating a mildly affected cohort.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographics of MS patients.

Clinical Tests

All patients performed the MS functional composite (MSFC) battery (36) including the PASAT-3 s. They also performed a low-contrast visual acuity (LCVA) and the SDMT. As the purpose was to explore the potential functional biomarkers related to cognition, the sub-tests that primarily reflected ability of motor and physical coordination in MSFC, such as the Timed 25-Foot Walk and 9-Hole Peg Test, were not included in the study. Although the LCVA does not estimate cognitive function, this outcome was included in the analysis to explore whether clinically common visual problems were related to brain connectivity. The raw scores of these tests were used in the analyses in this study.

Image Acquisition

Images were acquired with a Philips Achieva 3.0 Tesla MRI scanner (Best, The Netherlands). We collected 3D T1-weighted images with CLEAR homogeneity correction with 1 × 1 × 3 mm3 resolution. In detail, the 3D T1-weighted gradient echo scan was acquired with TR = 28 ms, TE = 4 ms, 60 axial slices acquired at 3-mm slice thickness, in-plane voxel size = 1 × 1 mm2, flip angle = 27°, and field of view (FOV) = 250 × 188 × 180 mm3. After structural image acquisition, 8 min of rs-fMRI data were acquired with an echo-planar imaging sequence with 3 × 3 × 3 mm3 resolution, 2,000-ms TR, 36 slices, 90° flip angle, and 240 volumes. Participants were instructed to close their eyes and to not think about anything in particular.

Image Preprocessing

Several image preprocessing steps were applied to the fMRI data including slice-timing, isotropic reslicing, and motion correction in MATLAB using SPM8 functions (the Well-come Trust Center for Neuroimaging, UK) and in-house Matlab code. Subjects who showed more than 2° of rotation and 2 mm of translation were supposed to be removed from the analysis, but none of the subjects fulfilled the criteria. We further investigated the motions in patients; and the average displacements for translation and rotation are 0.32 ± 0.3 mm and 0.0051 ± 0.004°, respectively. FLIRT (the FMRIB Center, UK) was used to register fMRI images and structural images. T1-weighted images were used for cortical and subcortical parcellation, which was carried out using FreeSurfer software (version 4.5.0, Massachusetts General Hospital, USA) and parcellated based on the Desikan–Killiany atlas. After parcellation, 38 cognition-related regions of interest (ROIs) were chosen (Table 2). These ROIs, especially the ones in the frontoparietal areas and association cortex, have been commonly reported as important regions for cognitive function, especially higher-order function, in the literature and were chosen by an experienced neuropsychologist and a senior neurologist (3743). These ROIs are located in the frontal (six ROIs), parietal (six ROIs), temporal (four ROIs), occipital (one ROI), and cingulate cortices (two ROIs). As the frontoparietal areas and association cortex were the main targets, subcortical regions were not included. The mean time courses over all voxels within one region in fMRI data were extracted from the given ROIs and detrended. All calculations were done in the subject's native space.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. The 38 ROIs (FreeSurfer version 4.5.0) in the study (19 bilateral regions).

Methodological Considerations: Accurately Estimating Interhemispheric Connectivity

Technical issues may complicate interpretations of functional IC. For stationary interhemispheric connections, similarity between neural activities in homologous regions may not necessarily be based on transcallosal activity, but rather both hemispheres may be influenced by common brainstem and/or subcortical input. In other words, pairwise/Pearson's correlation, which is the most common approach to assess functional connectivity, estimates connectivity between two regions as well as regions that are potentially not of interest (44). This issue can be solved by partial correlation, whereby the relation between homologous pairs is assessed controlling for the activity in another region (44, 45). For dynamic connectivity, there is also the challenge of choosing the size of the sliding window when using this approach to estimate connectivity as it directly affects statistical power (46). The length of the window may affect connectivity results, and how to quantify connectivity changes across time so that the measures can be used to correlate with behavior is also challenging (47, 48). Calculating network features based on connectivity differences between time periods may show benefits for further analysis.

Stationary Functional Interhemispheric Connectivity Analysis

We computed correlation coefficients to examine IC across homologous regions. For each subject, we first divided the time courses from n ROIs into n/2 homologous left (L = {left ROI1, left ROI2, …}), and right (R = {right ROI1, right ROI2, …}) pairs. For the ith pair (e.g., {L{i}, R{i})}, we examined the remainder right and left homologous pairs {L{1, …, n/2, ≠ i}, R{1, …, n/2, ≠ i}} and computed both the simple correlation (Pearson's r) and the partial correlation conditioned on both L{i} and R{i}. We then computed the sum of the differences in correlations and partial correlations between the remaining homologous pairs and attributed the result to {L{i}, R{i}}:

Di=j=1,,n2,ji|C({L{j},R{j}})           -C({L{j},R{j}}|L{i},R{i})|

where Di is the correlation difference attributed to the pair of ROIs L{i}, R{i}, C(*,*) is the correlation operator. The analysis was done in original fMRI time courses and the augmented time courses with one time point lag (details in Supplementary Material).

In order to determine if IC, described by Di's, was associated with cognitive performance and whether certain homologous pairs had the largest influence on predicting clinical and cognitive scores, we performed regression with a sparsity penalty on the regression coefficients by implementing least absolute shrinkage and selection operator (LASSO) regression. Age was also included as a nuisance covariate. This algorithm attempts to find a balance between predicting cognitive scores and including as few interhemispheric connections as possible to make that prediction, which was one of the strategies to avoid overfitting.

To aid in interpretation, we then performed linear regression using the fitlm function in matlab and another linear regression with the connections selected by LASSO (Supplementary Figure 2 in the Supplementary Material). In order to assess significance of the results, we performed cross-validation. We permuted the rows of the selected connections and calculated the p-value of LASSO coefficients. This was repeated 100 times to create a null distribution, against which the unshuffled results were compared.

Dynamic Functional Connectivity on Interhemispheric Connections

In order to estimate dFC, a sliding window approach, with window length (WL) of 30 time points (=60 s), was used; and it fulfilled the suggested criteria that WL should be ranged between 30 and 60 s (48). The window was shifted one time point forward in each simple correlation analysis, resulting in 211 windowed correlation matrices for each subject. Further features were obtained from these windowed correlation matrices. Flexibility of homologous connections FOCcs (flexibility of connectivity cross hemisphere in symmetric regions) was calculated using the connectivity differences of homologous connections between two adjacent windows. The values of connectivity differences were then summed up and divided by the total number of windows, which form one value representing how much the IC fluctuates across time. Specifically,

FOCcs= 1(L-1) t=2Li,j H(Mij(t)-Mij(t-1))2

where H is the set of i, j indices corresponding to all homologous connections, and Mij(t) represents i, jth element of the correlation matrix estimated at window t. L represents the total number of windowed correlation matrices (here, L = 211).

To explore the relations between dFC in IC, correlation analyses were carried out on FOCcs and cognitive scores.

Results

Processing Speed Performance

As the HC did not perform cognitive tests in the study, we compared our MS subjects' PASAT and SDMT performance with a previous study that included normative data in order to verify whether the patients showed cognitive decline (49). Our patients demonstrated mean SDMT score 1.25 SD below the normative data in the previous study (our MS: PASAT 42.44 ± 15.38 and SDMT 49.88 ± 11.03; normative data: PASAT 48.0 ± 10.7 and SDMT 61.9 ± 9.6), but the mean PASAT performance in our MS did not show a difference lower than 1 SD. Individually, six subjects demonstrated impairments on PASAT (showed score smaller than 1.5 SD of normative data), and nine subjects were impaired on SDMT test.

This indicated that most subjects had cognitive decline on both tests especially SDMT, but they did not reach the standard of impairments.

Stationary Interhemispheric Connectivity

We observed significant differences between MS and HC (Figure 1). Figure 1 shows the differences between simple and partial correlation (y axis) across homologous regions (x axis) in the left panel. The changes in overall IC were significantly different [corrected p < 0.05, controlled for false discovery rate (FDR)] between MS and HC for three bilateral regions using partial correlation: the superior parietal cortex, superior occipital gyrus, and precuneus. In addition, the mean difference for all regions was higher for the MS compared with the HC group, regardless of what pairs were conditioned.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Functional interhemispheric connectivity in healthy controls (HCs) and multiple sclerosis (MS). Upper left panel and lower left panel represent instantaneous and delayed interhemispheric connectivity in MS (red) and HC (black), respectively. The differences between Pearson's correlation and partial correlation represent the relative importance (connectivity values) of the brain regions of interest [conditional region of interest (ROI)] when it is included or excluded from the analysis. A larger difference reflects greater importance of that region. Black stars and red squares indicate the connection pairs, which significantly distinguish the two groups in instantaneous and delayed connectivity [p < 0.05, false discovery rate (FDR) corrected], respectively. Upper right and lower right panels show the altered connections on a brain template. The visualization was done in the BrainNet Viewer (50). Abbreviations are shown in Table 2.

Prediction of Cognitive Scores

In linear regression, SDMT was significantly associated with IC pairs in the middle frontal, supramarginal, and posterior parietal regions (Table 3). In LASSO, even with the sparsity constraint on the regression coefficients, 13 and 7 ROI pairs were still included in the model to predict SDMT and PASAT scores, respectively. In Figure 2, the upper panel demonstrated the weightings of selected variables in LASSO. The variables with stronger weightings were influential to the model that was used to predict cognitive scores as shown in the lower panel. The signs of weightings represented how the variables influenced/contributed in the model in regard to the predicted variables. The variables with positive weightings tended to be positively associated with the response variable in the model, whereas negative weightings indicated negative associations. Age and several connectivity pairs jointly predicted SDMT scores (average R2 = 0.96, leave one out) including the frontal pole, middle frontal gyrus, inferior prefrontal cortex, temporal pole, insula, amygdala regions, superior temporal gyrus, postcentral gyrus, supramarginal gyrus, parietal and occipital junction areas, superior occipital lobe, anterior cingulate cortex, posterior parietal cortex, lateral orbitofrontal cortex, and posterior parietal gyrus. On the other hand, in addition to age, the superior temporal gyrus, medial temporal gyrus, hippocampal regions, supramarginal areas, parietal occipital regions, superior occipital gyrus, anterior cingulate cortex, and lateral orbitofrontal gyrus were highly associated with PASAT performance (average R2 = 0.59, leave one out). Interestingly, the three significant IC pairs that were associated with SDMT performance in the linear regression model could also predict SDMT in the LASSO regression.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Estimated coefficients in a linear regression model with IC pairs and SDMT score.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Important brain regions for cognitive performances. With least absolute shrinkage and selection operator (LASSO) regression, the upper left and upper right panels express weights of important connectivity pairs in predicting SDMT and PASAT, respectively. The lower panel shows positive correlations between real scores and predicted scores of SDMT and PASAT with the limited number of pairs. Real and predicted SDMT scores especially demonstrate a good correlation with average R2 = 0.96 in leave-one-out calculation. Predicted and real PASAT scores also show a strong correlation with average R2 = 0.59. Abbreviations are shown in Table 2. PASAT3, Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test-3 s; SDMT, Symbol Digit Modalities Test.

Finally, in the permutation test, p = 0.0009 for SDMT and 0.007 for PASAT against null distribution.

Dynamic Aspect of Functional Interhemispheric Connectivity

A sliding window approach was used, and a network measure was calculated to summarize the connectivity changes of interhemispheric connections (i.e., FOCcs). FOCcs showed a negative correlation with PASAT scores (r = −0.44, p = 0.03) (Figure 3), illustrating that better PASAT performance was associated with smaller flexibility of interhemispheric connections. However, this measure did not show differences between MS and HC groups. None of the other clinical or cognitive scores demonstrated correlation with FOCcs. Supplementary Material showed similar results with different parameters (different WL) using the same approach (Supplementary Figure 3).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Dynamic functional connectivity and cognition in multiple sclerosis (MS). FOCcs, which represent dynamic interhemispheric connectivity, are negatively correlated with Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT) scores. FOCcs: flexibility of homologous connections. Each * represents each dataset.

Discussion

Enhanced Interhemispheric Connectivity May Be a Result of Adaptation and Compensation in Multiple Sclerosis

We found overall enhanced IC in MS subjects as demonstrated in Figure 1, which is consist with prior studies (5154). In this study, we discovered significantly altered functional IC in MS through an approach that specifically assessed homologous connections. Previous fMRI studies have reported decreased functional connectivity in MS (51, 55), whereas increased functional connectivity has also been reported during rest as well as tasks (5153), which may reflect compensatory mechanisms, results of adaptation, and/or loss of cognitive flexibility (54, 56, 57). For example, within the default mode network areas, several cortices demonstrate enhanced functional coupling in MS, which is associated with a concomitant loss of cognitive efficiency and may be a maladaptive process (56).

In this study, we propose a direct measure representing IC, which made the methodology in this study unique. Supplementary Figure 4 in the Supplementary Material describes the rationale and mathematical explanations of this approach in detail. Because the correlation differences (i.e., the actual IC values as acquired by subtracting direct correlation from full correlation) were consistently larger across all regions in MS (Figure 1) and the overall IC was also larger in the MS group, there is both enhancement and homogenization of IC in MS, meaning that the interhemispheric connections in MS are more likely to be affected by each other. If one connection is altered, the rest of the connections are more likely to be influenced as well. This also implies that interhemispheric connections in MS lose their ability to independently modulate neural communication between homologous regions. Instead, the homologous regions require further support from other connectivity pairs in order to transfer information across the two hemispheres, which indicates that enhanced interhemispheric communication could be an early compensatory change in MS.

However, recent research has questioned whether increased functional connectivity can simply represent compensation/adaptation in MS or whether the connectivity changes indicate a combination of both compensation and adaptation (58, 59). As our control subjects did not perform the same cognitive tests, it is hard to verify whether the enhanced IC solely represents compensatory effects or whether it can be an adaptive process. Regardless of this uncertainty, we propose that enhancement and homogenization of IC are a potential biomarker for MS.

Homologous Regions Across Cortices and Dynamic Interhemispheric Connectivity Are Associated With Cognitive Performance

In addition to the finding of overall increased IC, we have also demonstrated that altered functional IC closely reflects performance on cognitive tests, namely, the SDMT and PASAT, with LASSO (Figure 2). If the LASSO regression did not find significant results, all the coefficients would be zero, and therefore, the regression cannot predict behavioral scores. In addition, with a permutation test, the p-value also indicated that the model of LASSO is significant. Interestingly, even when we used a sparsity constraint on the regression in LASSO, we still found several ROI pairs across cortices, which were required for accurate prediction of cognitive performance. Thus, both SDMT and PASAT performance appear to be strongly associated with widespread IC. In other words, working memory, sustained attention, and processing speed reflected in PASAT and SDMT are more distributed across cortices rather than localized (57, 60).

Nevertheless, some cortical regions showed different weighting to the cognitive performance. For SDMT, LASSO regression has shown that regions across cortices, especially the frontal pole, superior temporal gyrus, and posterior parietal cortex, demonstrated positive weighting, whereas the middle frontal and supramarginal gyrus presented stronger negative weighting to the performance in MS. Similarly, the regions shown with linear regression were all included in LASSO regression with the same sign of weighting (i.e., positive and negative). This linear combination of weighting indicated that even though different ROIs might show opposite effects to the performance, the coordination between these positive and negative effects might be the key to facilitate processing speed ability. In other words, some regions may show positive weighting of connectivity to support cognitive function, whereas other regions may show negative weighting of connectivity (i.e., suppress the model) to modulate performance. For PASAT, fewer ROIs with strong weighting were required to predict performance. The strong positive weighting from the superior temporal gyrus, supramarginal gyrus, and anterior cingulate cortex can predict and support sustained attention, working memory, and processing speed together with the negative weighting from the parietal occipital junction and lateral orbitofrontal gyrus to modulate performance in MS. Compared with SDMT, fewer ROIs were selected to predict PASAT performance. Because during the test, sustained attention was highly demanded as well as the skills with high cognitive load, the brain might purposely exclude non-crucial ROIs to save brain energy for the processes, which is especially important in the diseased brain. Overall, our results not only reinforce that 1) distributed regions support these cognitive performances (6163) but also indicate that 2) the coordination between positive and negative effects of IC may be the key to facilitating cognitive performance. Although it is not clear how different connectivity pairs modulate cognitive function exactly or why such patterns are crucial to cognition, the trade-offs in cognition with limited neural resource may provide insights (64, 65).

Although dynamic IC is not significantly different between MS and HC, we do observe relations between IC and processing speed performance, indicating that subtle differences of FOCcs are related to behavioral outcome in MS subjects. Such a relation was only observed with PASAT, implying that unique strategies may be needed in different tests. In other words, one test may require stronger and more static IC compared with the other test. As the subjects in this study are not severely impaired, this indicates that the impairments of dFC in early MS may not be manifest and that the measure cannot serve as a biomarker, but the subtle changes are sensitive to early alterations of processing speed and provide insights into how interhemispheric connections react to cognitive load.

Contrasting Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test and Symbol Digit Modalities Test

Although both PASAT and SDMT are clinically used for investigating cognitive deficits in MS, numerous studies have reported the SDMT to be a more sensitive, valid, and reliable measure than the PASAT (18, 19, 49, 6668). Indeed, we found a strong correlation between SDMT and IC measures (Figure 2, Table 3), consistent with prior studies suggesting that SDMT is more strongly associated with MRI measures (18, 69).

SDMT has been thought of as measuring mainly processing speed and selective attention over a short time frame (62), and in which other strategies related to complex processes (e.g., working memory) are not used compared with PASAT. Therefore, fewer factors can affect the measurement, and SDMT may better reflect impairments in processing speed. SDMT performance requires a great deal of long-range connections such as frontoparietal and frontal-occipital networks (62). In long-range connections, the existence of “hubs,” regions that show denser connections to other regions and play a central role in the network (7072), improves efficiency of neuronal signal transfer to support brain function and facilitate cognitive processes (73, 74). Given that IC has been shown to be highly dependent on functional IC (75), we propose that processing speed ability may be strongly associated with hubs. As interhemispheric connections shape the centrality of regions (i.e., hubs) (75), performance of processing speed tasks should also be highly related to IC.

We also show that IC of the frontal pole, temporal pole, insula, amygdala, superior temporal cortex, parietal and occipital regions, anterior cingulate cortex, and posterior parietal cortex is important for SDMT performance (Figure 2), which is partially consistent with previous studies showing that parietal areas play a role in SDMT as well as frontal areas and occipital regions (61), which are considered to be long-range connections. Moreover, baseline connectivity in these regions can predict future processing speed (60), emphasizing the relationship between processing speed ability and connectivity in distributed cortices. The IC of regions such as temporal pole, insula, amygdala, and superior temporal cortex has not been mentioned in previous studies. Although we suspect that these regions act as mediators in information communication between frontal and parietal areas for a short time, there is not enough evidence to support the hypothesis, and how the IC of these regions mediates cognitive performance requires further research. For example, although the link between IC and hubs has been shown (75), whether the IC pairs are linked to each other through hubs or other regions ipsilaterally may require whole brain analysis.

In contrast, performance on PASAT may require information coordination between frontal, parietal regions, and cerebellum because activation patterns mainly locate within one hemisphere among these regions, which will not be captured by the IC measures examined here (61). However, we found that only PASAT scores were related to dFC and better performance is associated with lower dynamics (Figure 3). PASAT examines processing speed and working memory, which involves higher-order cognitive processes, and these processes have been proposed to be related to variation in functional connectivity (i.e., dFC) (26, 76). Note also that the performance of PASAT requires sustained attention, which coordinates the recruitment of task-relevant resource, the discharge of resources for task-irrelevant processes, and the selection of exhibition and inhibition of cognitive processes in healthy subjects (77, 78). Therefore, the neuronal mechanism of sustained attention should also be dynamic and flexible. However, our results indicate an opposite trend, whereby better processing speed, working memory, and sustained attention ability measured in PASAT is associated with lower variation of functional connectivity even though our subjects are not severely impaired. Perhaps such flexibility is dysfunctional in early MS even though the alteration is not statistically significant, and neither is the patient performance. The subtle changes of losing flexibility require stable neuronal resources to constantly main the function. Therefore, we propose that PASAT performance may require unfluctuating/rigid IC in MS, but further research is needed to draw firm conclusions including taking into account learning effects, anxiety, and patients burden while performing PASAT (79).

Taken together, our results imply that interhemispheric connections between temporal areas, anterior cingulate gyrus, and a portion of parietal regions are crucial in PASAT and that the connectivity inflexibility of these connections are associated with the test performance. Perhaps, the performance of PASAT requires more stationary connectivity to support focused attention and inhibit unnecessary information rather than constantly coordinating signals across time and brain areas in MS. These mechanisms may also reduce the connectivity cost during PASAT performance as presumably less fluctuating connections require less energy consumption, which helps accomplish such a demanding task in the diseased brain.

Limitations

There are some limitations to our study. We emphasized ROIs associated with higher-order cognition especially in the association cortex and thus would not detect other types of connectivity disruption affecting the primary senses, such as vision. In addition, here, we aimed to study cortical connectivity so some subcortical regions, which might be important for high-order cognition, were not included. Therefore, our data were not sufficient to investigate cortico-striatal loops, for example. Moreover, although several processes were performed to ensure the data quality, we did not specifically investigate the motion effects and remove the potential noise source with the use of commonly applied methods such as RETROICOR or FIX toolboxes (80, 81). Owing to the lack of physiological data and limited number of subjects, we did not apply these methods. In the future studies, removing potential noise from fMRI data requires further investigation. Owing to the design of this clinical trial, we did not administer a comprehensive neuropsychological test battery to examine all cognitive domains, and some variables were not collected either such as education and socoeconomic status. Therefore, the results only represent the cognitive tests that have been commonly used in clinical trials. Nonetheless, the PASAT and SDMT are well-validated, sensitive, and widely used measures of cognition in MS; and inclusion of these measures allows our study to be comparable with previous imaging and cognition studies in MS. As education was not included as a nuisance covariance, we cannot estimate whether education modulates the cognitive performance in the current study. Future studies should seek to determine IC across a wide range of cognitive domains with the control of nuisance covariance. Moreover, task-driven fMRI was not included in the original study design. Because we propose a relation between IC and cognitive performance in this study, implementing task-driven fMRI may provide further insights for future research.

Although we made attempts to prevent overfitting, including cross-validation, more subjects would enhance the robustness of our results. Furthermore, because we only had cognitive scores for the MS group, we lack the knowledge of how SDMT and PASAT relate to IC in HCs. In addition, the patients were not recruited based on cognitive impairment, so our results might not be representative of MS cohorts with severe cognitive problems. Furthermore, even though the suggested criteria for the sliding window approach have been proposed, this approach is still under debate (46). Further research of dynamic connectivity with different approaches is needed to fully understand how network dynamics are related to clinical data. Finally, linking lesion burden and cognitive impairments has always been an interest in this field. As we also acquired structural MRI data, it would be important to explore the relations between structural damages, functional disruptions, and cognitive/clinical impairments in MS (Supplementary Material includes preliminary results) as recent studies have investigated whether structural or functional impairments can better predict processing speed performance (82). However, the focus of this study is the functional aspect of IC and its relation to cognitive performance. Linking structural changes, functional alterations, and behavior is beyond the scope of this study.

Conclusion

This study found increased stationary functional IC in MS, which were related to worsened cognitive performance. We have demonstrated altered IC in MS: these connections are more homogeneous in MS as well as enhanced, which might possibly serve as biomarkers. Furthermore, SDMT and PASAT scores were robustly correlated with stationary IC. Moreover, we discovered that PASAT performance is negatively correlated with dynamic IC in MS, possibly demonstrating that sustained attention and high cognitive load are related to inflexibility of interhemispheric connection. The results provide insights into how interhemispheric connections are affected in MS and how they are related to cognitive performance, which enhances our understanding of the disease.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Office of Research Ethics, University of British Columbia. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

S-JL performed the analysis, interpreted the results, and wrote the manuscript. SK edited the manuscript, contributed to the study design, and coordinated the clinical trial. AL contributed to technical aspects. KM edited the manuscript and helped with interpretations. IV edited the manuscript and helped with study coordination. ZW supervised technical aspects. AT supervised the clinical trial. MM initiated the analysis, edited the manuscript, and supervised this study.

Funding

This study was supported by F. Hoffmann-La Roche, Ltd. (OPERA II; NCT01412333).

Conflict of Interest

SK has consulted for Genzyme and received research support from Roche and Genzyme. AT has the following competing financial interests: Research funding from Chugai, Roche, Novartis, Genzyme, Biogen. Consultancy honoraria from Genzyme, Roche, Teva, Biogen, Serono. MM is supported by the UBC/PPRI Chair in Parkinson's Research.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors also thank Saurabh Garg and Tobias R. Baumeister for their help with revisions.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fneur.2020.00407/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Miller D, Grossman R, ReingoldS McFarland H. The role of magnetic resonance techniques in understanding and managing multiple sclerosis. Brain. (1998) 121:3–24. doi: 10.1093/brain/121.1.3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Hulst HE, Steenwijk MD, Versteeg A, Pouwels PJW, Vrenkan H, Uitdehaag BMJ, et al. Cognitive impairment in MS: impact of white matter integrity, gray matter volume, and lesions. Neurology. (2013) 80:1025–32. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31828726cc

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Langdon DW. Cognition in multiple sclerosis. Curr Opin Neurol. (2011) 24:244–9. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0b013e328346a43b

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Chiaravalloti ND, DeLuca J. Cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Lancet Neurol. (2008) 7:1139–51. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(08)70259-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Schüz A, Preiβl H. Basic connectivity of the cerebral cortex and some considerations on the corpus callosum. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (1996) 20:567–70. doi: 10.1016/0149-7634(95)00069-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Zarei M, Johansen-Berg H, Smith S, Ciccarelli O, Thompson AJ, Matthews PM. Functional anatomy of interhemispheric cortical connections in the human brain. J Anat. (2006) 209:311–20. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7580.2006.00615.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Bodini B, Cercignani M, Khaleeli Z, Miller D, Ron M, Penny S, Thompson A, Ciccarelli O. Corpus callosum damage predicts disability progression and cognitive dysfunction in primary-progressive MS after five years. Hum Brain Mapp. (2013) 34:1163–1172. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21499

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Lee D, Fletcher E, Martinez O, Zozulya N, Kim J, Tran J, et al. Vascular and degenerative processes differentially affect regional interhemispheric connections in normal aging, mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer disease. Stroke. (2010) 41:1791–7. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.110.582163

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Mwansisya T, Wang Z, Tao H, Zhang H, Hu A, Guo S, et al. The diminished interhemispheric connectivity correlates with negative symptoms and cognitive impairment in first-episode schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. (2013) 150:144–50. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.07.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Paul L, Brown W, Adolphs R, Tyszka J, Richards L, Mukherjee P, et al. Agenesis of the corpus callosum: genetic, developmental and functional aspects of connectivity. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2007) 8:287–99. doi: 10.1038/nrn2107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Saar-Ashkenazy R, Cohen J, Guez J, Gasho C, Shelef I, Friedman A, et al. Reduced corpus-callosum volume in posttraumatic stress disorder highlights the importance of interhemispheric connectivity for associative memory. J Trauma Stress. (2014) 27:481–8. doi: 10.1002/jts.21887

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Bergendal G, Martola J, Stawiarz L, Kristoffersen-Wiberg M, Fredrikson S, et al. Callosal atrophy in multiple sclerosis is related to cognitive speed. Acta Neurol Scand. (2013) 127:281–9. doi: 10.1111/ane.12006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Ozturk A, Smith SA, Gordon-Lipkin EM, Harrison DM, Shiee N, Pham DL, et al. MRI of the corpus callosum in multiple sclerosis: association with disability. Mult Scler. (2010) 16:166–77. doi: 10.1177/1352458509353649

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Zito G, Luders E, Tomasevic L, Lupoi D, Toga AWW, Thompson PMM, et al. Inter-hemispheric functional connectivity changes with corpus callosum morphology in multiple sclerosis. Neuroscience. (2014) 266:47–55. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.01.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Cover KS, Vrenkenb H, Geurtsb JJG, vanOosten BW, Jellesc B, Polmanc CH, et al. Multiple sclerosis patients show a highly significant decrease in alpha band interhemispheric synchronization measured using MEG. NeuroImage. (2006) 29:783–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.08.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Zhou Y, Milham M, Zuo XN, Kelly C, Jaggi H, Herbert J, et al. Functional homotopic changes in multiple sclerosis with resting-state functional MR imaging. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. (2013) 34:1180–1187. doi: 10.3174/ajnr.A3386

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Tombaugh T. A comprehensive review of the paced auditory serial addition test (PASAT). Arch Clin Neuropsychol. (2006) 21:53–76. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2005.07.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Benedict RHB, DeLuca J, Phillips G, LaRocca N, Hudson LD, Rudick R. Validity of the symbol digit modalities test as a cognition performance outcome measure for multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler J. (2017) 23:721–33. doi: 10.1177/1352458517690821

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Langdon D, Amato M, Boringa J, Brochet B, Foley F, Fredrikson S, et al. Recommendations for a brief international cognitive assessment for multiple sclerosis (BICAMS). Mult Scler J. (2012) 18:891–8. doi: 10.1177/1352458511431076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Au Duong MV, Boulanouar K, Audoin B, Treseras S, Ibarrola D, Malikova I, et al. Modulation of effective connectivity inside the working memory network in patients at the earliest stage of multiple sclerosis. NeuroImage. (2005) 24:533–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.08.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Dobryakova E, Costa SL, Wylie GR, Deluca J, Genova HM. Altered effective connectivity during a processing speed task in individuals with multiple sclerosis. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2016) 22:216–24. doi: 10.1017/S1355617715001034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Deco G, Jirsa VK, McIntosh AR. Emerging concepts for the dynamical organization of resting-state activity in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2011) 12:43–56. doi: 10.1038/nrn2961

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Medaglia JD, Lynall M-E, Bassett DS. Cognitive network neuroscience. J Cogn Neurosci. (2015) 27:1471–91. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00810

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Hutchison RM, Womelsdor fT, Allen EA, Bandettini PA, Calhoun VD, Corbetta M, et al. Dynamic functional connectivity: promise, issues, and interpretations. NeuroImage. (2013) 80:360–78. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.079

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Allen EA, Damaraju E, Plis SM, Erhardt EB, Eichele T, Calhoun VD. Tracking whole-brain connectivity dynamics in the resting state. Cereb Cortex. (2014) 24:663–76. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs352

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Mattar MG, Cole MW, Thompson-Schill SL, Bassett DS. A functional cartography of cognitive systems. PLoS Comput Biol. (2015) 11:e1004533. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004533

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. McIntosh AR, Kovacevic N, Itier RJ. Increased brain signal variability accompanies lower behavioral variability in development. PLoS Comput Biol. (2008) 4:e1000106. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Kucyi A, Hove MJ, Esterman M, Hutchison RM, Valera EM. Dynamic brain network correlates of spontaneous fluctuations in attention. Cereb Cortex. (2016) 27:1831–40. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Shine JM, Bissett PG, Bell PT, Koyejo O, Balsters JH, Gorgolewski KJ, et al. The Dynamics of functional brain networks: integrated network states during cognitive task performance. Neuron. (2016) 92:544–54. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.09.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Braun U, Schäfer A, Walter H, Erk S, Romanczuk-Seiferth N, HaddadL, et al. Dynamic reconfiguration of frontal brain networks during executive cognition in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2015) 112:11678–83. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1422487112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Douw L, Wakeman DG, Tanaka N, Liu H, Stufflebeam SM. State-dependent variability of dynamic functional connectivity between frontoparietal and default networks relates to cognitive flexibility. Neuroscience. (2016) 339:12–21. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.09.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Nomi JS, Vij SG, Dajani DR, Steimke R, Damaraju E, Rachakonda S, et al. Chronnectomic patterns and neural flexibility underlie executive function. NeuroImage. (2017) 147:861–71. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.10.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Lin SJ, Vavasour I, Kosaka B, Li DKB, Traboulsee A, MacKay A, et al. Education, and the balance between dynamic and stationary functional connectivity jointly support executive functions in relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis. Human Brain Mapp. (2018) 39:5039–49. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24343

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. vanGeest Q, Douw L, van't Klooster S, Leurs CEE, Genova HMM, Wylie GRR, et al. Information processing speed in multiple sclerosis: relevance of default mode network dynamics. NeuroImage Clin. (2018) 19:507–15. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2018.05.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Hauser SL, Bar-Or A, Comi G, Giovoni G, Hartung HP, Hemmer B, et al. Ocrelizumab versus interferon beta-1a in relapsing multiple sclerosis. New Eng J Med. (2017) 376:221–34. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1601277

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Cutter GR, Baier ML, Rudick RA, Cookfair DL, Fischer JS, Petkau J, et al. Development of a multiple sclerosis functional composite as a clinical trial outcome measure. Brain. (1999) 122:871–82. doi: 10.1093/brain/122.5.871

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Stevens FL, Hurley RA, Taber KH, Hurley RA, Hayman LA, Taber KH. Anterior cingulate cortex: unique role in cognition and emotion. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2011) 23:121–5. doi: 10.1176/jnp.23.2.jnp121

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Leech R, Sharp DJ. The role of the posterior cingulate cortex in cognition and disease. Brain. (2014) 137:12–32. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Koechlin E, Basso G, Pietrini P, Panzer S, Grafman J. The role of the anterior prefrontal cortex in human cognition. Nature. (1999) 399:148. doi: 10.1038/20178

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Goldman-Rakic PS. Topography of cognition: parallel distributed networks in primate association cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci. (1988) 11:137–156. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.11.030188.001033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. McIntosh AR. Mapping cognition to the brain through neural interactions. Memory. (1999) 7:523–48. doi: 10.1080/096582199387733

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Duncan J, Owen AM. Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands. Trends Neurosci. (2000) 23:475–83. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(00)01633-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Nyhus E, Barceló F. The wisconsin card sorting test and the cognitive assessment of prefrontal executive functions: a critical update. Brain Cogn. (2009) 71:437–51. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Schmittmann VD, Jahfari S, Borsboom D, Savi AO, Waldorp LJ. Making large-scale networks from fMRI data. PLoS ONE. (2015) 10:e0129074. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0129074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Kim J, Wozniak JR, Mueller BA, Pan W. Testing group differences in brain functional connectivity : using correlations or partial correlations? Brain Connect. (2015) 5:214–31. doi: 10.1089/brain.2014.0319

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Hindriks R, Adhikari MH, Murayama Y, Ganzetti M, Mantini D, Logothetis NK, et al. Can sliding-window correlations reveal dynamic functional connectivity in resting-state fMRI? NeuroImage. (2016) 127:242–56. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.11.055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Leonardi N, Richiardi J, Gschwind M, Simioni S, Annoni JM, Schluep M, et al. Principal components of functional connectivity: a new approach to study dynamic brain connectivity during rest. NeuroImage. (2013) 83:937–50. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.07.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Zalesky A, Breakspear M. Towards a statistical test for functional connectivity dynamics. NeuroImage. (2015) 114:466–70. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.03.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Drake A, Weinstock-Guttman B, Morrow S, Hojnacki D, Munschauer F, Benedict R. Psychometrics and normative data for the multiple sclerosis functional composite: replacing the PASAT with the symbol digit modalities test. Mult Scler J. (2010) 16:228–37. doi: 10.1177/1352458509354552

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Xia M, Wang J, He Y. BrainNet viewer: a network visualization tool for human brain connectomics. PLoS ONE. (2013) 8:e68910. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0068910

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Kingwell K. Disease mechanisms in MS: neuronal network connectivity is altered in multiple sclerosis. Nat Rev Neurol. (2012) 8:593. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2012.205

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Janssen A, Boster A, Patterson B, Abduljalil A, Prakash R. Resting-state functional connectivity in multiple sclerosis: an examination of group differences and individual differences. Neuropsychologia. (2013) 51:2918–29. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.08.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Specogna I, Casagrande F, Lorusso A, Catalan M, Gorian A, Zugna L, et al. Functional MRI during the execution of a motor task in patients with multiple sclerosis and fatigue. Radiol Med. (2012) 117:1389–407. doi: 10.1007/s11547-012-0845-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Dogonowski A, Siebner H, Sørensen P, Wu X, Biswal B, Paulson O, et al. Expanded functional coupling of subcortical nuclei with the motor resting-state network in multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler. (2013) 19:559–66. doi: 10.1177/1352458512460416

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Dogonowski A, Andersen K, Madsen K, Sørensen P, Paulson O, Blinkenberg M, et al. Multiple sclerosis impairs regional functional connectivity in the cerebellum. Neuroimage Clin. (2013) 4:130–8. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2013.11.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Hawellek D, Hipp J, Lewis C, Corbetta M, Engel A. Increased functional connectivity indicates the severity of cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2011) 19066–71. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1110024108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Loitfelder M, Fazekas F, Koschutnig K, Fuchs S, Petrovic K, Ropele S, et al. Brain activity changes in cognitive networks in relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis - insights from a longitudinal fMRI study. PLoS ONE. (2014) 9:e93715. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0093715

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Castellazzi G, Debernard L, Melzer TR, Dalrymple-Alford JC, D'Angelo E, Miller DH, et al. Functional connectivity alterations reveal complex mechanisms based on clinical and radiological status in mild relapsing remitting multiple sclerosis. Front Neurol. (2018) 9:690. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2018.00690

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Tahedl M, Levine SM, Greenlee MW, Weissert R, Schwarzbach JV. Functional connectivity in multiple sclerosis : recent findings and future directions. Front Neurol. (2018) 9:828. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2018.00828

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Kuceyeski A, Monohan E, Morris E, Fujimoto K, Vargas W, Gauthier SA. Baseline biomarkers of connectome disruption and atrophy predict future processing speed in early multiple sclerosis. NeuroImage Clin. (2018) 19:417–24. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2018.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Forn C, Belenguer A, Belloch V, Sanjuan A, Parcet MA, Avila C. Anatomical and functional differences between the paced auditory serial addition test and the symbol digit modalities test. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. (2011) 33:42–50. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2010.481620

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Forn C, Ripollés P, Cruz-Gómez AJ, Belenguer A, González-Torre JA, ÁvilaC. Task-load manipulation in the symbol digit modalities test: an alternative measure of information processing speed. Brain Cogn. (2013) 82:152–60. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2013.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Loitfelder M, Filippi M, Rocca M, Valsasina P, Ropele S, Jehna M, et al. Abnormalities of resting state functional connectivity are related to sustained attention deficits in MS. PLoS ONE. (2012) 7:e42862. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0042862

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. DelGiudice M, Crespi BJ. Basic functional trade-offs in cognition: an integrative framework. Cognition. (2018) 179:56–70. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.06.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Christie ST, Schrater P. Cognitive cost as dynamic allocation of energetic resources. Front Neurosci. (2015) 9:289. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2015.00289

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Sonder J, Burggraaff J, Knol D, Polman C, Uitdehaag B. Comparing long-term results of PASAT and SDMT scores in relation to neuropsychological testing in multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler. (2014) 20:481–8. doi: 10.1177/1352458513501570

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Parmenter BA, Weinstock-Guttman B, Garg N, Munschauer F, Benedict RH. Screening for cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis using the symbol digit modalities test. Mult Scler J. (2007) 13:52–7. doi: 10.1177/1352458506070750

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Strober L, Englert J, Munschauer F, Weinstock-Guttman B, Rao S, Benedict RHB. Sensitivity of conventional memory tests in multiple sclerosis: comparing the rao brief repeatable neuropsychological battery and the minimal assessment of cognitive function in MS. Mult Scler. (2009) 15:1077–84. doi: 10.1177/1352458509106615

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Benedict R, Bruce J, Dwyer M, Abdelrahman N, Hussein S, Weinstock-Guttman B, et al. Neocortical atrophy, third ventricular width, and cognitive dysfunction in multiple sclerosis. Arch Neurol. (2006) 63:1301–6. doi: 10.1001/archneur.63.9.1301

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Achard S, Salvador R, Whitcher B, Suckling J, Bullmore E. A resilient, low-frequency, small-world human brain functional network with highly connected association Cortical hubs. J Neurosci. (2006) 26:63–72. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3874-05.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Hagmann P, Cammoun L, Gigandet X, Meuli R, Honey CJ, VanWedeen J, et al. Mapping the structural core of human cerebral cortex. PLoS Biol. (2008) 6:e159. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0060159

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. van denHeuve IM, Sporns O. Rich-club organization of the human connectome. J Neurosci. (2011) 31:15775–86. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3539-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Sporns O. Network attributes for segregation and integration in the human brain. Curr. Opin Neurobiol. (2013) 23:162–71. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2012.11.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Hofman MA. Evolution of the human brain: when bigger is better. Front Neuroanat. (2014) 8:15. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2014.00015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Martínez JH, Buldú JM, Papo D, Fallani FDV, Chavez M. Role of inter-hemispheric connections in functional brain networks. Sci Rep. (2018) 8:1–10. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-28467-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Park HJ, Friston K. Structural and functional brain networks: from connections to cognition. Science. (2013) 342:1238411. doi: 10.1126/science.1238411

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Clayton MS, Yeung N, Kadosh RC. The roles of cortical oscillations in sustained attention. Trends Cogn Sci. (2015) 19:188–95. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Esterman M, Rosenberg MD, Noonan SK. Intrinsic fluctuations in sustained attention and distractor processing. J Neurosci. (2014) 34:1724–30. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2658-13.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Cardina IKS, Wilson SM, Giesser BS, Drain AE, Sicotte NL. A longitudinal fMRI study of the paced auditory serial addition task. Mult Scler. (2008) 14:465–71. doi: 10.1177/1352458507084263

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Griffanti L, Salimi-Khorshidi G, Beckmann CF, Auerbach EJ, Douaud G, Sexton CE, et al. ICA-based artefact removal and accelerated fMRI acquisition for improved resting state network imaging. NeuroImage. (2014) 95:232–47. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.03.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Glover GH, Li TQ, Ress D. Image-based method for retrospective correction of physiological motion effects in fMRI: RETROICOR. Magn Reson Med. (2000) 44:162–7. doi: 10.1002/1522-2594(200007)44:1<162::AID-MRM23>3.0.CO;2-E

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Meijer KA, vanGeest Q, Eijlers AJC, Geurts JJG, Schoonheim MM, Hulst HE. Is impaired information processing speed a matter of structural or functional damage in MS? NeuroImage Clin. (2018) 20:844–50. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2018.09.021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: static functional connectivity, dynamic functional connectivity, multiple sclerosis, symbol digit modalities test, paced auditory serial addition test, resting state fMRI

Citation: Lin S-J, Kolind S, Liu A, McMullen K, Vavasour I, Wang ZJ, Traboulsee A and McKeown MJ (2020) Both Stationary and Dynamic Functional Interhemispheric Connectivity Are Strongly Associated With Performance on Cognitive Tests in Multiple Sclerosis. Front. Neurol. 11:407. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2020.00407

Received: 08 November 2019; Accepted: 20 April 2020;
Published: 04 June 2020.

Edited by:

Yulin Ge, New York University, United States

Reviewed by:

Scott Kolbe, Monash University, Australia
Jordi A. Matias-Guiu, Servicio de Neurología, Hospital Clínico San Carlos, Spain
Fuqing Zhou, First Affiliated Hospital of Nanchang University, China

Copyright © 2020 Lin, Kolind, Liu, McMullen, Vavasour, Wang, Traboulsee and McKeown. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Martin J. McKeown, bWFydGluLm1ja2Vvd24mI3gwMDA0MDt1YmMuY2E=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.