Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurol., 29 January 2020
Sec. Neuro-Ophthalmology
This article is part of the Research Topic Neuro-Ophthalmology Editor's Pick 2021 View all 7 articles

Cognitive Profile of Patients With Mitochondrial Chronic Progressive External Ophthalmoplegia

  • 1Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China
  • 2Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China
  • 3Department of Neurology, Peking University First Hospital, Beijing, China
  • 4Key Laboratory of Primate Neurobiology, Institute of Neuroscience, Center for Excellence in Brain Science and Intelligence Technology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Shanghai, China

Mitochondrial chronic progressive external ophthalmoplegia (CPEO) is a major manifestation of human mitochondrial encephalomyopathies. Previous studies have shown cognitive deficits in patients with mitochondrial diseases. However, these studies often included patients with heterogeneous subtypes of mitochondrial diseases. Here, we aimed to provide a better cognitive profile of patients with CPEO by applying a comprehensive battery of neuropsychological assessments in a pure sample of patients with CPEO. We recruited 28 patients with CPEO (19 women, age 16–62 years) and 38 age- and education-matched healthy control subjects (25 women, age 16–60 years). The neuropsychological assessments covered global cognition and five cognitive domains (executive functions, language, working memory, memory, and visuospatial functions). We found that the patients were impaired in global cognition [Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA)], executive functions [Trail Making Test Part B (TMT-B)], and language [Boston Naming Test (BNT)], but not in working memory, memory or visuospatial functions. Moreover, individual patients' performances in the TMT-B (completion time) were predicted by the severity of non-ophthalmoplegia mitochondrial symptoms/signs [Newcastle Mitochondrial Disease Adult Scale (NMDAS)] and duration of the mitochondrial disease (years). Namely, patients with more severe non-ophthalmoplegia mitochondrial symptoms/signs and a longer disease duration took a longer time to complete the TMT-B. No clinical measures predicted individual patients' performances in the BNT.

Introduction

Mitochondrial encephalomyopathies are a heterogeneous group of inherited multisystem disorders that predominantly affect tissues with major aerobic metabolisms, including the central nervous system, muscle, retina, and tubular epithelium (1, 2). The major manifestations of mitochondrial encephalomyopathy include mitochondrial encephalopathy, lactic acidosis and stroke-like episodes (MELAS), chronic progressive external ophthalmoplegia (CPEO), Kearn-Sayre syndrome (KSS), and myoclonus epilepsy with ragged-red fibers syndrome (MERRF). In this study, we aimed to provide a cognitive profile of patients with CPEO by assessing their global cognition and five cognitive domains (executive functions, language, working memory, memory, and visuospatial functions).

Previous studies have systematically examined cognitive deficits and brain structural alterations in patients with MELAS. Symptoms of MELAS include epileptic seizures, transient ischemic attack, lactic acidosis, and ragged red fibers in muscle biopsy. MELAS has been associated with various point mutations. The most common mutation is m.3243A>G mutation (3). Neuropsychological studies showed that patients with MELAS are impaired in general intelligence (Wechsler Intelligence Scale), executive functions (Wisconsin Card Sorting Test and Trail Making Test), language [Boston Naming Test (BNT) and Verbal Fluency Test], attention/working memory (Digit Span Test), memory (Auditory Verbal Learning Test), and visuospatial functions (Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test and Block Design Test) (46). Moreover, patients with MELAS often show more severe deficits in working memory, memory, and visuospatial functions than patients with CPEO and patients with KSS (7). Brain imaging studies showed that patients with MELAS have a higher water and lactate level in the parietal and occipital regions, as well as calcification of the basal ganglia (6, 8).

The cognitive impact of CPEO is less understood. Symptoms of CPEO include progressive weakness of extraocular muscles, with or without extra-ocular manifestations. CPEO includes both sporadic and autosomal dominant and recessive inheritance. Sporadic CPEO is associated with single large-scale mitochondrial DNA deletions or point mutations. Autosomal dominant and recessive CPEO is associated with nuclear gene mutations which often cause secondary multiple mitochondrial DNA deletions (9). Because CPEO is rare (the incidence is only 1–2 in 100,000), previous studies on cognitive impairment often included a heterogeneous group of patients with CPEO and patients with other subtypes of mitochondrial diseases in order to reach an appropriate sample size. For example, Turconi et al. (10) included patients with CPEO, patients with KSS, and patients with MERRF. Bosbach et al. (11) included patients with CPEO and patients with KSS. These studies reported deficits in executive functions (Wisconsin Card Sorting Test), working memory (Digit Span Test), and visuospatial functions (Block Design Test and Rey Figure Copy Test), but not in general intelligence. It is unclear whether patients with CPEO are impaired in all three specific domains or only some of them.

To provide a better cognitive profile of patients with CPEO, we measured global cognition and five cognitive domains in a pure sample of patients with CPEO. We used the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) and the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) to evaluate global cognition; the Trail Making Test Part B (TMT-B) for executive functions, the BNT and Animal Fluency Test for language, the Digit Span Forward Test and Adaptive Digit Ordering Test for working memory, the Rey's Auditory Verbal Learning Test for memory, and the Block Design Test and Clock Drawing Test for visuospatial functions (1114). These tests were selected because (1) a revised and validated version of each test was readily available in China; and (2) the tests were less visually demanding (e.g., compared to the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test). We first examined whether the patients with CPEO were impaired in global cognition. After observing the global cognitive impairment, we then measured cognitive functions in each specific domain. Finally, we examined whether domain-specific cognitive impairment can be predicted by clinical features.

Materials and Methods

This study was approved by the ethics committee of Peking University First Hospital in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Each participant signed a written informed consent before participating in the study. For participants under the age of 18 years, their legal guardians signed the written informed consent.

Patients and Clinical Assessments

We recruited 28 patients with CPEO at the Peking University First Hospital Department of Neurology between September 2017 and October 2018. Inclusion criteria were (1) diagnosed with CPEO [modified diagnostic criteria for mitochondrial respiratory chain disorders (15)]; (2) age 16–65 years; (3) having completed primary education (≥5 years); (4) Mandarin Chinese speaking. Exclusion criteria were (1) symptoms of other mitochondrial diseases (e.g., stroke-like episode); (2) a history of other major neurologic (e.g., epilepsy) or psychiatric diseases (e.g., schizophrenia), stroke or brain injury; (3) alcohol or substance abuse; (4) severe ptosis, or visual or hearing deficits that may impair performance in cognitive assessments.

All patients were diagnosed by an experienced neurologist (Z.W.). Table 1 presents the demographic features, clinical phenotypes, brain structures, genotypes, and drugs of individual patients. All patients showed external ophthalmoplegia. Other clinical phenotypes included exercise intolerance (10 patients), muscle weakness (six patients), dysarthria (six patients), dysphagia (five patients), dyspnea (two patients), peripheral neuropathy (two patients), hearing loss (two patients), migraine (one patient), diabetes mellitus (one patient), and premature ovarian failure (one patient). We evaluated the severity of the mitochondrial disease using the Newcastle Mitochondrial Disease Adult Scale (NMDAS) (16). To assess the severity of ophthalmoplegia and that of other mitochondrial symptoms/signs, we separately calculated the ophthalmoplegia relevant items (items 2 and 3 of the current clinical assessment section) and the non-ophthalmoplegia items (all other items).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic features, clinical phenotypes, brain structures, genotypes, and drugs of individual patients.

All patients completed a brain magnetic resonance imaging examination. Two of the patients showed symmetric high T2 signals in cerebral subcortical white matter, indicating that they had leukoencephalopathy.

The clinical diagnosis was confirmed by muscle histopathology and genetic examinations (17). All patients showed ragged red or blue fibers, and cytochrome oxidase deficiency fibers on muscle biopsies (18). The genetic test was performed following a standard protocol as previously reported (1921). Twenty-four sporadic patients had single large-scale mitochondrial DNA deletions and four autosomal inherited patients had nuclear gene mutations, including ribonucleotide reductase M2B (two patients), DNA polymerase gamma (one patient), and thymidine kinase 2 (one patient).

All patients were assessed under their regular treatment, including vitamins (nine patients), coenzyme Q10 (eight patients), levocarnitine (eight patients), idebenone (seven patients), creatine monohydrate (seven patients), alpha-lipoic acid (two patients), or metformin (one patient).

Neuropsychological Assessments

We first evaluated the global cognition with the MoCA (22) and the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised in China (WAIS-RC) (23, 24). Note, the WAIS-RC was designed for individuals older than 16 years. We then assessed the patients' performances in five cognitive domains including executive functions (TMT-B), language (BNT and Animal Fluency Test), working memory (Digit Span Forward Test and Adaptive Digit Ordering Test), memory (Rey's Auditory Verbal Learning Test), and visuospatial functions (Clock Drawing Test and Block Design Test). Below we briefly describe each test and its key parameters.

Executive Functions

In the TMT-B (25), participants were asked to connect a sequence of 25 numbers in the ascending order as quickly as possible, and to alternate between numbers in squares and numbers in circles (e.g., yes). Key parameters of the test included the time used to complete the test and the number of errors.

Language

In the BNT (14, 26, 27), participants were given a set of 30 pictures and asked to name each picture within 20 s. They would be given a semantic or phonemic cue if they failed to name a particular picture correctly. We scored the total number of correct responses with or without semantic cues. In the Animal Fluency Test (28), participants were asked to name as many animals as possible in 1 min. We scored the total number of correct responses by excluding duplicates and by excluding category names when specific examples were given (e.g., excluding “fish,” when it was followed by “goldfish”).

Working Memory

In the Digit Span Forward Test (24, 29, 30), participants heard a sequence of 3–12 random digits at the rate of one digit per s in each trial. They were asked to immediately recall the digits in the original order. The test was adaptive regarding the length of each trial and was terminated when participants failed in both trials of a particular length. The Adaptive Digit Ordering Test (31) was similar to the digit span forward test except that participants were asked to recall the digits in ascending order. For each test, we scored the length of the last correctly recalled trial (span).

Memory

In Rey's Auditory Verbal Learning Test (14, 32), participants were asked to learn 15 words and to recall as many words as possible five times immediately, and once after 30 min. We scored the total number of correctly recalled words in the immediate recalls and the number of correctly recalled words in the delayed recall.

Visuospatial Functions

In the Clock Drawing Test (14, 33), participants were asked to draw a clock, to place the numbers in the correct positions, and to draw the hands to indicate 10 past 11. We used the scoring method Shulman (34). In the Block Design Test (14, 24), participants were asked to arrange a set of nine blocks to match a given pattern as quickly as possible. We used the scoring method of Peng and Zhang (14).

We additionally monitored mood and sleep symptoms using the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) (35) and REM Sleep Behavior Disorder Screening Questionnaire (RBDSQ) (36).

Healthy Control Subjects

We recruited 38 age- and education-matched healthy control subjects. Exclusion criteria were (1) a history of significant neurologic or psychiatric disorders, stroke, or brain injury; (2) possible dementia or mild cognitive impairment (MoCA ≥ 26/30); (3) possible current depression (BDI-II ≤ 7); (4) alcohol abuse or use of addictive drugs (e.g., opioid drugs). They completed the same assessments for cognition, mood, and sleep as the patients.

Statistical Analysis

Data were processed with IBM SPSS Statistics 20. First, we examined whether the patients were different from the healthy control subjects in global cognition (MoCA and WAIS-RC) using two-sample t-tests (two-tailed, p < 0.025 Bonferroni correction for two tests). Given the presence of deficits in global cognition, we then examined whether the patients performed worse than healthy control subjects in each cognitive domain using two-sample t-tests (one-tailed, p < 0.005 Bonferroni correction for 10 tests). After observing group differences in executive functions and language, we finally used linear regression models to examine whether individual patients' performances in the TMT-B (the completion time) or BNT (the number of correct responses) can be statistically predicted by their clinical features (p < 0.025 Bonferroni correction for two models). Independent variables were the severity of ophthalmoplegia (ophthalmoplegia relevant NMDAS subscore), the severity of other mitochondrial symptoms/signs (non-ophthalmoplegia NMDAS subscore), disease duration (years), weight status (body mass index), and sleep symptoms (RBDSQ score). The body mass index and the RBDSQ score were included because they showed significant group differences (see Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Demographic and clinical features of the patients and healthy control subjects (means, standard deviations, and group differences).

Results

Demographic and Clinical Features

Table 2 shows the demographic and clinical features of the patients and healthy control subjects. The two-sample t-tests (two-tailed, p < 0.008 Bonferroni correction for six tests) revealed significant group differences in the body mass index [t(64) = 4.14, p < 0.001] and RBDSQ score [t(64) = 3.23, p = 0.002]. The patients had a lower body mass index and a higher RBDSQ score. The underweight of the patients may be due to the dysfunction of aerobic energy production (2). The potential RBD sleep problems are not often reported in patients with mitochondrial disorders but are common in neurodegenerative disorders such as Parkinson's disease.

Global Cognition

Table 3 shows measures of the patients and healthy control subjects in global cognition and in each cognitive domain. The two-sample t-tests (two-tailed, p < 0.025 Bonferroni correction for two tests) revealed a significant group difference in the MoCA [t(64) = 2.34, p = 0.025] but not in the WAIS-RC [t(64) = 1.69, p = 0.099]. MoCA may be more specific to the types of cognitive deficits in patients with CPEO.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Cognitive measures of the patients and healthy control subjects (means, standard deviations, and group differences).

Five Cognitive Domains

Having observed the deficits in global cognition, we then examined whether the patients performed worse than healthy control subjects in each cognitive domain using two-sample t-tests (one-tailed, p < 0.005 Bonferroni correction for 10 tests). In brief, the patients were impaired in executive functions and language but not in working memory, memory or visuospatial functions (see Table 3).

For executive functions, the patients tended to perform more slowly [t(63) = 2.66, p = 0.006] but not to make more errors than healthy control subjects in the TMT-B. In the language domain, the patients scored lower than healthy control subjects in the BNT [t(61) = 2.91, p = 0.003], but not in the Animal Fluency Test [t(64) = 1.34, p = 0.093]. We obtained no group difference in working memory, memory, or visuospatial functions.

Relationship Between Executive Dysfunction and Clinical Features

Figure 1 shows the relationship between the measures of executive functions and clinical features. The linear regression model for the TMT-B was significant [F(5,21) = 8.02, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.57]. The TMT-B completion time was statistically predicted by the severity of non-ophthalmoplegia mitochondrial symptoms/signs (beta = 8.84, t = 3.52, p = 0.002) and the disease duration (beta = 2.66, t = 2.56, p = 0.018), but not by the severity of ophthalmoplegia (beta = −2.64, t < 1). The patients with more severe non-ophthalmoplegia mitochondrial symptoms/signs and a longer disease duration performed more slowly in the TMT-B.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Individual patients' performances in the TMT-B (completion time) were predicted (A) not by the severity of ophthalmoplegia (ophthalmoplegia relevant NMDAS subscore) but by (B) the severity of other mitochondrial symptoms/signs (non-ophthalmoplegia NMDAS subscore) and (C) the disease duration (years) when weight status and sleep symptoms were controlled. Values were demeaned.

No Relationship Between Language and Clinical Features

The linear regression model for the BNT was not significant [F(5,21) = 1.17, p = 0.356, R2 = 0.03]. The patients' performances in the BNT were not predicted by the severity of ophthalmoplegia, the severity of non-ophthalmoplegia mitochondrial symptoms/signs, or other clinical features.

Discussion

In this study, we measured the global cognition and five cognitive domains in patients with CPEO and healthy control subjects. The patients were impaired in global cognition and in executive functions and language, but not in working memory, memory or visuospatial functions. To be specific, the patients scored lower in MoCA. They tended to perform more slowly in the TMT-B and to name fewer pictures in the BNT. Individual patients' performances in the TMT-B, but not in the BNT, were predicted by the severity of non-ophthalmoplegia mitochondrial symptoms/signs and the duration of the mitochondrial disease. Patients who had more severe non-ophthalmoplegia mitochondrial symptoms/signs and a longer disease duration tended to perform more slowly in the TMT-B.

This study is consistent with previous findings that patients with CPEO performed equally well as healthy control subjects in the WAIS (11). However, we observed a worse MoCA in patients with CPEO, which suggests the existence of the deterioration in global cognition in these patients. The type of assessment made by MoCA may be more specific to the types of deficits in patients with CPEO (e.g., executive dysfunction).

We also confirmed that patients with CPEO are impaired in executive functions. Previous studies showed that a heterogeneous group of patients with CPEO and patients with KSS made more preservative errors than healthy control subjects in the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (11). Here we demonstrated that patients with CPEO took a longer time to complete the TMT-B than healthy control subjects. The Wisconsin Card Sorting Test and the TMT-B share a common feature that participants have to shift frequently between rules or items. Together, these findings suggest that patients with CPEO may be impaired in task switching and set-shifting. A novel finding of this study is that individual patients' differences in the TMT-B were predicted by the severity of non-ophthalmoplegia mitochondrial symptoms/signs. It suggests that the patients' poor performances in the TMT-B was unlikely due to saccade problems (e.g., moving visual fixation from one number or letter to the next).

Regarding language deficits, previous studies showed that patients with CPEO or KSS were impaired in word comprehension (11). This study suggests that patients with CPEO may also be impaired in object naming, even though they seemed to perform normally in animal name generation. Similar deficits in object naming have been reported in patients with MELAS (37). As patients with MELAS do not often have significant ophthalmoparesis, the underlying pathophysiology of the naming deficits is likely beyond the ophthalmoplegia. This hypothesis is supported by the absence of a correlation between the naming performance and the severity of ophthalmoplegia in this study.

In mitochondrial diseases, mutant mitochondrial DNA accumulates in the frontal lobe more than the parietal, occipital, or temporal lobes (38). The progressive accumulation of mutant mitochondrial DNA in the frontal lobe may lead to increased oxidative metabolism stress that impairs functions of frontal neurons and glial cells (39, 40) and consequently may harm the cognitive functions that heavily rely on the frontal lobe (e.g., executive functions and language).

We did not find deficits in working memory, memory, or visuospatial functions, which have been reported by previous studies that included patients with heterogeneous subtypes of mitochondrial diseases. The inconsistency may come from the variability in working memory deficits, memory deficits, and visuospatial deficits in the subtypes of mitochondrial diseases. For example, Lang et al. (7) showed that patients with MELAS tended to perform worse than patients with CPEO in working memory, memory, and visuospatial tests. Bosbach et al. (11) showed that patients with KSS scored abnormally lower in 31% of the neuropsychological tests, while patients with CPEO only scored abnormally in 24% of the neuropsychological tests (11). Potential deficits in working memory, memory, or visuospatial functions may not be central to the cognitive profile of patients with CPEO.

In conclusion, we provided a cognitive profile of patients with CPEO. Patients with CPEO were impaired in global cognition and specifically in executive functions and language. Individual patients' differences in executive functions can be predicted by the severity of non-ophthalmoplegia mitochondrial symptoms/signs and the duration of the mitochondrial disease. CPEO patients with more severe non-ophthalmoplegia mitochondrial symptoms/signs and a longer disease duration tended to be worse in executive functions. Executive dysfunction may be a characteristic cognitive feature of patients with CPEO. Future research needs to explore the neural circuits underlying the executive dysfunction (e.g., impaired set-shifting) and potential therapeutic approaches for enhancing executive functions in patients with CPEO.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

This study involving human participants was reviewed and approved by the ethics committee of Peking University First Hospital. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants or their legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

ZY and ZW designed and organized the study. GZ and YH collected the data. GZ analyzed the data. GZ and ZY wrote the first draft of the manuscript. ZW and YH revised the manuscript. All authors approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (31771216) and the Shanghai Municipal Science and Technology Major Project (2018SHZDZX05).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Yuanyuan Ma, Qi Wang, and Minghong Su for their assistance in data acquisition.

References

1. DiMauro S, Bonilla E, Zeviani M, Nakagawa M, DeVivo DC. Mitochondrial myopathies. Ann Neurol. (1985) 17:521–38. doi: 10.1002/ana.410170602

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Morgan-Hughes JA. Mitochondrial diseases. Trends Neurosci. (1986) 9:15–9. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(86)90006-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Schon EA, Bonilla E, Dimauro S. Mitochondrial DNA mutations and pathogenesis. J Bioenerg Biomembr. (1997) 29:131–49. doi: 10.1023/A:1022685929755

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Kraya T, Neumann L, Paelecke-Habermann Y, Deschauer M, Stoevesandt D, Zierz S, et al. Cognitive impairment, clinical severity and MRI changes in MELAS syndrome. Mitochondrion. (2017) 44:53–7. doi: 10.1016/j.mito.2017.12.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Sartor H, Loose R, Tucha O, Klein HE, Lange KW. MELAS: a neuropsychological and radiological follow-up study. Acta Neurol Scand. (2002) 106:309–13. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0404.2002.01089.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Neargarder SA, Murtagh MP, Wong B, Hill EK. The neuropsychologic deficits of MELAS. Cogn Behav Neurol. (2007) 20:83–92. doi: 10.1097/WNN.0b013e3180335faf

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Lang CJG, Brenner P, Heub D, Engelhardt A, Reichmann H, Seibel P, et al. Neuropsychological status of mitochondrial encephalomyopathies. Eur J Neurol. (1995) 2:171–6. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-1331.1995.tb00112.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Barkovich AJ, Good WV, Koch TK, Berg BO. Mitochondrial disorders: analysis of their clinical and imaging characteristics. Am Soc Neuroradiol. (1993) 14:1119–37.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

9. Viscomi C, Zeviani M. MtDNA-maintenance defects: syndromes and genes. J Inherit Metab Dis. (2017) 40:587–99. doi: 10.1007/s10545-017-0027-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Turconi AC, Benti R, Castelli E, Pochintesta S, Felisari G, Comi G, et al. Focal cognitive impairment in mitochondial encephalomyopathies: a neuropsychological and neuroimaging study. J Neurol Sci. (1999) 170:57–63. doi: 10.1016/S0022-510X(99)00199-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Bosbach S, Kornblum C, Schröder R, Wagner M. Executive and visuospatial deficits in patients with chronic progressive external ophthalmoplegia and Kearns-Sayre Syndrome. Brain. (2003) 126:1231–40. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Maruta C, Guerreiro M, De Mendonça A, Hort J, Scheltens P. The use of neuropsychological tests across Europe: the need for a consensus in the use of assessment tools for dementia. Eur J Neurol. (2011) 18:279–85. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-1331.2010.03134.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Guo Q-H, Hong Z. Neuropsychological Assessments. Shanghai: Shanghai Scientific and Technical Publishers (2016).

Google Scholar

14. Peng D-T, Zhang Z-J. Guidelines for the Operation of Neuropsychological Scales. Beijing: People's Medical Publishing House (2015).

15. Bernier FP, Boneh A, Dennett X, Chow CW, Cleary MA, Thorburn DR. Diagnostic criteria for respiratory chain disorders in adults and children. Neurology. (2002) 59:1406–11. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000033795.17156.00

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Schaefer AM, Phoenix C, Elson JL, McFarland R, Chinnery PF, Turnbull DM. Mitochondrial disease in adults: a scale to monitor progression and treatment. Neurology. (2006) 66:1932–4. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000219759.72195.41

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Finsterer J, Harbo HF, Baets J, Van Broeckhoven C, Di Donato S, Fontaine B, et al. EFNS guidelines on the molecular diagnosis of mitochondrial disorders. Eur J Neurol. (2009) 16:1255–64. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-1331.2009.02811.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Inczédy-Farkas G. Cognitive and psychiatric aspects of mitochondrial encephalomyopathies (Doctoral dissertation). Semmelweis University, Budapest, Hungary (2014). doi: 10.14753/SE.2014.1965

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Hou Y, Xie Z, Zhao X, Yuan Y, Dou P, Wang Z. Appendicular skeletal muscle mass: a more sensitive biomarker of disease severity than BMI in adults with mitochondrial diseases. PLoS ONE. (2019) 14:e0219628. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0219628

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Zhao D, Wang Z, Hong D, Zhang W, Yuan Y. Chronic progressive external ophthalmoplegia coexistent with motor neuron disease in a patient with a novel large-scale mitochondrial DNA deletion. Clin Neurol Neurosurg. (2013) 115:1490–2. doi: 10.1016/j.clineuro.2012.11.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Yu M, Zheng Y, Jin S, Gang Q, Wang Q, Yu P, et al. Mutational spectrum of Chinese LGMD patients by targeted next-generation sequencing. PLoS ONE. (2017) 12:e0175343. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0175343

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Chen K-L, Xu Y, Chu A-Q, Ding D, Liang X-N, Nasreddine ZS, et al. Validation of the Chinese version of montreal cognitive assessment basic for screening mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc. (2016) 64:e285–90. doi: 10.1111/jgs.14530

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Guertin WH, Rabin AI, Frank GH, Ladd CE. Research with the wechsler intelligence scales for adults: 1955–60. Psychol Bull. (1962) 59:1. doi: 10.1037/h0040560

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Gong Y-X. The Handbook of Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised in China. Haikou: Hainan Map Publishing House (1992).

25. Lu J-C, Guo Q-H, Hong Z, Shi W-X, Lü C-Z. Trail making test used by chinese elderly patients with mild cognitive impairment and mild Alzheimer's dementia. Chin J Clin Psychol. (2006) 14:118–20. doi: 10.16128/j.cnki.1005-3611.2006.02.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Strauss E, Sherman EMS, Spreen O. A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests. Administration, Norms, and Commentary. New York, NY: Oxford University Press (1998).

Google Scholar

27. Guo Q-H, Hong Z, Shi W-X, Sun Y-M, Lü C-Z. Boston naming test in chinese elderly patients with mild cognitive impairment and mild Alzheimer's dementia. Chin J Clin Psychol. (2006) 20:81–4.

Google Scholar

28. Fang R, Wang G, Huang Y, Zhuang J-P, Tang H-D, Wang Y, et al. Validation of the Chinese version of Addenbrooke's cognitive examination-revised for screening mild Alzheimer's disease and mild cognitive impairment. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. (2014) 37:223–31. doi: 10.1159/000353541

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Wechsler D. WMS-R Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised. Manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation (1987).

30. Wechsler D. A standardized memory scale for clinical use. J Psychol. (1945) 19:87–95. doi: 10.1080/00223980.1945.9917223

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Werheid K, Hoppe C, Thone A, Muller U, Mungersdorf M, Von Cramon DY. The adaptive digit ordering test clinical application, reliability, and validity of a verbal working memory test. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. (2002) 17:547–65. doi: 10.1093/arclin/17.6.547

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Rey A. L' Examen Clinique en Psychologie. Paris: Press Universitaire de France (1958).

Google Scholar

33. Shulman KI, Shedletsky R, Silver IL. The challenge of time: clock-drawing and cognitive function in the elderly. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. (1986) 1:135–40. doi: 10.1002/gps.930010209

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Shulman KI. Clock-drawing: is it the ideal cognitive screening test? Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2000) 15:548–61. doi: 10.1002/1099-1166(200006)15:6<548::AID-GPS242>3.0.CO;2-U

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Beck AT, Beamesderfer A. Assessment of depression: the depression inventory. Psychol Meas Psychopharmacol. (1974) 7:151–69. doi: 10.1159/000395074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Stiasny-Kolster K, Mayer G, Schäfer S, Möller JC, Heinzel-Gutenbrunner M, Oertel WH. The REM sleep behavior disorder screening questionnaire-A new diagnostic instrument. Mov Disord. (2007) 22:2386–93. doi: 10.1002/mds.21740

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Salsano E, Giovagnoli AR, Morandi L, Maccagnano C, Lamantea E, Marchesi C, et al. Mitochondrial dementia: a sporadic case of progressive cognitive and behavioral decline with hearing loss due to the rare m.3291T>C MELAS mutation. J Neurol Sci. (2011) 300:165–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2010.09.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Amemiya S, Hamamoto M, Goto Y, Komaki H, Nishino I, Nonaka I, et al. Psychosis and progressing dementia: presenting features of a mitochondriopathy. Neurology. (2000) 55:600–1. doi: 10.1212/WNL.55.4.600

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Finsterer J. Mitochondrial disorders, cognitive impairment and dementia. J Neurol Sci. (2009) 283:143–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2009.02.347

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Roland PE, Eriksson L, Stone-Elander S, Widen L. Does mental activity change the oxidative metabolism of the brain? J Neurosci. (1987) 7:2373–89.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Keywords: mitochondrial chronic progressive external ophthalmoplegia, cognition, executive functions, language, working memory, memory, visuospatial functions

Citation: Zhang G, Hou Y, Wang Z and Ye Z (2020) Cognitive Profile of Patients With Mitochondrial Chronic Progressive External Ophthalmoplegia. Front. Neurol. 11:36. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2020.00036

Received: 17 September 2019; Accepted: 10 January 2020;
Published: 29 January 2020.

Edited by:

Victoria Susan Pelak, University of Colorado, United States

Reviewed by:

Essam Mohamed Elmatbouly Saber, Benha University, Egypt
Daniel Ross Gold, Johns Hopkins University, United States

Copyright © 2020 Zhang, Hou, Wang and Ye. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Zhaoxia Wang, drwangzx@163.com; Zheng Ye, yez@ion.ac.cn

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.