Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurol., 14 November 2019
Sec. Applied Neuroimaging
This article is part of the Research Topic The Cerebellum: From Vascular Disease to Neurodegeneration View all 6 articles

Impaired Adaptive Motor Learning Is Correlated With Cerebellar Hemispheric Gray Matter Atrophy in Spinocerebellar Ataxia Patients: A Voxel-Based Morphometry Study

\nKyota Bando,,Kyota Bando1,2,3Takeru HondaTakeru Honda4Kinya IshikawaKinya Ishikawa5Yuji TakahashiYuji Takahashi3Hidehiro MizusawaHidehiro Mizusawa3Takashi Hanakawa,,
Takashi Hanakawa1,2,6*
  • 1Department of Advanced Neuroimaging, Integrative Brain Imaging Center, National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, Tokyo, Japan
  • 2Department of NCNP Brain Physiology and Pathology, Graduate School, Tokyo Medical and Dental University, Tokyo, Japan
  • 3National Center Hospital, National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, Kodaira, Japan
  • 4Motor Disorders Project, Tokyo Metropolitan Institute of Medical Science, Tokyo, Japan
  • 5Department of Neurology and Neurological Science, Graduate School, Tokyo Medical and Dental University, Tokyo, Japan
  • 6Department of Integrated Neuroanatomy and Neuroimaging, Kyoto University Graduate School of Medicine, Kyoto, Japan

Objective: To evaluate the degree to which recently proposed parameters measured via a prism adaptation task are correlated with changes in cerebellar structure, specifically gray matter volume (GMV), in patients with spinocerebellar degeneration (SCD).

Methods: We performed whole-brain voxel-based morphometry (VBM) analysis on 3-dimensional T1-weighted images obtained from 23 patients with SCD [Spinocerebellar ataxia type 6 (SCA6), 31 (SCA31), 3/Machado-Joseph disease (SCA3/MJD), and sporadic cortical cerebellar atrophy (CCA)] and 21 sex- and age-matched healthy controls (HC group). We quantified a composite index representing adaptive motor learning abilities in a hand-reaching task with prism adaptation. After controlling for age, sex, and total intracranial volume, we analyzed group-wise differences in GMV and regional GMV correlations with the adaptive learning index.

Results: Compared with the HC group, the SCD group showed reduced adaptive learning abilities and smaller GMV widely in the lobules IV-VIII in the bilateral cerebellar hemispheres. In the SCD group, the adaptive learning index was correlated with cerebellar hemispheric atrophy in the right lobule VI, the left Crus I. Additionally, GMV of the left supramarginal gyrus showed a correlation with the adaptive learning index in the SCD group, while the supramarginal region did not accompany reduction of GMV.

Conclusions: This study indicated that a composite index derived from a prism adaptation task was correlated with GMV of the lateral cerebellum and the supramarginal gyrus in patients with SCD. This study should contribute to the development of objective biomarkers for disease severity and progression in SCD.

Introduction

Spinocerebellar degeneration (SCD) is a degenerative neurological disease that manifests as brain atrophy in multiple brain areas, particularly the cerebellum, and is characterized by clinical manifestations of ataxic gait, dysarthria, dysmetria, and dysdiadochokinesia (1). To date, more than 40 subtypes of SCDs and 38 causative genes are identified (2). SCD is roughly classified into a pure cerebellar atrophy type (e.g., SCA6 and SCA31) and a multiple system atrophy type (e.g., SCA1, 2, and 3) (1).

Previous interventional and cross-sectional studies of SCD have mostly adopted omnibus functional motility scores such as the Scale for the Assessment and Rating of Ataxia (SARA) as outcome measure (3, 4). To advance the development of therapeutic options, development of a fine-graded, specific, quantitative measure for the assessment of cerebellar function is necessary, particularly one that is applicable even at the early stages of the disease.

The cerebellum has multiple functions in motor regulation, learning, and behavior (5, 6). Since the pioneering theoretical work of Albus (7) and Marr (8), the cerebellum has been considered a crucial brain structure for motor learning through the development of “internal models (911)”. Indeed, experimental evidence supports the contribution of the cerebellum to conditioning (12), habituation (13), and scaling of various reflexes (14, 15). The cerebellum has also been implicated in complex forms of motor learning, such as sensorimotor adaptation to visual and mechanical distortions. Indeed, previous clinical studies observed that sensorimotor adaptation was often reduced or abolished in SCD patients (16, 17). Among existing sensorimotor adaptation tasks, prism adaptation has been long known as a method for testing cerebellar function and has been used to assess sensorimotor adaptation in individuals with SCD (18, 19).

However, which quantitative measures from prism adaptation are useful in the assessment of cerebellar functions in SCD patients is unclear. Recently, Hashimoto et al. developed the Adaptability Index (AI), which is a composite index computed from several parameters measured during prism adaptation, and demonstrated its clinical efficacy in discriminating patients with SCD from healthy people (20) However, whether the AI is associated with the degree of cerebellar atrophy and which sectors of the cerebellum are responsible for reduced AI values in SCD patients remain unclear.

Voxel-based morphometry (VBM) is a well-established image analysis technique that provides an unbiased and comprehensive assessment of anatomical differences throughout the brain with high local specificity (21) VBM analysis is regarded as a useful method for elucidating the pattern of brain atrophy across different types of SCD and for correlating disease severity with the degree of brain atrophy (2225).

Here, we conducted structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and compared gray matter volume (GMV) with scores on a hand-reaching task with prism adaptation in patients with SCD and healthy controls (HC). Our objective was to examine the relationship between adaptive learning ability and regional patterns of cerebellar atrophy.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Twenty-three patients with SCD were enrolled (Table 1). In the SCD group, 22 patients had genetically confirmed spinocerebellar ataxia (SCA) (2628) and the remaining one patient was diagnosed clinically as CCA (29, 30). All of the patients were assessed using the SARA (31). The disease onset was defined as the age at which the patients first noticed signs of cerebellar ataxia. This allowed us to define disease duration. As an HC group, 21 age- and sex-matched healthy people were recruited (Table 2). All participants were right-handed. The results of neurological examinations were normal for the individuals in the HC group, indicating no subjective/objective cognitive disturbances. In both the HC and SCD groups, the exclusion criteria were a Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) score of <24 point (32) and organic brain lesions as indicated via MRI, including age-related changes in white matter (a periventricular hyperintensity rating of 2 or greater or a deep white matter hyperintensity rating of 2 or greater) (33). The SCD group and HC group were similar in terms of mean age [SCD group = 61.3 ± 10.1 years old and HC group = 57.8 ± 11.1 years old; t(42) = 1.1, p = 0.27], sex distribution (13 males and 10 females in SCD group; 8 males, and 13 females in HC group = x2 = 1.49, p = 0.22), and MMSE score [SCD group = 28.3 ± 2.2, HC group = 28.8 ± 1.4, t(42) = 0.96, p = 0.34]. This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of Bioethics Committee of National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the Bioethics Committee of National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characteristics of the SCD.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Characteristics of the HC group.

Prism Adaptation Experiment

The experimental apparatus was previously described by Hashimoto et al. (20). In brief, the participants wore goggles containing a Fresnel prism plate, which shifted the field of view 25° to the right. An electrically controlled shutter screen was installed in the goggles to interrupt visual information during reaching. A participant initially touched his/her index finger to a sensor switch attached to the right earlobe, and then a target (a white circle with a 15-mm radius) was presented on a touchscreen. When the participant started a reaching movement, the release of the sensor switch triggered the electrical shutter, which prevented the participant from seeing his/her hand and the target during reaching. The participant was able to see the target and their hand only after the screen had been touched and the electrical shutter had reopened. In the next trial, the participant attempted to correct his/her reaching movements on the basis of the reaching error in the previous trial. We used original software (Katano Tool Software) on a personal computer to control the task.

The participants performed 100 practice trials without the prism lens, followed by three consecutive test sessions: (1) 50 trials without the prism plate (BASELINE), (2) 100 trials with the prism plate (PRISM), and (3) 50 trials without the prism plate (REMOVAL). According to previous research (20), trials with a correct touch movement were defined as those with a touch error below 25 mm.

Analysis of the Prism Experiment Data

We analyzed the data from the prism adaptation trial using the method proposed by Hashimoto et al. (20). First, we calculated AI as follows:

AI=a ×b×c

where “a” is the adaptation index defined as the probability of correct touches in the last 10 trials of the PRISM session; “b” is the retention index defined as the probability of incorrect touches in the initial five trials of the REMOVAL session; and “c” is the extinction index designated as the probability of correct touches in the last 10 trials of the REMOVAL session. The AI score is a composite index that primarily reflects the combined ability for motor adaption including initial adaptation, retention, and re-adaptation.

Second, we measured the magnitude of finger touch errors (r) as the degree of horizontal displacement between the touch points and the center of the target on the touchscreen. The adaptation curve in PRISM session was fitted as follows:

r=η·exp(-t/τ)

where “η” is the degree of finger touch errors in the first trial of the PRISM session; “t” is the trial number from the start of the session; and the time constant “τ” is the number of trials that had been completed in a session at which the finger touch error approached 36.8% of η. Finally, we performed natural logarithmic conversion according to the skewed distribution of τ (τ_log).

MRI Acquisition

MRI measurements were performed using a 3-Tesla scanner (Siemens Verio) with a 32-channel phased array head coil. Structural MRI was acquired with a 3-dimensional T1-weighted magnetization-prepared rapid gradient echo (MPRAGE) sequence: sagittal acquisition, repetition time (TR) = 1,900 ms, echo time (TE) = 2.52 ms, inversion time (TI) = 900 ms, flip-angle (FA) = 9°, field of view (FOV) = 256 mm, 256 × 256 acquisition matrix, and a voxel size of 1 × 1 × 1 mm3. We also referred to a T2-weighted sequence (TR = 4,000 ms, TE = 91 ms, FA = 150°, FOV = 220, 256 × 256 matrix, a voxel size of 0.5 × 0.5 × 3.0 mm3, and a 10 mm gap between slices).

Voxel-Based Morphometry With DARTEL

The MRI data were processed using statistical parametric mapping 12 (SPM12; Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging) in MATLAB version 9.1.0 (Mathworks). For each participant, the T1-weighted image was segmented into gray matter (GM), white matter (WM), cerebrospinal fluid (CSF), skull, and scalp using the “new segment” routine. Then, the GM images were rigid body-aligned to the tissue probability maps in Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) standard space. We created a study-specific template by averaging images from 23 patients with SCD and 21 subjects from the HC group, using the DARTEL (diffeomorphic anatomical registration through exponentiated Lie algebra) toolbox (34). Subsequently, all native-space GM images were registered to this study-specific template and further spatially normalized into standard MNI space (1.5 mm isotropic voxel). The resulting GM images were modulated using the Jacobian determinant of the corresponding deformation, filed to correct for volume changes. Finally, the modulated GM images were smoothed using an isotropic Gaussian kernel of 8 mm full-width at half maximum.

Statistical Analysis

All statistical analyses of demographic data were performed using R version 3.3.3. (35). Age, sex, MMSE scores, and adaptive learning ability parameters (AI and τ_log) were compared between the groups using two-sample Student's t-tests and Pearson's chi-squared test. Spearmans's rank order correlation (rs) was used to examine the strength of the association between two variables: between AI and SARA score, between AI and disease duration, between τ_log and SARA score, and between τ_log and disease duration. All data are reported as the mean ± standard deviation (SD). The P-value for statistical significance was set at < 0.05. To examine between-group differences in regional GMV, we used a voxel-wise general linear model with a 1-factor 2-level ANCOVA design with age, sex, and TIV as covariates (36). To examine correlations between regional GMV and adaptive learning ability parameters, we performed a multiple regression analysis after controlling for age and TIV. A peak-level threshold was initially set at P < 0.05 with family-wise error (FWE) correction for multiple comparisons. This conservative approach assumed significant relationship between GMV and the effects of interest in a voxel-wise manner. We also used a second, complementary approach in which we assumed moderate relationship between GMV and effects of interest might be expanded in space beyond the chance level. For this cluster-level approach, the initial voxel-wise peak threshold was set at P < 0.001 uncorrected, and the resultant clusters were considered significant when falling below an extent threshold of FWE-corrected P < 0.05. For this approach, since structural images display local variation in smoothness, cluster-level correction was applied using Random Field Theory combined with non-stationary correction (37).

Results

Differences in Adaptive Learning Ability Parameters Between Groups

AI was significantly lower in the SCD group compared with the HC group [SCD group = 0.24 ± 0.22, HC group = 0.71 ± 0.18; t(42) = 7.57, p < 0.001]. Also, τ_log was significantly larger in the SCD group compared with the HC group [SCD group = 5.45 ± 5.09, HC group = 1.42 ± 1.15; t(42) = 3.47, p < 0.001].

Correlation of Adaptive Learning Ability Parameters With Clinical Indices of SCD

In the SCD group, a negative correlation was observed between AI and SARA score (rs = −0.63, p < 0.01, Figure 1A). There was no significant correlation between AI and disease duration (rs = 0.1, p = 0.66, Figure 1B), between τ_log and SARA (rs = 0.09, p = 0.69, Figure 1C), or between τ_log and disease duration (rs = 0.23, p = 0.32, Figure 1D).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Adaptive learning indexes and other clinical indexes in SCD group. (A,B) Scatter plots comparing AI with SARA score (A), disease duration (B). (C,D) Scatter plots comparing τ_log with SARA score (C), disease duration (D). rs, Spearman's rank order correlation coefficients.

Differences in Regional GMV Between Groups

The SCD group had significantly smaller GMV in the cerebellar right hemispheric Lobule IV, V, VI, VIII, and IX, and in the left hemispheric Lobule IV, V, VI, VIII, and IX, and Crus I and II (Peak-level threshold of FWE corrected p < 0.05) (Figure 2, Table 3).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Significant GMV atrophy in the SCD group compared with the HC group. Coronal brain sections. Individuals with SCD had smaller GMVs compared with the HC group in widespread regions of the cerebellum. FWE corrected p < 0.05.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Clusters of significant GM atrophy in the SCD group relative to the HC group.

Correlations Between Adaptive Learning Ability Parameters and GMV in the SCD Group

In the SCD group, a whole-brain multiple regression analysis indicated that lower AI scores were associated with smaller GMV in the right cerebellar hemispheric lobule VI and left cerebellar Crus I (Peak-level threshold of FWE corrected p < 0.05, Figure 3A, Table 4). Additionally, a complementary multiple regression analysis identified the left supramarginal gyrus, the right cerebellar hemisphere (Crus I and lobule VI–VIII) and the left cerebellar hemisphere (lobule IV–VI and Crus I) showing non-negligible association between GM atrophy and reduced AI in the SCD group (Cluster-level threshold of FWE corrected P < 0.05) (Figure 3A, Table 5). We did not observe any clear differences among individuals with different types of SCA in terms of the correlation pattern between AI score and cerebellar or parietal GMV (Figure 3B). There was no correlation between AI and GMV within the HC group at both thresholds. The τ_log values were not significantly correlated with GMV in either the SCD group or the HC group even at the liberal threshold.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Regions with correlations between GMV and AI in the SCD group. (A) Coronal sections showing regions in which gray matter was significantly correlated with AI. Red color = peak-level p < 0.05 (FWE corrected). Yellow color, cluster-level p < 0.05 (FWE corrected). (B) Scatter plot depicting the correlation between regional GMV at the peak voxel and AI. The dotted crossed line shows the mean and standard deviation (SD) of the AI and peak voxel values in the SCD group. The solid crossed line shows the mean and SD of the AI and peak voxel values in the HC group. rs, Spearman's rank order correlation coefficients; SMG, supramarginal gyrus.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Clusters with significant correlations between GM volume and AI in the SCD group (peak-level corrected).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Clusters with significant correlations between GM volume and AI in the SCD group (cluster-level corrected).

Discussion

The main finding of the present study was that reduction AI was correlated with GM atrophy in the cerebellum and parietal cortex in the SCD group, pooling across SCA types and CCA. We observed three main types of SCA amongst our participants. SCA6 is an autosomal dominant type of cerebellar ataxia, and is considered to be a “pure” type of cerebellar ataxia (32). SCA31 is a type of elderly-onset, pure cerebellar ataxia that is unique in Japan (38). In both SCA6 and SCA31, Purkinje cells, which are the only projecting neurons in the cerebellar cortex, are predominantly affected (26, 27). In SCA3/MJD, degenerative processes involve widely distributed parts of the brain, including the cerebral cortex, basal ganglia, and pontomedullary systems, in addition to the cerebellum (22, 24).

The studied SCA subtypes were consistent with those included in the previous behavioral study by Hashimoto et al. (20). In their study, 17% of the SCD patients (12 out of 70) showed an AI of 0; consistently, 13% of the SCD patients (3 out of 23) showed an AI of 0 here. This finding of apparently “no adaptation at all (AI = 0)” reflects the design of the equation computing AI in which 0 in any one of the parameters (a, b, and c) results in AI of 0, emphasizing a subtle adaptation failure. Moreover, we replicated significant correlation between SARA and AI. Overall, these findings validated the clinical usefulness of AI for assessing patients with SCD. The failure to replicate the relationship between AI and disease duration suggests that the current cohort contained various types of SCDs (32, 38).

Our VBM results showed that AI score was correlated with GMV in the right cerebellar hemisphere (Crus I and lobule VI–VIII) and the left cerebellar hemisphere (lobule IV–VI and Crus I) in the SCD group. Particularly, the right lobule VI and the left Crus I showed most robust correlation. The supporting evidence is available from three perspectives, described below. First, these cerebellar regions in which GMV was correlated with AI score are consistent with the correlates of prism adaptation in previous human and non-human primate studies. Lesion (39) and pharmacological inactivation (40) studies in non-human primates have shown that cerebellar hemisphere lobules VII, VIII, IX, and the dentate nucleus were involved in prism adaptation during a reaching task. Furthermore, human lesion studies have suggested that cerebellar hemispheres IV, V, and VI are involved in adaptation during the hand-reaching task (41, 42).

Second, the right lobule VI and left Crus I have been found to be the correlates of eye-hand coordination required for prism adaptation. The lobule VI likely corresponds to hand representations in the cerebellum, which are somatotopically organized (43), and an MRI study indicated that the right cerebellar hemisphere IV–VI was active during a tapping task involving the right upper limb (44). In our study, all participants performed the task with their right hand. This explains our finding with respect to GMV in lobule VI in the right cerebellar hemisphere. In addition, the left cerebellum has been found to be active in spatial cognition task (44). This explains our finding regarding GMV in the Crus 1 in the left hemisphere.

Third, although AI is a composite score summarizing several aspects of behavior during prism adaption, AI primarily reflects a late phase of adaptation learning in which the cerebellar VI and VII may play a critical role. The cerebellum (e.g., lobule VI) constitutes a semi-closed loop with the primary motor area (M1), thereby providing neural circuits for adaptive learning using reaching error as a training signal. This semi-closed loop is expected to feed the efference copy to the cerebellar internal model, which also receives error-related signals from the climbing fibers, and the dentate nucleus projects back to the M1 (9). As the prism adaptation task poses learning of new voluntary visuomotor behavior, it would be necessary for participants to reconstruct cerebellar internal models for reaching (45). A unit-recording study in monkeys indicated that complex spikes from the Purkinje cells in cerebellar hemispheric lobules IV–VI encode hand-reaching error signals (46). An fMRI study using prism adaptation showed that cerebellar hemisphere lobules VIII and IX, as well as the dentate nucleus, were active in the early phase of adaptation (47) while cerebellar lobules IV and VI were active in the late phase of adaptation (41, 47, 48). To calculate AI, we used the 10 last trials in the PRISM session. Thus, our results regarding AI may reflect activity associated with the late phase of adaptation. On the basis of their human fMRI study, Diedrichsen et al. suggested that hand-reaching error signals are encoded in cerebellar lobules V, VI, and VIII, as well as in the dentate nucleus (49). In addition, they found that the cerebellar hemisphere VI and the left cerebellar hemisphere VII (Crus I) were active during the prism adaptation task. Their results are in accordance with the VBM analysis results of the present study.

A complementary analysis (uncorrected P < 0.001 with cluster-level corrected P < 0.05) showed correlation between AI and GMV of the left supramarginal gyrus. In previous fMRI studies, the inferior parietal lobule including the supramarginal gyrus has been shown active in prism adaptation tasks (48, 50, 51). In the present study, the SCD group did not show clear atrophy in the parietal cortex (see right panel of Figure 3B), making it unlikely to interpret this finding as a direct effect of neural degeneration. Alternatively, it is possible that the decrease in AI is influenced by aging since a previous study showed that AI was significantly reduced in healthy controls over 70 years of age (20). Also, GMV of the cerebellum and cerebral cortex (including parietal lobe) decreases in association with aging (52, 53). However, the effect of aging is unlikely explanation because age was used as a covariate in the multiple regression analysis. Although the direct evidence is yet to be available, we speculate that the finding in the parietal cortex could result from remote effects of cerebellar degeneration onto the functional organization of the supramarginal gyrus, through the cortico-cerebellar circuits. To test this hypothesis in the future, we should analyze functional connectivity MRI data (54).

We also analyzed τ_log from the prism adaptation data. Although we found a statistically significant difference between the groups, we did not find a significant correlation between τ_log and cerebellar GMV in the VBM analysis. Researchers have proposed that prism adaptation consists of two phases (55, 56). The early phase encompasses the time required to consciously detect errors immediately after wearing the prism, and the late phase is characterized by recalibration of spatial maps. An event-related fMRI study showed that the posterior parietal cortex was activated during error detection in the early phase (48). Because it is mainly calculated using data from the early phase, τ may not reflect a purely cerebellar function.

Limitations

We pooled all of the participants with SCD in the multiple regression analysis because of the small number of participants with each SCA type. In future studies, it will be necessary to recruit more patients with each SCA type, and then conduct a multiple regression analysis taking the genotype into account. In this study, we investigated the relationship between localized cerebellar regions and motor learning ability (AI) in SCD. Previous resting-state fMRI studies in healthy people have suggested that the cerebellum is functionally connected to most cerebral cortical areas (57). In future research, network analyses using functional and effective connectivity will be important to fully understand the roles of the cerebellum as a network component implicated in prism adaptation problems in patients with SCD. Finally, in order to show that AI is useful as a clinical index, it is necessary to verify its reliability and validity test.

Conclusions

We examined the neural correlates of parameters measured via a prism adaptation task (AI and τ_log) in SCD using VBM. We discovered that AI, a recently proposed composite score to summarize several aspects of motor adaption, was associated with GMV loss in cerebellar sub-sectors and the inferior parietal lobule in patients with SCD. The brain regions found here for the correlates of prism adaptation were consistent with those from the previous studies (3942, 4649, 58). A novel point of our study is that we found a correlation between decreased sensorimotor adaptation and decreased GMV not only in the cerebellum but also in the parietal lobe in SCD patients. This finding should be further tested in the future from the viewpoint of cortico-cerebellar circuits. SCD is a progressive disease, which is difficult to treat at this point. For better management of the SCD patients, the development of biomarkers that can reflect the degree of disease progression is important. We believe that this study will contribute to development of biomarkers for SCD.

Data Availability Statement

Deidentified participant data will be shared, as well as the study protocol and statistical analyses, upon reasonable requests.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Bioethics Committee of National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

KB and THo conceived and designed the study, and acquired the data. KB analyzed the data and drafted the manuscript. KB and THa interpreted the data. KI, YT, and HM critically reviewed the manuscript.

Funding

This study was in part supported by AMED (19ek0109216h0003, 19dm0307004h0002, 19dm0207070s0101, and 19dm0307003h0002), KAKENHI (19H03536 and 26120008).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank the healthy volunteers, patients, and their families for their participation in this study; Mr. Hiroki Togo and Professor Takanori Yokota for study support.

References

1. Tada M, Nishizawa M, Onodera O. Redefining cerebellar ataxia in degenerative ataxias: lessons from recent research on cerebellar systems. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (2015) 86:922–8. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2013-307225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Huang M, Verbeek DS. Why do so many genetic insults lead to Purkinje Cell degeneration and spinocerebellar ataxia? Neurosci Lett. (2019) 688:49–57. 688:49–57. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2018.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Ilg W, Synofzik M, Brötz D, Burkard S, Giese M, Schöls L. Intensive coordinative training improves motor performance in degenerative cerebellar disease. Neurology. (2009) 73:1823–30. 73:1823–30. doi: 10.1212/wnl.0b013e3181c33adf

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Miyai I, Ito M, Hattori N, Mihara M, Hatakenaka M, Yagura H, et al. Cerebellar ataxia rehabilitation trial in degenerative cerebellar diseases. Neurorehab Neural Re. (2012) 26:515–22. 26:515–22. doi: 10.1177/1545968311425918

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Thach W. Does the cerebellum initiate movement? Cerebellum. (2014) 13:139–50. 13:139–50. doi: 10.1007/s12311-013-0506-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. D'Angelo E, Casali S. Seeking a unified framework for cerebellar function and dysfunction: from circuit operations to cognition. Front Neural Circuit. (2013) 6:116. 6:116. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2012.00116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Albus JS. A theory of cerebellar function. Math Biosci. (1971) 10:25–61. 10:25–61. doi: 10.1016/0025-5564(71)90051-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Marr D. A theory of cerebellar cortex. J Physiol. (1969) 202:437–70. 202:437–70. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1969.sp008820

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Wolpert DM, Miall RC, Kawato M. Internal models in the cerebellum. Trends Cogn Sci. (1998) 2:338–47. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(98)01221-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Roth M, Synofzik M, Lindner A. The cerebellum optimizes perceptual predictions about external sensory events. Curr Biol. (2013) 23:930–5. 23:930–5. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.04.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Synofzik M, Lindner A, Thier P. The cerebellum updates predictions about the visual consequences of one's behavior. Curr Biol. (2008) 18:814–8. 18:814–8. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.04.071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Gerwig M, Dimitrova A, Kolb F, Maschke M, Brol B, Kunnel A, et al. Comparison of eyeblink conditioning in patients with superior and posterior inferior cerebellar lesions. Brain. (2003) 126:71–94. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Maschke M, Drepper J, Kindsvater K, Kolb FP, Diener HC, Timmann D. Involvement of the human medial cerebellum in long-term habituation of the acoustic startle response. Exp Brain Res. (2000) 133:359–67. doi: 10.1007/s002210000417

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Nagao S, Honda T, Yamazaki T. Transfer of memory trace of cerebellum-dependent motor learning in human prism adaptation: A model study. Neural Netw. (2013) 47:72–80. 47:72–80. doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2013.01.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Anzai M, Kitazawa H, Nagao S. Effects of reversible pharmacological shutdown of cerebellar flocculus on the memory of long-term horizontal vestibulo-ocular reflex adaptation in monkeys. Neurosci Res. (2010) 68:191–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2010.07.2038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Maschke M, Gomez CM, Ebner TJ, Konczak J. Hereditary cerebellar ataxia progressively impairs force adaptation during goal-directed arm movements. J Neurophysiol. (2004) 91:230–8. 91:230–8. doi: 10.1152/jn.00557.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Diedrichsen J, Verstynen T, Lehman SL, Ivry RB. Cerebellar involvement in anticipating the consequences of self-produced actions during bimanual movements. J Neurophysiol. (2005) 93:801–12. 93:801–12. doi: 10.1152/jn.00662.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Fernandez-Ruiz J, Velásquez-Perez L, Díaz R, Drucker-Colín R, Pérez-González R, Canales N, et al. Prism adaptation in spinocerebellar ataxia type 2. Neuropsychologia. (2007) 45:2692–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.04.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Hanajima R, Shadmehr R, Ohminami S, Tsutsumi R, Shirota Y, Shimizu T, et al. Modulation of error-sensitivity during a prism adaptation task in people with cerebellar degeneration. J Neurophysiol. (2015) 114:2460–71. doi: 10.1152/jn.00145.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Hashimoto Y, Honda T, Matsumura K, Nakao M, Soga K, Katano K, et al. Quantitative evaluation of human cerebellum-dependent motor learning through prism adaptation of hand-reaching movement. PLoS ONE. (2015) 10:e0119376. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0119376

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Ashburner J, Friston KJ. Why Voxel-based morphometry should be used. Neuroimage. (2001) 14:1238–43. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0961

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Hernandez-Castillo CR, Diaz R, Campos-Romo A, Fernandez-Ruiz J. Neural correlates of ataxia severity in spinocerebellar ataxia type 3/Machado-Joseph disease. Cerebellum Ataxias. (2017) 4:7. 4:7. doi: 10.1186/s40673-017-0065-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Goel G, Pal P, Ravishankar S, Venkatasubramanian G, Jayakumar PN, Krishna N, et al. Gray matter volume deficits in spinocerebellar ataxia: an optimized voxel based morphometric study. Parkinsonism Relat D. (2011) 17:521–7. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2011.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Lukas C, Schöls L, Bellenberg B, Rüb U, Przuntek H, Schmid G, et al. Dissociation of grey and white matter reduction in spinocerebellar ataxia type 3 and 6: a voxel-based morphometry study. Neurosci Lett. (2006) 408:230–5. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2006.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Brenneis C, Bösch SM, Schocke M, Wenning GK, Poewe W. Atrophy pattern in SCA2 determined by voxel-based morphometry. Neuroreport. (2003) 14:1799–802. 14:1799–802. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200310060-00008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Sato N, Amino T, Kobayashi K, Asakawa S, Ishiguro T, Tsunemi T, et al. Spinocerebellar ataxia type 31 is associated with “inserted” penta-nucleotide repeats containing (TGGAA)n. Am J Hum Genet. (2009) 85:544–57. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2009.09.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Owada K, Ishikawa K, Toru S, Ishida G, Gomyoda M, Tao O, et al. A clinical, genetic, and neuropathologic study in a family with 16q-linked ADCA type III. Neurology. (2005) 65:629–32. 65:629–32. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000173065.75680.e2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Ishikawa K, Watanabe M, Yoshizawa K, Fujita T, Iwamoto H, Yoshizawa T, et al. Clinical, neuropathological, and molecular study in two families with spinocerebellar ataxia type 6 (SCA6). J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (1999) 67:86–9. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.67.1.86

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Manto M, Marmolino D. Cerebellar ataxias. Curr Opin Neurol. (2009) 22:419–29. 22:419–29. doi: 10.1097/wco.0b013e32832b9897

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Brusse E, Maat-Kievit J, Swieten VJ. Diagnosis and management of early- and late-onset cerebellar ataxia. Clin Genet. (2007) 71:12–24. 71:12–24. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-0004.2006.00722.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Schmitz-Hübsch T, du Montcel TS, Baliko L, Berciano J, Boesch S, Depondt C, et al. Scale for the assessment and rating of ataxia. Neurology. (2006) 66:1717–20. 66:1717–20. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000219042.60538.92

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Schulz JB, Borkert J, Wolf S, Schmitz-Hübsch T, Rakowicz M, Mariotti C, et al. Visualization, quantification and correlation of brain atrophy with clinical symptoms in spinocerebellar ataxia types 1, 3 and 6. Neuroimage. (2010) 49:158–68. 49:158–68. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.07.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Fazekas F, Chawluk J, Alavi A, Hurtig H, Zimmerman R. MR signal abnormalities at 1.5 T in Alzheimer's dementia and normal aging. Am J Roentgenol. (1987) 149:351–6. 149:351–6. doi: 10.2214/ajr.149.2.351

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Ashburner J. A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. Neuroimage. (2007) 38:95–113. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Ihaka R, Gentleman RR. A language for data analysis and graphics. J Comput Graph Stat. (1996) 5:299–314. doi: 10.1080/10618600.1996.10474713

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Friston K, Holmes A, Worsley K, Poline JP, Frith C, Frackowiak R. Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: a general linear approach. Hum Brain Mapp. (1994) 2:189–210. doi: 10.1002/hbm.460020402

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Hayasaka S, Phan KL, Liberzon I, Worsley KJ, Nichols TE. Nonstationary cluster-size inference with random field and permutation methods. Neuroimage. (2004) 22:676–87. 22:676–87. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.01.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Sakai H, Yoshida K, Shimizu Y, Morita H, Ikeda S, Matsumoto N. Analysis of an insertion mutation in a cohort of 94 patients with spinocerebellar ataxia type 31 from Nagano, Japan. Neurogenetics. (2010) 11:409–15. doi: 10.1007/s10048-010-0245-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Baizer J, Kralj-Hans I, Glickstein M. Cerebellar lesions and prism adaptation in macaque monkeys. J Neurophysiol. (1999) 81:1960–5. doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.81.4.1960

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Norris SA, Hathaway EN, Taylor JA, Thach TW. Cerebellar inactivation impairs memory of learned prism gaze-reach calibrations. J Neurophysiol. (2011) 105:2248–59. 105:2248–59. doi: 10.1152/jn.01009.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Donchin O, Rabe K, Diedrichsen J, Lally N, Schoch B, Gizewski E, et al. Cerebellar regions involved in adaptation to force field and visuomotor perturbation. J Neurophysiol. (2012) 107:134–47. 107:134–47. doi: 10.1152/jn.00007.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Burciu R, Reinold J, Rabe K, Wondzinski E, Siebler M, Müller O, et al. Structural correlates of motor adaptation deficits in patients with acute focal lesions of the cerebellum. Exp Brain Res. (2014) 232:2847–57. doi: 10.1007/s00221-014-3956-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Mottolese C, Richard N, Harquel S, Szathmari A, Sirigu A, Desmurget M. Mapping motor representations in the human cerebellum. Brain. (2013) 136:330–42. 136:330–42. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws186

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Stoodley CJ, Valera EM, Schmahmann JD. Functional topography of the cerebellum for motor and cognitive tasks: An fMRI study. Neuroimage. (2012) 59:1560–70. 59:1560–70. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Imamizu H, Kuroda T, Miyauchi S, Yoshioka T, Kawato M. Modular organization of internal models of tools in the human cerebellum. Proc National Acad Sci USA. (2003) 100:5461–6. 100:5461–6. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0835746100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Kitazawa S, Kimura T, Yin PB. Cerebellar complex spikes encode both destinations and errors in arm movements. Nature. (1998) 392:33141. doi: 10.1038/33141

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Küper M, Wünnemann M, Thürling M, Stefanescu RM, Maderwald S, Elles HG, et al. Activation of the cerebellar cortex and the dentate nucleus in a prism adaptation fMRI study. Hum Brain Mapp. (2014) 35:1574–86. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22274

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Luauté J, Schwartz S, Rossetti Y, Spiridon M, Rode G, Boisson D, et al. Dynamic Changes in brain activity during prism adaptation. J Neurosci. (2009) 29:169–78. 29:169–78. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.3054-08.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Diedrichsen J, Hashambhoy Y, Rane T, Shadmehr R. Neural correlates of reach errors. J Neurosci. (2005) 25:9919–31. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.1874-05.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Danckert J, Ferber S, Goodale MA. Direct effects of prismatic lenses on visuomotor control: an event-related functional MRI study. Eur J Neurosci. (2008) 28:1696–704. 28:1696–704. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06460.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Crottaz-Herbette S, Fornari E, Clarke S. Prismatic adaptation changes visuospatial representation in the inferior parietal lobule. J Neurosci. (2014) 34:11803–11. 34:11803–11. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.3184-13.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Smith CD, Chebrolu H, Wekstein DR, Schmitt FA, Markesbery WR. Age and gender effects on human brain anatomy: a voxel-based morphometric study in healthy elderly. Neurobiol Aging. (2007) 28:1075–87. 28:1075–87. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2006.05.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Zhang C, Zhu Q, Hua T. Aging of cerebellar Purkinje cells. Cell Tissue Res. (2010) 341:341–7. doi: 10.1007/s00441-010-1016-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Tsujimoto K, Mizuno K, Nishida D, Tahara M, Yamada E, Shindo S, et al. Correlation between changes in functional connectivity in the dorsal attention network and the after-effects induced by prism adaptation in healthy humans: a dataset of resting-state fMRI and pointing trials after using prism adaptation. Data Brief. (2018) 22:583–9. doi: 10.1016/j.dib.2018.12.053

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Redding GM, Rossetti Y, Wallace B. Applications of prism adaptation: a tutorial in theory and method. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2005) 29:431–4. 29:431–4. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Redding GM, Wallace B. Adaptive spatial alignment and strategic perceptual-motor control. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. (1996) 22:379–94. 22:379–94. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.22.2.379

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Buckner RL, Krienen FM, Castellanos A, Diaz JC, Yeo TB. The organization of the human cerebellum estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol. (2011) 106:2322–45. 106:2322–45. doi: 10.1152/jn.00339.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Chapman HL, Eramudugolla R, Gavrilescu M, Strudwick MW, Loftus A, Cunnington R, et al. Neural mechanisms underlying spatial realignment during adaptation to optical wedge prisms. Neuropsychologia. (2010) 48:2595–601. 48:2595–601. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.05.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: voxel-based morphometry, spinocerebellar degeneration, spinocerebellar ataxia, prism adaptation, MRI, volumetric MRI, motor learning

Citation: Bando K, Honda T, Ishikawa K, Takahashi Y, Mizusawa H and Hanakawa T (2019) Impaired Adaptive Motor Learning Is Correlated With Cerebellar Hemispheric Gray Matter Atrophy in Spinocerebellar Ataxia Patients: A Voxel-Based Morphometry Study. Front. Neurol. 10:1183. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2019.01183

Received: 04 August 2019; Accepted: 24 October 2019;
Published: 14 November 2019.

Edited by:

Sirio Cocozza, University of Naples Federico II, Italy

Reviewed by:

Carlos Hernandez-Castillo, University of Western Ontario, Canada
Imis Dogan, RWTH Aachen University, Germany

Copyright © 2019 Bando, Honda, Ishikawa, Takahashi, Mizusawa and Hanakawa. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Takashi Hanakawa, hanakawa@ncnp.go.jp

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.