Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurol., 21 May 2019
Sec. Applied Neuroimaging
This article is part of the Research Topic Neuropsychology Through the MRI Looking Glass View all 13 articles

Impaired Self-Other Distinction and Subcortical Gray-Matter Alterations Characterize Socio-Cognitive Disturbances in Multiple Sclerosis

\nKristína Czekov,Kristína Czekóová1,2Daniel Joel Shaw,*Daniel Joel Shaw1,3*Kristína SaxunovKristína Saxunová4Michal DufekMichal Dufek4Radek Mare
ekRadek Mareček5Ji&#x;í Vaní
ekJiří Vaníček6Milan Brzdil,Milan Brázdil1,4
  • 1Behavioral and Social Neuroscience, Central European Institute of Technology (CEITEC), Masaryk University, Brno, Czechia
  • 2Institute of Psychology, Czech Academy of Sciences, Brno, Czechia
  • 3Department of Psychology, School of Life and Health Sciences, Aston University, Birmingham, United Kingdom
  • 4First Department of Neurology, Faculty of Medicine, Masaryk University and St. Anne's University Hospital, Brno, Czechia
  • 5Multimodal and Functional Neuroimaging, Central European Institute of Technology (CEITEC), Masaryk University, Brno, Czechia
  • 6Department of Imaging Methods, Masaryk University and St. Anne's University Hospital, Brno, Czechia

Introduction: Recent studies of patients with multiple sclerosis (MS) have revealed disturbances in distinct components of social cognition, such as impaired mentalizing and empathy. The present study investigated this socio-cognitive profile in MS patients in more detail, by examining their performance on tasks measuring more fundamental components of social cognition and any associated disruptions to gray-matter volume (GMV).

Methods: We compared 43 patients with relapse-remitting MS with 43 age- and sex-matched healthy controls (HCs) on clinical characteristics (depression, fatigue), cognitive processing speed, and three aspects of low-level social cognition; specifically, imitative tendencies, visual perspective taking, and emotion recognition. Using voxel-based morphometry, we then explored relationships between GMV and these clinical and behavioral measures.

Results: Patients exhibited significantly slower processing speed, poorer perspective taking, and less imitation compared with HCs. These impairments were related to reduced GMV throughout the putamen, thalami, and anterior insula, predominantly in the left hemisphere. Surprisingly, differences between the groups in emotion recognition were not significant.

Conclusion: Less imitation and poorer perspective taking indicate a cognitive self-bias when faced with conflicting self- and other-representations. This suggests that impaired self-other distinction, and an associated subcortical pattern of GM atrophy, might underlie the socio-cognitive disturbances observed in MS.

Introduction

Multiple sclerosis (MS) is a chronic demyelinating disease of the central nervous system. As part of a complex neurological symptomatology, MS patients present frequently with disturbances in various aspects of cognitive functioning; slower processing speed, impaired episodic memory and executive function are among the most affected (1, 2). Another domain in which dysfunction manifests is social cognition (3)—that is, the broad repertoire of cognitive and affective skills that allow us to infer others' mental and emotional states in order to interact with them effectively (4, 5). Patients' quality of life is reduced considerably by disruptions to their social environment, and so the development of effective interventions requires a better understanding of the socio-cognitive deficits that impact negatively on their interpersonal relationships. Since the range of abilities comprising social cognition span various levels of complexity, however, a precise characterization of these impairments is difficult with standard neuropsychological tests (6).

Unlike the long history of research into general cognitive impairments in MS, only recently have studies begun to unveil the nuanced nature of disturbances in social cognitive abilities exhibited by these patients. The findings of these studies often converge to reveal difficulties in lower-level capacities, particularly in the recognition of negatively valenced facial emotions [e.g., sadness, anger, fear; (7)]. In contrast, investigations of other higher-level components of social cognition provide less consistent insights; while some report that patients are impaired in their ability to attribute mental (“mentalizing”) and affective states to others [“empathy”; (8, 9)], other studies have observed disturbances only in cognitive mentalizing (10, 11). Moreover, it remains to be seen whether impairments in these high-level facets of social cognition result from disruptions to more fundamental components.

Although the precise structure of social cognition is yet to be defined, current research suggests a hierarchical organization in which lower-level mechanisms contribute to or provide a necessary prerequisite for higher-level processes (4). In this model, mentalizing is believed to build upon a more fundamental ability to take another's perspective. Consistent with this notion, both perspective taking and mentalizing engage the superior temporal/temporo-parietal cortices [e.g., (12)]. Similarly, emotion recognition is considered a necessary prerequisite of empathy, and both processes are associated frequently with brain responses within the insulae and anterior cingulate cortex (1315). More recently, we have shown that empathic expression is mediated by imitative tendencies, suggesting that a process of emotional simulation might be necessary for empathy (16).

Furthermore, multiple components of social cognition are thought to recruit a common mechanism of self-other distinction (SOD), which enables us to treat independently and distinguish flexibly between cognitive self and other representations (15, 17). Without efficient SOD, we might egocentrically misattribute our own cognitive and affective states onto others. This would be evident particularly when our own self states are incongruent with those of others (18), resulting in poor emotion recognition and empathic awareness, and inappropriate responding in social situations. Since the viewpoints of other individuals often conflict with our own, this mechanism is also necessary when inferring our interaction partners' perspective—adopting another's perspective requires us to detach ourselves from our own representations (19). On the other hand, dysfunction to this low-level cognitive mechanism might result in uncontrolled self-other merging. Humans exhibit an involuntary tendency to mimic one another during social interaction (20), which appears to reflect a common neural coding of self- and other-action [e.g., (21)]. Control of imitative tendencies therefore requires SOD to differentiate between our own and others' actions (17, 22); without this mechanism, we might exhibit hyperimitation [see (17)]. Consistent with the notion of SOD providing a mechanism common to both perspective taking and mimicry, past research has demonstrated that the expression of involuntary imitation is related inversely to perspective-taking performance (23, 24). Moreover, SOD is associated frequently with brain activity in the temporo-parietal cortices (15). Disturbances to SOD might therefore underlie the higher-level socio-cognitive deficits observed in MS.

Traditionally, MS has been characterized in terms of white matter (WM) pathology, but recent research indicates that gray matter (GM) abnormalities can predict dysfunctional social cognition in this patient population (25). GM atrophy within deep nuclei and the limbic system is present in the very early stages of MS (26), and progresses rapidly in all MS phenotypes (27). This is observed in the thalamus, putamen, caudate nucleus, globus pallidus, and amygdala (26, 28). While other brain regions are associated more frequently with social cognition, these subcortical structures do appear to play an important role in socio-cognitive functioning; the limbic and paralimbic system (including amygdala, striatum, temporal pole, and anterior cingulate) have been implicated in representation of self and other mental states, for instance, and the dorsal striatum has been associated with cognitive mentalizing (29). Correspondingly, disturbances in social cognition also comprise the symptomatology of Parkinson's and Huntington's disease—disorders characterized partly by disruptions to cortico-basal ganglia-thalamo-cortical circuits [e.g., (30)]. Focal GM atrophy among these structures might therefore contribute to the deficits in social cognition exhibited in MS. Unfortunately, however, the majority of research in MS has been performed exclusively at the behavioral or self-report level (8, 9, 11, 31), with relatively few studies combining this with neuroimaging data (25, 32, 33).

To achieve a better characterization of the disturbances in social cognition exhibited by MS patients, the present study utilized three experimental tasks designed to measure discrete, low-level socio-cognitive capacities; specifically, in line with the model described above (4) we measured emotion recognition, visual perspective taking, and imitative tendencies. To assess perspective taking, we measured patients' performance on a task that required them to infer other person's viewpoint when it is incongruent with their own. To examine emotion recognition we assessed their ability to infer the emotional state of another person from just their eyes. Finally, to quantify imitative tendencies we measured the degree to which they imitated the actions of another person automatically, even when this interfered with another task. Using GM volume (GMV) as a metric of brain structure, we then applied voxel-based morphometry to examine whether any of these behavioral indices of social cognition were related to patterns of neural atrophy. Given that MS is associated frequently with a pattern of GM atrophy throughout deep subcortical nuclei, and the apparent role of these nuclei in social cognition, we hypothesized that the degree of GM reduction throughout subcortical brain structures would be related positively to disruptions of SOD.

Materials and Methods

Sample

We recruited 43 patients with relapsing-remitting MS consecutively from the Department of Neurology at St. Anne's University Hospital, Czech Republic, and 43 healthy controls (HCs) matched on age, sex, and handedness (for details on demographics, see Table 1). Handedness was assessed with the revised version of the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (34); a laterality quotient (LQ) was calculated as (right–left)/(right + left) × 100. Physical disability was assessed in MS patients with the Expanded Disability Status Scale [EDSS; (35)]. All patients had been diagnosed according to the revised McDonald criteria (36), and had no other neurological or psychiatric diagnoses. Patients reporting mild to moderate depressive symptoms were included in the study, but the extent of symptoms reported by the two groups were not statistically different (see below). The minimum duration in education was 12 years (i.e., completion of secondary education). Importantly, the availability of disease-modifying treatment for MS patients in the Czech Republic is currently limited to those meeting certain criteria. For this reason, only 36 of these asymptomatic patients (84%) were undergoing treatment: interferon beta 1a (n = 9), interferon beta 1b (n = 3), fingolimod (n = 6), glatiramer acetate (n = 6), dimethyl fumarate (n = 6), teriflunomide (n = 3), natalizumab (n = 2), and daklizumab (n = 1). The experiment was approved by the Institutional Review Board of St. Anne's University Hospital, and all individuals provided written informed consent prior to participating.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sample demographics and clinical characteristics.

Procedure

Participants underwent behavioral assessment prior to brain scanning, which took place no longer than seven months afterwards (M = 5.2 months; SD = 1.6). Importantly, no relapses were presented during these examination periods. The test battery was performed in a single session lasting ~1 h, with each assessment administered in the order in which they are described below. Implicit task-performance measures were obtained before explicit and self-report assessments so that the latter could not influence the former.

Cognitive Processing Speed

To screen for possible cognitive impairment, we employed a paper version of the Symbol Digit Modalities Test [SDMT; (37)]. This test has been established as a reliable and valid measure of cognitive processing speed in MS patients, and the best predictor of cognitive dysfunction in this population given the influence of processing speed on other cognitive functions (38, 39).

Imitative Tendencies

To measure imitative tendencies we employed a computerized stimulus–response compatibility procedure (40), whereby participants are required to execute finger-lifting actions in response to a colored dot (imperative stimulus) while observing task-irrelevant finger actions performed by a stimulus hand (Figure 1A). The degree to which participants are faster and more accurate at executing finger movements signaled by the imperative stimulus when they observe simultaneous matching (compatible) compared with opposing (incompatible) movements is referred to as automatic imitation, and is considered an experimental measure of spontaneous mimicry; higher scores represent greater imitative tendencies. Importantly, Genschow et al. (41) report high split-half reliability (0.86) for this compatibility effect.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Examples of experimental stimuli for (A) the Stimulus-response compatibility procedure and (B) the Director Task. (A) A right stimulus hand was presented at a 90° counter-clockwise rotation. In this example, a green dot (imperative stimulus) signals that the participant should lift index finger. Whether the observed finger movement was the same or different to the one signaled by the imperative stimulus defined the condition (Compatible [Com.] or Incompatible [Incom.], respectively). (B) An audible instruction given by the Director asked the participant to move one of the items to a new box (“Move the smallest apple one box down”). On experimental trials (Exp.), the instruction referred to an object that created a discrepancy between the Director's and participants' perspectives, while no such discrepancy existed on control trials; no distractor object was present in Cont. 1, and in Cont. 2 the instruction referred to a different object (“Move the biggest apple one box down”). Written informed consent was obtained from the individual for the publication of this image. For full instructions, see the Supplementary Information.

All trials began with a warning stimulus, comprising a model's pronated right hand with all fingers resting on a flat surface but rotated 90° counter-clockwise from the participants' perspective. At this point, the participant depressed both the left and right directional arrows on a standard keyboard with the index and middle finger of their right hand, respectively. After 800, 1,600, or 2,400 ms, selected randomly, the warning stimulus was replaced with an end-point image of the same stimulus hand performing either an index or middle finger extension. In this end-point image, a red or green dot was presented between the index and middle finger. The color of this dot signaled whether the participant should move their own index or middle finger (e.g., green = index finger, red = middle finger; the color-finger pairing was counterbalanced across participants). In response to the imperative stimulus, the participant lifted the corresponding finger as quickly as possible, thereby releasing a key. The trial then ended with a blank screen lasting 1,000 ms. The task consisted of three trial types: compatible (the same finger action was signaled and observed), incompatible (opposite finger actions were signaled and observed), and baseline (movement was signaled but not observed). Overall, the paradigm comprised 70 trials−30 compatible, 30 incompatible, and 10 baseline trials—with accuracy and response time (RT) measured on each trial. It is important to emphasize that we used a right stimulus hand rotated 90° counter-clockwise. Since participants responded with their right hands, this stimulus isolated imitative- from any confounding spatial-compatibility effects to provide a pure measure of imitative tendencies (24).

Perspective Taking

The Director task was used to assess individuals' ability to differentiate between their own visual perspective and that of another when the two viewpoints conflict [visual perspective taking; e.g., (42)]. Recently we observed that both RTs and accuracy demonstrated excellent split-half reliability in each condition [> 0.96; (16)].

As illustrated in Figure 1B, the stimulus consisted of a grid of shelves forming 16 boxes, with a different object placed in each of eight boxes. On each trial, the participant received a recorded verbal instruction from a female “Director” to move one of the objects to a different box. The Director sat behind the shelves and therefore could not see the contents of five boxes with opaque backs, which were visible only from the participant's (front) perspective. On experimental trials, the instruction referred to an object that created a discrepancy between the Director's and participants' perspectives (e.g., “Move the smallest apple one box down,” when the director could see only the medium-sized apple). To perform the instruction correctly on these experimental trials, the participant had to discount any “distractor” objects not visible to the Director (e.g., they were required to move the medium-sized apple rather than the smallest). In both control conditions, there was no conflicting object to discount: the distractor was replaced with a non-conflicting object in the first control condition (Cont.1), and in the second (Cont.2) the Director's instruction changed so as to render the distractor irrelevant (e.g., “Move the biggest apple one box down”). Each condition comprised 20 trials presented randomly. The audio recordings of instructions were equivalent across all trials [mean 3.26 (SD .22) sec]. Participants responded by clicking with the mouse on the box where the object should be moved. Errors involved selection of the wrong (e.g., distractor) object or wrong location, the latter including omission of left–right switching (moving the target object left one box when they were instructed to move it rightwards, or vice versa). Any potential difference in perspectives was emphasized on practice trials that included a front and rear view of the shelves (see Supplementary Material for instructions given to participants).

Emotion Recognition

A paper version of the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test [RMET; (43)] was employed to measure participants' ability to infer the emotional state of others. Although the task is employed frequently as a measure of affective mentalizing, it is considered by some scholars to measure only the first stage of this process—emotion recognition (44). This task contains 36 images depicting the eye region of actors' emotional facial expressions. Facial expressions represent complex emotional states with positive (e.g., playful, interested), negative (e.g., hostile, suspicious), and neutral valence (e.g., reflective, pensive). Each image is presented sequentially, and participants are required to select one of four labels that best match the expression without a time limit. A sum of correct responses is used as a measure of success.

Depression

All participants completed the Beck Depression Inventory [BDI-II; (45)]. Using 21 items, this self-report instrument assesses cognitive, affective, physiological and motivational symptoms of depression experienced over the preceding 2 weeks. Scores of 20–28 indicate moderate depression. The BDI-II has been found to be a valid and reliable instrument for the evaluation of depressive symptoms in MS (46).

Fatigue

Patients and controls completed the Modified Fatigue Impact Scale [MFIS; (47)], a self-report instrument that assesses the degree to which physical, cognitive, and psychosocial fatigue experienced over the preceding 4 weeks has affected every-day functions.

MRI Acquisition

High-resolution T1-weighted anatomical images were acquired on a 1.5T Siemens Symphony scanner, using a standard 32-channel array head coil and an MPRAGE sequence: 176 sagittal slices (slice thickness = 1.17 mm); TR = 1,700 ms, TE = 3.93 ms, TI = 1,100 ms, flip angle = 15°; in plane matrix size 256 × 256, resampled to 512 × 512, FOV = 246 × 246 mm, in-plane resolution = 0.48 × 0.48 mm.

Statistical Analyses

Behavioral Data

Differences between the groups were assessed using parametric or non-parametric t-tests, depending on the normality of variable distributions. Independent-samples t- and Mann-Whitney U-tests were employed to contrast MS patients with HCs. Since normality was violated for the majority of the variables, associations between them were examined by Spearman correlation coefficients, all of which were entered into a multivariate bootstrapping procedure (1,000 iterations) to obtain 95% confidence intervals (CIs). These intervals provide an estimate of population values for each coefficient, providing an alternative measure of significance; CIs including zero should be considered unreliable.

For calculating measures of imitative tendencies and perspective taking, we employed an approach that we have used previously to investigate relationships between these two components of social cognition (16, 24). The strength of imitative tendencies was expressed as the difference in response time (RT) between the incompatible relative to the compatible condition, and perspective-taking performance was expressed as the difference in RT and accuracy on the experimental relative to the control conditions. Importantly, there was no evidence of a speed-accuracy trade-off for perspective taking in this sample (p = 0.165), so relative measures for RT and accuracy scores were calculated separately. For both measures, responses on the control condition were regressed from those in the corresponding experimental condition(s), resulting in residualized scores that reflect the difference between the conditions: specifically, greater residuals reflect poorer performance (slower RTs and poorer accuracy in the experimental relative to the control conditions. It is important to emphasize that measures of both imitation and perspective taking are relative (RTs on incompatible vs. compatible trials, and experimental vs. control trials, respectively), and should therefore be uninfluenced by any differences in processing speed between MS patients and HCs. The statistical analyses were performed using SPSS 24 software.

Neuroimaging Data

To compare GMV between the brains of HCs and MS patients we analyzed MR images with the optimized VBM pipeline provided in FSL (48). This analysis pipeline produces results that converge closely with those from the Statistical Parametric Mapping platform (49).

First, the anatomical images were brain-extracted and segmented into GM, WM and cerebrospinal fluid using FAST (50), and the resulting GM partial-volume maps were affine-registered to the MNI-152 standard space template using FLIRT (51). The registered GM images from the entire sample (both HCs and MS patients) were then concatenated and averaged, and flipped along the x-axis. By re-averaging each mirror image to the MNI-152 template, a first-pass left-right symmetric, study-specific “affine” GM template was created. This step avoids introducing any bias during the registration process. Second, all native GM images were re-registered non-linearly to the affine template with FNIRT (52), concatenated, averaged, and flipped along the x-axis. Symmetric, study-specific “non-linear” GM templates were then created by averaging both mirror images, and native GM partial-volume maps were registered to their corresponding non-linear template. Importantly, this optimized protocol modulates each registered GM image to compensate for any contraction/enlargement due to the non-linear transformation; specifically, each voxel of each image was multiplied by the Jacobian of the warp field [see (53)]. Since this modulation does not include the affine part of the registration, however, no correction for total intracranial volume is needed (48). The modulated GM images were then smoothed with an isotropic Gaussian kernel with a sigma of 3 mm.

General Linear Modeling (GLM) was then applied to the resampled, smoothed and modulated GM images to assess localized differences between the HC and MS group. Since MS is characterized by localized WM lesions, appearing as hypointensities on T1 images that can result in an overestimation of GMV, we added to these group comparisons a covariate of no interest representing subject-specific values of mean WM calculated from the corresponding partial-volume map. Subsequently, by adding measures from patients' clinical assessment or behavior on each experimental task as covariate regressors in further GLM analyses, we examined whether localized GMV in the MS group was related to clinical characteristics or socio-cognitive performance. Using randomize (37), all resulting statistical maps were thresholded with permutation-based non-parametric inference; 5,000 permutations were performed with threshold-free cluster enhancement (54), and family-wise error (FWE)-corrected for multiple comparisons.

Results

The values below present means (±SD).

Clinical Assessment

The MS patients performed worse than the HCs on the SDMT (56.42 [±9.26] vs. 69.21 [±10.00]; t(84) = 6.154; p < 0.001, d = 1.33), and reported greater fatigue on the MFIS (29.65 [±12.99] vs. 18.72 [±13.59]; U = 518.00; p < 0.001; r = 0.38). Although the MS group also expressed more depression (10.53 [±8.20] vs. 7.35 [±6.06]), this difference was not statistically significant (p = 0.055; r = 0.21; see Figure 2A).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Comparisons between the HC and MS group in (A) clinical assessment and cognitive processing speed, (B) emotion recognition and imitative tendencies, and (C) response time and accuracy in visual perspective taking. *p < 0.05.

Behavioral Performance

Five MS patients and three HCs were excluded from the analyses of perspective-taking performance because they achieved a score of zero in the experimental condition of the Director Task, suggesting a misunderstanding of task instructions. Both imitation and perspective taking differed between the groups: compared with HCs, patients showed significantly less imitation (23.78 [±30.22] vs. 38.97 [±32.89] ms; t(84) = 2.23, p = 0.028; d = 0.48), and both longer response time (0.21 [±1.14] vs. −0.20 [±0.79]; U = 525.00, p = 0.019; r = 0.27) and poorer accuracy in perspective taking (−0.20 [±0.97] vs. 0.20 [±0.99]; U = 649.00, p = 0.017, r = 0.26). Surprisingly, however, emotion recognition was similar in the MS patients and HCs (24.74 [±3.44] vs. 25.81 [±3.57]); U = 731.50, p = 0.094; r = 0.18; See Figures 2B,C).

Correlations between the clinical assessments and socio-cognitive measures revealed significant relationships only between disease duration and accuracy in perspective taking (ρ(36) = −0.36; p = 0.026; CI = [−0.62, −0.06]). No associations emerged with respect to other measures of social cognition (p ≥ 0.086), or self-reported fatigue and depression (p ≥ 0.257; see Table S1). As expected, there were no significant relationships between cognitive processing speed (SDMT scores) and any measure of socio-cognitive performance (p ≥ 0.217, see Table S2).

Neuroanatomy

A whole-brain GLM analysis revealed a diffuse collection of cortical and subcortical regions in which GMV was reduced in MS patients relative to HCs, after accounting for variability in mean WMV (p < 0.01, FWE-corrected): this encompassed right lateral temporal cortex and the amygdala; and the bilateral amygdala, caudate nucleus, pallidum, putamen, thalamus, and hippocampus. We refer to this herein as GMoverall, and these results are presented in Table 2 and Figure 3A. Interestingly, only cognitive processing speed (SDMT scores) was related with GMoverall–higher processing speed was associated with more GMV throughout this pattern of brain regions (ρ(41) = 0.36, p = 0.019; CI = [0.01, 0.63]; see Figure 4A).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Results of voxel-based morphometry analyses, presenting peak voxels of clusters in which gray matter volume was higher in the HC relative to the MS group (HC > MS; pFWE < 0.01), or associated with imitation (IMI), perspective-taking performance (VPT) or clinical characteristics (EDSS; pFWE < 0.05).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Results of voxel-based morphometry (VBM) analyses. (A) Brain regions expressing greater gray-matter volume (GMV) in the HC relative to MS group (p < 0.01, FWE-corrected). (B) Brain regions in which GMV was associated positively with imitation measured on the Stimulus-response compatibility procedure (IMI) and accuracy of visual perspective taking on the Director Task (VPT), and negatively with clinical scores (EDDS; p < 0.05, FW-corrected). VBM results are presented on the Colin template in MNI space, neurological orientation, with values presenting the x, y, or z coordinate of the corresponding slice.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Scatter plots illustrating significant brain-behavior relationships for the MS group. (A) Positive association between cognitive processing speed (SDMT) and the overall pattern of relative GMV decline (GMoverall; left), and negative association between EDSS scores and GMV in the left amygdala (left). (B) Positive associations between accuracy in perspective taking and GMV in the left putamen (left), and imitative tendencies and GMV in the left thalamus (middle) and left insula (right).

When adding clinical measures as covariates in GLM analyses of MS patients, neither disease duration nor SDMT scores showed significant associations with localized GMV. Scores on the EDDS, however, were associated negatively with GMV in the bilateral amygdalae and left caudate nucleus (p < 0.05, FWE-corrected). In terms of performance on the experimental tasks, accuracy in perspective taking was associated positively with GMV in a small ventral aspect of the left putamen (N = 38; p < 0.05, FWE-corrected). To investigate relationships between brain structure and behavior on the tasks expressed as response time, we added SDMT scores as an additional covariate of no interest; although we observed no significant relationships between task performance and cognitive processing speed, this allowed us to identify brain-behavior relationships that were independent of general processing speed. Response times in perspective taking showed no significant associations with GMV, but the degree of imitative tendencies demonstrated on the stimulus–response compatibility procedure was associated positively with GMV in a large portion of the left thalamus and left anterior insula cortex (N = 43; p < 0.05, FWE-corrected). These results are presented in Figure 3B and Table 2, and Figure 4 plots selected significant brain-behavior relationships across the MS sample.

Finally, since our sample varied in achieved education level (see Table 1), we investigated whether this might have influenced the above findings; specifically, we compared both GMV and behavioral performance between the 19 patients with secondary school education and 24 with university degrees. Using the exact same parameters with randomize, no significant differences were observed between these two patient subgroups. The same (null) result was obtained when contrasting cognitive, socio-cognitive, and self-report scores (p ≥ 0.140).

Discussion

This study sought to achieve a better characterization of the disruptions to social cognition observed in MS by investigating lower-level facets of socio-cognitive abilities, and examining the neuroanatomical correlates of any impairments. Our findings indicate that, behaviorally, patients with relapse-remitting MS exhibit less involuntarily imitation toward the actions of others compared with HCs, and find it more difficult to adopt another individual's perspective when it differs from their own. Furthermore, this behavioral pattern is associated with reduced GMV in the patient group within deep brain nuclei, revealing a potential neuroanatomical correlate.

To our knowledge, this is the first neurobehavioral investigation of imitative tendencies and visual perspective taking in a neurological population. On the basis of our own and others' previous research, we interpret the pattern of behavior exhibited by our sample of MS patients to reflect impaired self-other distinction [SOD; (4, 16, 24)]. Efficient perspective-taking performance is shown by individuals who can switch flexibly between altercentric and egocentric viewpoints, and report a balanced attentional focus between the self and others during social interactions (55). In contrast, MS patients appear to fall back on a default cognitive state of self-bias when faced with competing self- and other-representations (56)—they appear to be less able to detach themselves from their own self-perspective in order to infer conflicting viewpoints. Likewise, MS patients are less influenced by others' actions. Interestingly, we observed the same pattern of behavior in a large healthy sample; specifically, poorer perspective taking and reduced imitation was observed in individuals characterized by an inflexible personality profile compared with those exhibiting more flexibility (16). In this light, the disturbances presented by MS patients in high-level socio-cognitive capacities, such as mentalizing, might reflect dysfunction to a more fundamental, low-level cognitive mechanism responsible for distinguishing flexibly between self and other representations.

In contrast to previous research (8, 25, 57), MS patients performed equally as well as HCs on a task measuring emotion recognition (RMET). While this might indicate that emotion recognition is preserved in this sample of MS patients, it may simply highlight an important difference between the three experimental tasks we employed; namely, their differential requirements for fast and flexible switching between self and other representations: While successful performance on the Director Task (DT) and Stimulus-response Compatibility (SRC) procedure necessitates swift SOD, the RMET involves a selection of one of four choices describing the mental states expressed by eyes, placing less demands on SOD flexibility. In a similar vein, differential performance on these tasks could result from their different demands on executive function; both the DT and SRC procedure are essentially response inhibition tasks, whereby successful performance necessitates the speeded selection of relevant and suppression of irrelevant information [e.g., (58)]. This is less true of the RMET. Indeed, we suggested that individual differences in cognitive control might underlie the opposing behaviors we observed previously between flexible and inflexible personality profiles (16). It is particularly noteworthy that we also observed equivalent empathic responses between these two profiles. Since the present study did not perform a thorough assessment of executive functioning in MS patients, we are unable to make further claims about its influence of SOD. However, recent research points to the functional independence of executive dysfunction and impairments to social cognition in the MS population [see (59)].

In line with previous research, our measures of social cognition were not associated with self-reported depression (3, 79), fatigue (11), or cognitive decline [(8, 11); but see (3)]. This suggests that these frequent symptoms of MS do not predict disturbances in social cognition on their own. Additionally, we observed no relationships between physical disability and our measures of socio-cognitive performance. In contrast, poorer accuracy in perspective taking was associated with disease duration.

Turning now to our neuroanatomical findings, the pattern of GMV reduction we have observed in MS patients aligns closely with previous studies; these structural alterations occurred in the thalamus, putamen, caudate nucleus, globus pallidus, and the amygdala, structures in which GM atrophy frequently appears first and progresses rapidly (2628). Furthermore, relationships between gray matter and task performance in a selection of these deep brain structures implicate them in the socio-cognitive impairments exhibited by MS patients; reduced GMV was associated with poorer perspective taking in the left putamen, and reduced automatic imitation in the thalami and the left anterior insula (AI), independent of cognitive processing speed. Although the neuroanatomical basis of socio-cognitive disturbances in MS remains understudied (60), these results do converge with studies in the healthy population: First, brain function within the dorsal striatum is reported during the cognitive aspect of mentalizing (29), when differentiating between social actions performed by the self and others, and when processing social behavior in general (61). Second, recent research indicates that intact functioning of thalamus is critical for dynamic integration of information across various cortical networks (62), including those implicated in socio-cognitive and -affective processes [e.g., mentalizing; see (29)]. Interestingly, this region seems to be integral for behavioral flexibility (63)—a role that may extend to flexible self-other switching. Third, previous research has associated brain function within the AI with imitative tendencies (5), as well as processes of interoception, empathy, and social awareness (64). Although the majority of existing studies have linked AI with affective components of social cognition, the left AI has been reported to be activated by both emotional and cognitive aspects (13).

It is noteworthy that all GM areas associated significantly with social cognition were localized primarily to the left hemisphere. Although this observation is in line with other findings reporting a predominantly left-lateralized network of brain regions associated with other aspects of social cognition (65), research into the lateralization of disturbances exhibited by MS patients remains scarce and inconsistent (25); socio-cognitive processes have been related to GMV throughout both left and right hemisphere (33, 65). Interestingly, previous research has indicated that cognitive flexibility is associated with brain regions in the left hemisphere (66), while self-referential processing is linked primarily with the right (67). As such, it might be that our results reflect a selective deficit of cognitive flexibility and preserved self-processing. This finding, then, presents a new avenue of investigation in MS.

Given the potential for functional neuroimaging as an evaluative tool for the early detection of brain alterations in MS, future studies should explore the brain networks engaged by the two experimental tasks we have used to reveal disruptions in SOD. It is important to acknowledge that we have investigated GM volume in what is primarily a WM disorder. While patterns of GM pathology appear to be associated more heavily with cognitive dysfunction in MS than concomitant WM lesions (68), diffuse WM abnormalities are likely to result in the disconnection of brain networks supporting social cognition (32). Interestingly, Plata-Bello et al. (69) report decreased functional connectivity in the brains of MS patients relative to HCs during action observation/execution. Future research should investigate whether the subcortical pattern of GMV reduction that we have observed in MS is related to a loss of WM integrity.

It is important to acknowledge aspects of our study that might limit the generalization of the present results. First, we examined only one index of cognitive functioning—scores on the SDMT. Although this is considered a gold-standard screening tool in MS, and a reliable predictor of cognitive decline in MS (38, 39), we were unable to explore relationships between social cognition and other, more general cognitive domains. As alluded to earlier, the DT and SRC procedure are believed to require response inhibition (58). A more detailed neuropsychological examination of cognitive performance might provide better insights into the relationship between this aspect of executive function and low-level components of social cognition. Second, the group of MS patients recruited in this study differed significantly in their level of educational attainment from those in the HC group. While this difference did not manifest in either behavioral performance or our metric of brain structure, the results of the present study should be treated with caution until they are replicated in comparisons of more educationally balanced MS patients and healthy controls. Third, behavioral assessment was performed 5 months prior to brain scanning on average. While substantial GM atrophy is unlikely to occur during this time interval in MS patients with cognitive reserve (70), it is possible that new WM lesions could have developed. On a related note, because this study recruited a sample of asymptomatic MS patients, a non-routine MRI protocol was performed that involved the acquisition of only T1-weighted images. Lesions of WM appear as hypointensities on T1 images, resulting potentially in an overestimation of GMV. We have attempted to control for this by adding estimates of total WMV calculated from the same T1 image as a covariate of no interest. This is a crude approach, however, and much more accurate methods are available; with the addition of T2-weighted images, automated WM lesion-detection tools (71) can provide accurate estimates of focal lesion load, allowing for lesion masking that improves volumetric estimation [e.g., (25)]. As such, although the brain-behavior relationships we have observed in the present study converge closely with previous research, our findings require replication in future research that addresses these limitations.

In conclusion, our study revealed a potential low-level cognitive mechanism underlying the socio-cognitive disturbances exhibited by patients with MS; at the behavioral level, the performance of MS patients indicated increased self-bias when faced with conflicting self- and other-representations—while emotion recognition seems to be preserved, they showed poorer perspective taking and less involuntary imitation. These selective behavioral impairments were associated with a pattern of reduced GMV that encompasses deep brain nuclei, pointing toward a neuroanatomical correlate for this socio-cognitive profile. Future research should build on our findings by clarifying the influence of structural alterations in these discrete brain structures on SOD, and how this manifests in other socio-cognitive capacities.

Ethics Statement

The experiment was approved by the Institutional Review Board of St. Anne's University Hospital, and all individuals written informed consent prior to participating.

Author Contributions

KC: experimental design, behavioral data analyses, manuscript preparation. DJS: experimental design, neuroimaging data analyses, manuscript preparation. KS: recruitment and data collection, manuscript preparation. MD: clinical evaluation, manuscript preparation. RM: neuroimaging data analysis. JV: data collection. MB: experimental design, manuscript preparation.

Funding

This study has received funding from Czech Science Foundation (project No. GA15-16738S). We acknowledge the core facility MAFIL, CEITEC MU, supported by the MEYS CR (LM2015062 Czech-BioImaging).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fneur.2019.00525/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Chiaravalloti ND, DeLuca J. Cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Lancet Neurol. (2008) 7:1139–51. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(08)70259-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Sumowski JF, Benedict R, Enzinger C, Filippi M, Geurts JJ, Hamalainen P, et al. Cognition in multiple sclerosis: state of the field and priorities for the future. Neurology. (2018) 90:278–88. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000004977

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Bora E, Özakbaş S, Velakoulis D, Walterfang M. Social cognition in multiple sclerosis: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychol Rev. (2016) 26:160–72. doi: 10.1007/s11065-016-9320-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Happé F, Cook JL, Bird G. The structure of social cognition: in(ter)dependence of sociocognitive processes. Ann Rev Psychol. (2017) 68:243–67. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010416-044046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Frith CD, Frith U. Mechanisms of social cognition. Ann Rev Psychol. (2012) 63:287–313. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100449

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Henry JD, Von Hippel W, Molenberghs P, Lee T, Sachdev PS. Clinical assessment of social cognitive function in neurological disorders. Nat Rev Neurol. (2016) 12:28–39. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2015.229

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Cotter J, Firth J, Enzinger C, Elliott R, Drake RJ. Social cognition in multiple sclerosis A systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurology. (2016) 87:1727–36. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000003236

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Neuhaus M, Bagutti S, Yaldizli Ö, Zwahlen D, Schaub S, Frey B, et al. Characterization of social cognition impairment in multiple sclerosis. Eur J Neurol. (2018) 25:90–6. doi: 10.1111/ene.13457

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Raimo S, Trojano L, Pappacena S, Alaia R, Spitaleri D, Grossi D, et al. Neuropsychological correlates of theory of mind deficits in patients with multiple sclerosis. Neuropsychology. (2017) 31:811–21. doi: 10.1037/neu0000372

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Isernia S, Baglio F, D'Arma A, Groppo E, Marchetti A, Massaro D. Social mind and long-lasting disease: focus on affective and cognitive theory of mind in multiple sclerosis. Front Psychol. (2019) 10:218. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00218

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Roca M, Manes F, Gleichgerrcht E, Ibáñez A, González De Toledo ME, Marenco V, et al. Cognitive but not affective theory of mind deficits in mild relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis. Cogn Behav Neurol. (2014) 27:25–30. doi: 10.1097/WNN.0000000000000017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Schurz M, Aichhorn M, Martin A, Perner J. Common brain areas engaged in false belief reasoning and visual perspective taking: a meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies. Front Hum Neurosci. (2013) 7:1–14. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00712

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Fan Y, Duncan NW, de Greck M, Northoff G. Is there a core neural network in empathy? An fMRI based quantitative meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2011) 35:903–11. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Fusar-Poli P, Placentino A, Carletti F, Landi P, Allen P, Surguladze S, et al. Functional atlas of emotional faces processing: a voxel-based meta-analysis of 105 functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. J Psychiatry Neurosci. (2009) 34:418–32.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

15. Lamm C, Bukowski H, Silani G. From shared to distinct self–other representations in empathy: evidence from neurotypical function and socio-cognitive disorders. Philos Transac R Soc B Biol Sci. (2016) 371:20150083. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0083

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Shaw DJ, Czekóová K, Pennington CR, Qureshi AW, Špiláková B, Salazar M, et al. You ≠ Me: individual differences in the structure of social cognition. Psychol Res. (2018). doi: 10.1007/s00426-018-1107-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Steinbeis N. The role of self–other distinction in understanding others' mental and emotional states: neurocognitive mechanisms in children and adults. Philos Transac R Soc B Biol Sci. (2016) 371:20150074. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0074

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Silani G, Lamm C, Ruff CC, Singer T. Right supramarginal gyrus is crucial to overcome emotional egocentricity bias in social judgments. J Neurosci. (2013) 33:15466–76. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1488-13.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Epley N, Keysar B, Van Boven L, Gilovich T. Perspective taking as egocentric anchoring and adjustment. J Pers Soc Psychol. (2004) 87:327–39. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.87.3.327

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Chartrand TL, Lakin JL. The antecedents and consequences of human behavioral mimicry. Ann Rev Psychol. (2013) 64:285–308. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143754

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Catmur C, Walsh V, Heyes C. Associative sequence learning: the role of experience in the development of imitation and the mirror system. Philos Transac R Soc B Biol Sci. (2009) 364:2369–80. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. de Guzman M, Bird G, Banissy MJ, Catmur C. Self–other control processes in social cognition: from imitation to empathy. Philos Transac R Soc B Biol Sci. (2016) 371:20150079. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0079

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Santiesteban I, Banissy MJ, Catmur C, Bird G. Enhancing social ability by stimulating right temporoparietal junction. Curr Biol. (2012) 22:2274–7. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.10.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Shaw DJ, Czekóová K, Porubanová M. Orthogonal-compatibility effects confound automatic imitation: implications for measuring self–other distinction. Psychol Res. (2017) 81:1152–65. doi: 10.1007/s00426-016-0814-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Batista S, D'Almeida O, Afonso A, Freitas S, Macário C, Sousa L, et al. Impairment of social cognition in multiple sclerosis: amygdala atrophy is the main predictor. Mult Scler. (2017a) 23:1358–66. doi: 10.1177/1352458516680750

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Audoin B, Zaaraoui W, Reuter F, Rico A, Malikova I, Confort-Gouny S, et al. Atrophy mainly affects the limbic system and the deep grey matter at the first stage of multiple sclerosis. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (2010) 81:690–5. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2009.188748

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Eshaghi A, Prados F, Brownlee W, Altmann DR, Tur C, Cardoso MJ. Deep grey matter volume loss drives disability worsening in multiple sclerosis. Ann Neurol. (2017) 83:210–22. doi: 10.1002/ana.25145

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Datta S, Staewen TD, Cofield SS, Cutter GR, Lublin FD, Wolinsky JS, et al. Regional gray matter atrophy in relapsing remitting multiple sclerosis: Baseline analysis of multi-center data. Mult Scler Relat Disord. (2015) 4:124–36. doi: 10.1016/j.msard.2015.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Abu-Akel A, Shamay-Tsoory S. Neuroanatomical and neurochemical bases of theory of mind. Neuropsychologia. (2011) 49:2971–84. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.07.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Poletti M, Enrici I, Adenzato M. Cognitive and affective theory of mind in neurodegenerative diseases: neuropsychological, neuroanatomical and neurochemical levels. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2012) 36:2147–64. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Kraemer M, Herold M, Uekermann J, Kis B, Wiltfang J, Daum I, et al. Theory of mind and empathy in patients at an early stage of relapsing remitting multiple sclerosis. Clin Neurol Neurosurg. (2013) 115:1016–22. doi: 10.1016/j.clineuro.2012.10.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Batista S, Dineen R, Vilisaar J, Hlinka J, Bradshaw C, Morgan P, et al. Disconnection as a mechanism for cognitive dysfunction in multiple sclerosis. Neurology. (2017b) 89:38–45. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000004060

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Chalah MA, Kauv P, Lefaucheur JP, Hodel J, Créange A, Ayache SS. Theory of mind in multiple sclerosis: a neuropsychological and MRI study. Neurosci Lett. (2017) 658:108–13. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2017.08.055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Milenkovic S, Dragovic M. Modification of the Edinburgh Handedness inventory: a replication study. Laterality. (2013) 18:340–8. doi: 10.1080/1357650X.2012.683196

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Kurtzke JF. Rating neurologic impairment in multiple sclerosis: an expanded disability status scale (EDSS). Neurology. (1983) 33:1444. doi: 10.1212/WNL.33.11.1444

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Polman CH, Reingold SC, Banwell B, Clanet M, Cohen JA, Filippi M, et al. Diagnostic criteria for multiple sclerosis: 2010 Revisions to the McDonald criteria. Ann Neurol. (2011) 69:292–302. doi: 10.1002/ana.22366

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Winkler AM, Ridgway GR, Webster MA, Smith SM, Nichols TE. Permutation inference for the general linear model. NeuroImage. (2014) 92:381–97. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.01.060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Benedict RHB, DeLuca J, Phillips G, LaRocca N, Hudson LD, Rudick R, et al. Validity of the Symbol Digit Modalities Test as a cognition performance outcome measure for multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler J. (2017) 23:721–33. doi: 10.1177/1352458517690821

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Langdon D, Amato M, Boringa I, Brochet B, Foley F, Fredrikson S, et al. Recommendations for a Brief International Cognitive Assessment for Multiple Sclerosis (BICAMS). Mult Scler J. (2012) 18:891–8. doi: 10.1177/1352458511431076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Brass M, Bekkering H, Wohlschläger A, Prinz W. Compatibility between observed and executed finger movements: comparing symbolic, spatial, and imitative cues. Brain Cogn. (2000) 44:124–43. doi: 10.1006/brcg.2000.1225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Genschow O, Van Den Bossche S, Cracco E, Bardi L, Rigoni D, Brass M. Mimicry and automatic imitation are not correlated. PLoS ONE. (2017) 12:1–21. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0183784

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Keysar B, Barr DJ, Balin JA, Brauner JS. Taking perspective in conversation: the role of mutual knowledge in comprehension. Psychol Sci. (2000) 11:32–8. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00211

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Hill J, Raste Y, Plumb I. The ' ' Reading the Mind in the Eyes ' ' Test Revised Version : a study with normal adults, and adults with asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J Child Psychol Psychiat Assoc Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2001) 42:241–51. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00715

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Oakley BFM, Brewer R, Bird G, Catmur C. Theory of mind is not theory of emotion. J Abnorm Psychol. (2016) 125:1–25. doi: 10.1037/abn0000182

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK. Beck Depression Inventory-II: Manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation (1996).

Google Scholar

46. Sacco R, Santangelo G, Stamenova S, Bisecco A, Bonavita S, Lavorgna L, et al. Psychometric properties and validity of Beck Depression Inventory II in multiple sclerosis. Eur J Neurol. (2016) 23:744–50. doi: 10.1111/ene.12932

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Kos D, Kerckhofs E, Carrea I, Verza R, Ramos M, Jansa J. Evaluation of the modified fatigue impact scale in four different European countries. Mult Scler J. (2005) 11:76–80. doi: 10.1191/1352458505ms1117oa

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Good CD, Scahill RI, Fox NC, Ashburner J, Friston KJ, Chan D, et al. Automatic differentiation of anatomical patterns in the human brain: validation with studies of degenerative dementias. NeuroImage. (2002) 17:29–46. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1202

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Douaud G, Smith S, Jenkinson M, Behrens T, Johansen-Berg H, Vickers J, et al. Anatomically related grey and white matter abnormalities in adolescent-onset schizophrenia. Brain. (2007) 130:2375–86. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Zhang Y, Brady M, Smith S. Segmentation of brain MR images through a hidden Markov random field model and the expectation-maximization algorithm. IEEE Transac Med Imaging. (2001) 20:45–57. doi: 10.1109/42.906424

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Jenkinson M, Bannister P, Brady M, Smith S. Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. NeuroImage. (2002) 17:825–41. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1132

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Andersson JLR, Jenkinson M, Smith S. Non-linear registration, aka spatial normalisation. In: FMRIB Technial Report TR07JA2. Oxford Centre for Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging of the Brain, Department of Clinical Neurology, Oxford University, Oxford (2007).

Google Scholar

53. Good CD, Johnsrude IS, Ashburner J, Henson RNA, Friston KJ, Frackowiak RSJ. A voxel-based morphometric study of ageing in 465 normal adult human brains. NeuroImage. (2001) 14:21–36. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0786

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Smith SM, Nichols TE. Threshold-free cluster enhancement: addressing problems of smoothing, threshold dependence and localisation in cluster inference. NeuroImage. (2009) 44:83–98. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Bukowski H, Samson D. New insights into the inter-individual variability in perspective taking. Vision. (2017) 1:8. doi: 10.3390/vision1010008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Bird G, Viding E. The self to other model of empathy: Providing a new framework for understanding empathy impairments in psychopathy, autism, and alexithymia. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2014) 47:520–32. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.09.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Banati M, Sandor J, Mike A, Illes E, Bors L, Feldmann A, et al. Social cognition and Theory of Mind in patients with relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis. Eur J Neurol. (2010) 17:426–33. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-1331.2009.02836.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Zhang R, Geng X, Lee TMC. Large-scale functional neural network correlates of response inhibition: an fMRI meta-analysis. Brain Struct Function. (2017) 222:3973–90. doi: 10.1007/s00429-017-1443-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Batista S, Freitas S, Afonso A, Macário C, Sousa L, Cunha L, et al. Theory of mind and executive functions are dissociated in multiple sclerosis. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. (2018) 33:541–51. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acx101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Di Filippo M, Portaccio E, Mancini A, Calabresi P. Multiple sclerosis and cognition: synaptic failure and network dysfunction. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2018) 19:599–609. doi: 10.1038/s41583-018-0053-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Báez-Mendoza R, Schultz W. The role of the striatum in social behavior. Front Neurosci. (2013) 7:233. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00233

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Hwang K, Bertolero MA, Liu WB, D'Esposito M. The human thalamus is an integrative hub for functional brain networks. J Neurosci. (2017) 37:5594–607. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0067-17.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Saalmann YB, Kastner S. The cognitive thalamus. Front Syst Neurosci. (2015) 9:39. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2015.00039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Uddin LQ, Nomi JS, Hébert-Seropian B, Ghaziri J, Boucher O. Structure and function of the human insula. J Clini Neurophysiol. (2017) 34:300–6. doi: 10.1097/WNP.0000000000000377

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Mike A, Strammer E, Aradi M, Orsi G, Perlaki G, Hajnal A, et al. Disconnection mechanism and regional cortical atrophy contribute to impaired processing of facial expressions and theory of mind in multiple sclerosis: a structural MRI study. PLoS ONE. (2013) 8:e82422. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0082422

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Barbey AK, Colom R, Grafman J. Architecture of cognitive flexibility revealed by lesion mapping. NeuroImage. (2013) 82:547–54. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Uddin LQ, Iacoboni M, Lange C, Keenan JP. The self and social cognition: the role of cortical midline structures and mirror neurons. Trends Cogn Sci. (2007) 11:153–7. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Geurts JJG, Calabrese M, Fisher E, Rudick RA. Measurement and clinical effect of grey matter pathology in multiple sclerosis. Lancet Neurol. (2012) 11:1082–92. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70230-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Plata-Bello J, Pérez-Martín Y, Castañón-Pérez A, Modroño C, Fariña H, Hernández-Martín E, et al. The mirror neuron system in relapsing remitting multiple sclerosis patients with low disability. Brain Topogr. (2017) 30:548–59. doi: 10.1007/s10548-017-0558-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Modica CM, Bergsland N, Dwyer MG, Ramasamy DP, Carl E, Zivadinov R, et al. Cognitive reserve moderates the impact of subcortical gray matter atrophy on neuropsychological status in multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler. (2016) 22:36–42. doi: 10.1177/1352458515579443

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Schmidt P, Gaser C, Arsic M, Buck D, Förschler A, Berthele A, et al. An automated tool for detection of FLAIR-hyperintense white-matter lesions in multiple sclerosis. NeuroImage. (2012) 59:3774–83. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: multiple sclerosis, social cognition, self-other distinction, automatic imitation, visual perspective taking, voxel-based morphometry, gray-matter volume

Citation: Czekóová K, Shaw DJ, Saxunová K, Dufek M, Mareček R, Vaníček J and Brázdil M (2019) Impaired Self-Other Distinction and Subcortical Gray-Matter Alterations Characterize Socio-Cognitive Disturbances in Multiple Sclerosis. Front. Neurol. 10:525. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2019.00525

Received: 02 November 2018; Accepted: 02 May 2019;
Published: 21 May 2019.

Edited by:

Ovidiu Lungu, Université de Montréal, Canada

Reviewed by:

Patrizia Pantano, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy
Fiona Kumfor, University of Sydney, Australia
Antonio Gallo, Università degli Studi della Campania Luigi Vanvitelli Caserta, Italy

Copyright © 2019 Czekóová, Shaw, Saxunová, Dufek, Mareček, Vaníček and Brázdil. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Daniel Joel Shaw, d.j.shaw@aston.ac.uk

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.