Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurol., 31 May 2019
Sec. Neuroepidemiology
This article is part of the Research Topic Neuroepidemiology in Resource-Limited Areas View all 17 articles

Trajectories of (Bio)markers During the Development of Cognitive Frailty in the Doetinchem Cohort Study

\nM. Liset Rietman,
M. Liset Rietman1,2*Gerben HulseggeGerben Hulsegge3Astrid C. J. NooyensAstrid C. J. Nooyens1Martijn E. T. Doll,Martijn E. T. Dollé1,4H. Susan J. PicavetH. Susan J. Picavet1Stephan J. L. BakkerStephan J. L. Bakker5Ron T. GansevoortRon T. Gansevoort5Annemieke M. W. SpijkermanAnnemieke M. W. Spijkerman1W. M. Monique Verschuren,W. M. Monique Verschuren1,2
  • 1National Institute for Public Health and the Environment, Bilthoven, Netherlands
  • 2Julius Center for Health Sciences and Primary Care, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht University, Utrecht, Netherlands
  • 3Department of Public and Occupational Health, Amsterdam Public Health Research Institute, Amsterdam UMC, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands
  • 4Department of Molecular Medicine, University of Texas Health Science Center at San Antonio, San Antonio, TX, United States
  • 5Department of Internal Medicine, University Medical Center Groningen and University of Groningen, Groningen, Netherlands

Background: Long-term changes in (bio)markers for cognitive frailty are not well characterized. Therefore, our aim is to explore (bio)marker trajectories in adults who became cognitively frail compared to age- and sex-matched controls who did not become cognitively frail over a 15 year follow-up. We hypothesize that those who become cognitively frail have more unfavorable trajectories of (bio)markers compared to controls.

Methods: The Doetinchem Cohort Study is a longitudinal population-based study that started in 1987–1991 in men and women aged 20–59 years, with follow-up examinations every 5 years. For the current analyses, we used data of 17 potentially relevant (bio)markers (e.g., body mass index (BMI), urea) from rounds 2 to 5 (1993–2012). A global cognitive functioning score (based on memory, speed, and flexibility) was calculated for each round and transformed into education and examination round-adjusted z-scores. The z-score that corresponded to the 10th percentile in round 5 (z-score = −0.77) was applied as cut-off point for incident cognitive frailty in rounds 2–5. In total, 455 incident cognitively frail cases were identified retrospectively and were compared with 910 age- and sex-matched controls. Trajectories up to 15 years before and 10 years after incident cognitive frailty were analyzed using generalized estimating equations with stratification for sex and adjustment for age and, if appropriate, medication use. Results were further adjusted for level of education, depressive symptoms, BMI, and lifestyle factors.

Results: In men, (bio)marker trajectories did not differ as they ran parallel and the difference in levels was not statistically significant between those who became cognitively frail compared to controls. In women, total cholesterol trajectories first increased and thereafter decreased in cognitively frail women and steadily increased in controls, gamma-glutamyltransferase trajectories were more or less stable in cognitively frail women and increased in controls, and urea trajectories increased in cognitively frail women and remained more or less stable in controls. Results were similar after additional adjustment for potential confounders.

Conclusions: Out of the 17 (bio)markers included in this explorative study, differential trajectories for three biomarkers were observed in women. We do not yet consider any of the studied (bio)markers as promising biomarkers for cognitive frailty.

Introduction

Frailty is a state of increased vulnerability to adverse health outcomes when exposed to stressors caused by the cumulative decline in one or more domains of functioning, including the cognitive domain (14). Moderate cognitive decline is part of the normal aging process (5). Some elderly are confronted with accelerated cognitive decline, which could eventually lead to (mild) cognitive impairment or dementia. A (reversible) state of cognitive vulnerability within mild cognitive impairment has been termed “Cognitive frailty” (6). Although the existence of cognitive frailty and its definition are still under debate (7), there seems to be broad agreement that cognitively frail people experience accelerated cognitive decline (i.e., cognitive dysfunction) without having a form of dementia (8). In this study, people were considered to be cognitively frail when their global cognitive functioning was poor, given their level of education.

It is not yet fully understood how cognitive frailty develops and whether it can be detected at an early stage. However, there are indications that processes of inflammation and oxidative stress are involved and that C-reactive protein (CRP) could potentially serve as a biomarker (9). In addition, in previous studies we observed associations between body mass index (BMI), self-reported health, several biomarkers (e.g., β-cryptoxanthin and zeaxanthin), and cognitive frailty (10, 11). Unfavorable changes in these and other (bio)marker levels may precede cognitive frailty. Studying these changes can provide insight into the molecular pathways involved and could point out promising biomarkers for cognitive frailty.

In the Doetinchem Cohort Study (DCS), various markers (e.g., self-reported health, BMI) and biomarkers (e.g., CRP, urea), have been measured over a time span of at least 15 years (12). Out of these (bio)markers, we identified 17 possibly relevant (bio)markers for cognitive frailty. These are mainly cardiometabolic, inflammatory, and oxidative stress markers. These types of markers have been linked to cognitive decline (13) and could therefore possibly serve as biomarkers for cognitive frailty. Since cognitive frailty arises gradually, it is meaningful to study how (bio)markers “behave” in the course of developing cognitive frailty. These insights can be helpful for the development of treatment and prevention. Therefore, our aim is to explore the trajectories of several (bio)markers during the development of cognitive frailty in adults and compare these to the trajectories of age- and sex-matched controls. We hypothesize that those who become cognitively frail have more unfavorable trajectories of (bio)markers compared to controls.

Methods

Cohort

The DCS is a longitudinal population-based cohort study starting in 1987–1991 (round 1) examining 7,769 men and women aged 20–59 years living in Doetinchem, a town in the Netherlands. Adults who participated in the first round were invited for follow-up examinations in 1993–1997 (round 2, n = 6,117, mean age: 46 years), 1998–2002 (round 3, n = 4,918, mean age: 51 years), 2003–2007 (round 4, n = 4,520, mean age: 56 years), and 2008–2012 (round 5, n = 4,018, mean age: 60 years). Response rates were 75% or higher in all rounds. Verschuren et al. (14) and Picavet et al. (12) have described the study design in more detail. All participants gave written informed consent in each round and the study was approved by the Medical Ethics Committee of the University Medical Center Utrecht.

Incident Cognitive Frailty

Cognitive functioning was assessed among participants aged ≥45 years. Trained personnel carried out the cognitive tests according to a standardized protocol. In rounds 2–5, global cognitive functioning was measured with a neuropsychological test battery assessing three domains: memory function, information processing speed and cognitive flexibility. These were tested using the 15 Words Verbal Learning Test (VLT) (immediate and delayed recall) (15), the Stroop Color–Word Test (16), the Word Fluency Test (17), and the Letter Digit Substitution Test (18). Nooyens et al. (19) have described the cognitive tests in more detail. From the separate test scores, one global cognitive functioning score was calculated for each round. Next, the global cognitive functioning scores were transformed into z-scores, based on the mean and standard deviation in round 5, and were adjusted for level of education and for the number of tests performed during follow-up to take a possible learning effect into account. The z-score that corresponded to the 10th percentile in round 5 (z-score = −0.77) was applied as cut-off point for incident cognitive frailty in rounds 2, 3, 4, and 5. This is consistent with the definition used in one of our previous studies, where we also defined people as being cognitively frail when their global cognitive functioning was poor, given their level of education (11). Since the prevalence of frailty naturally increases with age, this was not included in the definition of cognitive frailty. Participants with a score below the cut-off point were considered incident cognitively frail and participants with a score above this value were considered not cognitively frail. As cognitive tests were only performed among participants aged ≥45 years, we defined participants < 45 years as not being cognitively frail. Participants aged ≥45 years without data on cognitive functioning were excluded.

Measurements

Markers

Weight and height (to calculate the BMI), waist circumference, and diastolic and systolic blood pressure were measured according to standard protocols (14). Standardized questionnaires were used to obtain data on self-reported health, depressive symptoms (assessed with the Mental Health Inventory-5 and the Vitality dimension of the 36-Item Short-Form Health Survey) (20, 21), level of education, smoking status, alcohol consumption, physical activity (categorized using the Cambridge Physical Activity Index) (22), use of anti-hypertensive medication, cholesterol-lowering medication, and glucose-lowering medication.

Biomarkers

Total and high-density lipoprotein (HDL) cholesterol were measured with standardized enzymatic methods. In 2013–2014, standardized enzymatic methods were used to retrospectively determine triglycerides, alanine aminotransferase (ALT), gamma-glutamyltransferase (GGT), high sensitivity CRP, albumin, uric acid, cystatin C, and creatinine of rounds 2–5 using blood plasma that had been stored in freezers. Participants with only one measurement of the (bio)markers were excluded and participants who were pregnant in a particular round were excluded for that round only. Details of all measurements are described in the Appendix A (Supplementary Material).

Statistical Analyses

Matching

The time of incident cognitive frailty was the first examination round in which participants scored below the cut-off point of −0.77. From this point in time, we were able to investigate trajectories with a maximum of 15 years before and 10 years after incident cognitive frailty. There were not enough observations to study the trajectories up to 15 years after incident cognitive frailty. Both cognitive frailty and biomarker levels vary strongly by sex and age. Therefore, we used a matching design in which each incident cognitively frail person was matched to two controls based on sex, age (with 5-year age categories), and examination round. We excluded two incident cognitively frail cases, as no suitable controls could be identified.

(Bio)marker Trajectories

Trajectories of (bio)markers for incident cognitively frail cases and controls were analyzed using generalized estimating equations (GEE) with an unstructured correlation structure. The GEE analysis was performed for each (bio)marker (dependent variable) separately and with cognitive frailty (“yes” vs. “no”) as the main determinant. This resulted in separate estimates (i.e., adjusted marginal means) at each point in time for cases and controls. With these estimates, trajectories for the cognitively frail and the controls were constructed. This approach is consistent with the method used by Hulsegge et al. (23).

Analyses were stratified for sex and the model included age (linear and quadratic), examination round (categorical variable with round 5 as reference category), and time (categorical variable). Time consisted of six categories ranging from T−15 to T+10 with T0 as the moment of incident cognitive frailty. Age was centered at 60 years because this was approximately the mean age at round 5, which resulted in (bio)marker levels of someone who was hypothetically 60 years old. Trajectories of systolic and diastolic blood pressure were adjusted for self-reported anti-hypertensive medication, trajectories of total cholesterol, HDL cholesterol and triglycerides were adjusted for self-reported cholesterol-lowering medication, and trajectories of glucose were adjusted for the self-reported use of glucose-lowering medication.

Triglycerides, ALT, GGT, and CRP had a skewed distribution. Therefore, we log-transformed these biomarkers and reported back-transformed geometric means. Differences between trajectories of cognitively frail people and their controls were tested using an overall interaction term between cognitive frailty and time, and a p-value lower than 0.1 was considered statistically significant. This was obtained via the joint tests for GEE, where differences in slopes were calculated based on five interaction terms (frailty*T−15, frailty*T−10, frailty*T−5, frailty*T+5, frailty*T+10) with T0 (i.e., moment of incident cognitive frailty) as reference category.

To summarize, trajectories up to 15 years before and 10 years after incident cognitive frailty were analyzed and compared to controls using GEE with stratification for sex and adjustment for age and, if appropriate, medication use (model 1). We verified whether the results changed after additional adjustment for level of education, depressive symptoms (model 2), BMI, smoking, alcohol intake, and physical activity (model 3). Trajectories of BMI and waist circumference were not adjusted for BMI.

We performed a sensitivity analysis to study the potential impact of loss to follow-up due to mortality on our results. To this end, we excluded participants who died during the follow-up period (1993–2012) along with their matched case and/or control(s) and compared the results to those obtained in the total population. All analyses were performed using SAS 9.4 software (SAS Institute, Cary, North Caroline, USA).

Results

Population Characteristics

After excluding participants ≥45 years without data on cognitive functioning and applying the additional exclusion criteria, 5,139 participants remained for further analyses. Over the course of the study, 6 participants were defined as incident cognitively frail in round 2, 116 participants in round 3, 134 participants in round 4, and 202 participants in round 5. In total, 455 participants became cognitively frail of which 303 (67%) were men and 152 (33%) were women. At incident cognitive frailty (T0), men had an average age of 65.5 (SD 7.6) and women of 66.9 (SD 7.8). Cognitively frail people more often had a low level of education and a slightly higher BMI, waist circumference, and systolic blood pressure than controls (Table 1). In addition, cognitively frail men and women more often reported to have poor or fair health and their medication use was higher compared to controls.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Population characteristics for incident cognitively frail men and women and their controls at T0.

(Bio)marker Trajectories

In our main model (i.e., model 1), we observed no differences in (bio)marker trajectories between incident cognitively frail men and controls as the trajectories ran parallel and the difference in levels was not statistically significant. In women, we observed differences in the shape of the trajectories of total cholesterol (p = 0.067), GGT (p = 0.008), and urea (p = 0.002) between incident cognitively frail women and controls (Figure 1 and Appendix B Table 1 in Supplementary Material). Total cholesterol increased before women became cognitively frail and decreased after incident cognitive frailty (T0), while in controls, total cholesterol levels steadily increased over time. GGT was more or less stable in incident cognitively frail women, while in controls, GGT levels slowly increased from T−10 onwards. Urea increased over time in incident cognitively frail women, while in controls, urea levels remained more or less stable.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Trajectories of (bio)markers for incident cognitively frail men and women and their controls. Trajectories of self-reported health (A), body mass index (B), waist circumference (C), diastolic blood pressure (D), systolic blood pressure (E), total cholesterol (F), HDL cholesterol (G), glucose (H), triglycerides (I), ALT (J), GGT (K), CRP (L), albumin (M), cystatin C (N), creatinine (O), uric acid (P), and urea (Q) of incident cognitively frail people (red lines) and controls (green lines) with 95% confidence intervals stratified by sex and corrected for age and, if appropriate, medication use (model 1), where men and women were hypothetically 60 years old at the time of incident cognitive frailty. A difference (p-value for interaction < 0.1) in (bio)marker trajectory between those with and without incident cognitive frailty are indicated by an asterisk. HDL cholesterol, high-density lipoprotein cholesterol; ALT, alanine aminotransferase; GGT, gamma glutamyltransferase; CRP, C-reactive protein. Geometric means are shown for triglycerides, ALT, GGT, and CRP.

After further adjustment for level of education and depressive symptoms (model 2), we found no differences in (bio)marker trajectories for men. In women, we found differences in the same biomarker trajectories as in model 1 (i.e., total cholesterol, GGT, urea) and additionally observed a difference in trajectories for ALT in women (p = 0.046) (Appendix B Table 1 in Supplementary Material). When further adjusting for BMI and life-style factors (model 3), we still observed no differences in (bio)marker trajectories for men. In women, differences in the trajectories for GGT, urea, and ALT between incident cognitively frail women and controls remained, but the difference in total cholesterol trajectory (p = 0.109) was just above our threshold (p-value for interaction < 0.1).

To explore the potential impact of loss to follow-up due to mortality on our results, we performed a sensitivity analysis using model 1 in which we excluded participants who died (n = 130) during the follow-up period (1993–2012). In men, 61 cognitively frail and 40 controls died. In women, 16 cognitively frail and 13 controls died. Excluding these participants, along with their matched case and/or control(s), resulted in the exclusion of 333 participants in total. After exclusion, in men, the trajectories of total cholesterol (p = 0.061) and BMI (p = 0.082) differed between cognitively frail men and controls. In women, consistent with the observed differences in the total population, the trajectories of total cholesterol (p = 0.066), GGT (p = 0.018), and urea (p = 0.001) differed between cognitively frail women and controls. In addition, the trajectories of ALT (p = 0.057), and albumin (p = 0.057) also became different between cognitively frail women and controls (Appendix B Figure 1 in Supplementary Material).

Discussion

Our aim was to examine whether (bio)marker trajectories differ for those who become cognitively frail compared to those who do not over a follow-up of 15 years. In addition, we hypothesized more unfavorable trajectories for those who became cognitively frail compared to controls. In our main model (model 1), we observed no differences in (bio)marker trajectories between incident cognitively frail men and controls as the trajectories ran parallel and the difference in levels was not statistically significant. In women, we observed differences in the shape of the trajectories of total cholesterol, GGT, and urea between incident cognitively frail women and controls.

Against our expectations, most of the 17 (bio)markers included in this study did not show deviating trajectories between those who became cognitively frail and those who did not. This was, for example, surprising for the inflammation marker CRP, since this biomarker has, longitudinally, been linked to physical frailty (24), cognitive decline (25), and risk of dementia (26, 27), and therefore could serve as a biomarker for cognitive frailty. However, also other epidemiological studies report inconsistent findings regarding longitudinal measures of inflammation markers and cognitive decline (28, 29). In addition, Soysal et al. (30) showed that higher CRP and interleukin-6 (IL-6) levels are cross-sectionally associated to physical frailty, but not longitudinally. Although this study focused on physical frailty, it seems consistent with our null findings regarding cognitive frailty.

We did find a difference in the trajectory for total cholesterol in women. Total cholesterol increased before women became cognitively frail and decreased after incident cognitive frailty (T0), while in controls, total cholesterol levels steadily increased over time. In a previous cross-sectional study in MARK-AGE, we also found lower cholesterol levels among people who were already cognitively frail compared to people without frailty (10). Solomon et al. (31) found that non-demented people with high total cholesterol levels around age 50 had poorer cognition 20 years later. In addition, their total cholesterol levels decreased after age 50. The pattern Solomon et al. (31) describe seems comparable to the trajectory we found in incident cognitively frail women. The decline we observed in total cholesterol levels after becoming cognitively frail could be caused by various factors, one of which is medication use. The analyses were adjusted for self-reported use of cholesterol-lowering medication, but information about medication type, dose and therapy compliance were not collected and could therefore not be included in these analyses. Hence, it is unclear whether the trajectories differ due to the occurrence of cognitive frailty, or whether other effects, like a treatment effect, is underlying this difference.

We also observed different urea and GGT trajectories in incident cognitively frail women compared to controls. Higher urea levels can be caused by disrupted blood flow through the kidneys for example through heart failure (32). In contrast, we found lower GGT levels in incident cognitively frail women, while heart failure would also cause increased, and not decreased, GGT levels (33). Lower GGT levels could be caused, for example, by the use of clofibrate, a lipid-lowering agent controlling high cholesterol and triglyceride levels. When adjusting for the use of cholesterol-lowering medication for GGT, results remained similar. It is suggested that serum GGT within the normal range is an early marker for oxidative stress (34). Oxidative stress has been suggested to be associated with frailty (35) and increased GGT levels in later life (80 years and older) were associated with cognitive decline (36). However, we unexpectedly observed lower instead of higher GGT levels in cognitively frail women compared to controls, indicating that cognitive frail women might have less oxidative stress. Overall, there does not seem to be a reasonable explanation for the course of these trajectories. We cannot exclude the possibility that the trajectories of GGT and urea are chance findings.

Differences in (bio)marker trajectories were observed among women, but not among men. We found that more men had poor cognitive functioning compared to women (i.e., more men were identified as incident cognitively frail than women). On the other hand, differences in cognitive functioning between the incident cognitively frail men and their controls were smaller compared to the differences observed in women. Possibly, the women who we identified as incident cognitively frail were relatively worse off than the incident cognitively frail men, having relatively poorer cognitive function and potentially poorer overall health and therefore we only found differences between trajectories for women.

We studied the potential impact of loss to follow-up due to mortality on our results. This was appropriate since some of the trajectories for women indicated a (rapidly) deteriorating health. For example, decreasing cholesterol levels could be caused by malnutrition and increasing urea levels could be caused by heart failure. As expected, mortality rate was higher among those who became cognitively frail compared to those who did not. However, excluding these participants did not materially change the results. In fact, the differences in trajectories for total cholesterol, GGT, and urea in women remained the same and seemed therefore rather robust.

We considered people as being cognitively frail when their global cognitive functioning was poor, given their level of education. Since the prevalence of frailty naturally increases with age, this was not included in the definition of cognitive frailty. The most important difference between our operationalization of cognitive frailty and the classic operationalization for mild cognitive impairment (MCI) (37) is that our definition of cognitive frailty did not include subjective memory complaints. Also, we did not include self-reported activities of daily living which is part of the MCI definition. Recently, a definition for cognitive frailty was proposed combining physical frailty with MCI (38). Since we have previously observed that it is possible to be cognitively frail without being physically frail (10), we defined cognitive frailty only based on cognitive functioning. In our manuscript, the term “cognitive frailty” represents cognitive dysfunction, independent of other (physical) limitations and is only based on poor cognitive functioning given the level of education. We explicitly adjusted for level of education, since highly educated people can also experience cognitive dysfunction and this would otherwise be masked by their cognitive reserves.

One of the strengths of this study is that both cognitive functioning, using a comprehensive neuropsychological test battery, and multiple (bio)markers were objectively measured in four rounds over a follow-up of 15 years, making this a unique cohort for studying the relation between (bio)markers and cognitive functioning over time. In addition, all biomarkers of rounds 2–5 were measured in a single run, limiting inter-assay variation.

This study has some limitations. We tried to include all relevant confounders in the analyses (model 3) but residual confounding may still be present. However, adjustment for confounders had a marginal effect in the results. Further, cognitive functioning was not assessed in participants younger than 45 years. We defined these participants as not being cognitively frail under the assumption that people become cognitively frail with advancing age, mostly from 60 years onwards. In addition, since this population was still relatively young, it could have been too young to find (bio)markers for cognitive frailty. Moreover, since cognitive frailty can be described as a complex syndrome, multiple factors can influence the development of this syndrome. This makes it challenging to identify biomarkers for cognitive frailty. Further, we were unable to exclude participants with dementia. However, given the age-distribution of the cohort, the prevalence and incidence of dementia would be quite low and is therefore unlikely to have influenced our results. Finally, due to the age- and sex-matched study design, the power was limited to analyze differences at any time point. However, more power would probably not lead to other differences in trajectories, because most trajectories run almost parallel and that aspect is unlikely to change with more power.

In conclusion, out of the 17 (bio)markers included in this explorative study, different trajectories between incident cognitively frail women and their controls were found for three biomarkers. However, the relation between these biomarkers and the development of cognitive frailty is unclear. Future studies are needed to confirm these findings. Given the results of this study, we do not yet consider any of the studied (bio)markers as promising (bio)markers for cognitive frailty.

Ethics Statement

The study was approved by the Medical Ethics Committee of the University Medical Center Utrecht.

Author Contributions

All authors were involved in interpreting the data, drafting, and approving the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the Ministry of Health, Welfare and Sport of the Netherlands, The National Institute for Public Health and the Environment (grant number S132002) and by Biobanking and Biomolecular Resources Research Infrastructure-NL (grant number CP2011-27).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the epidemiologists and fieldworkers of the Municipal Health Service in Doetinchem for their contribution to the data collection for this study.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fneur.2019.00497/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Clegg A, Young J, Iliffe S, Rikkert MO, Rockwood K. Frailty in elderly people. Lancet. (2013) 381:752–62. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)62167-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Fried LP, Tangen CM, Walston J, Newman AB, Hirsch C, Gottdiener J, et al. Frailty in older adultsevidence for a phenotype. J Gerontol Ser A. (2001) 56:M146–57. doi: 10.1093/gerona/56.3.M146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Morley JE, Vellas B, van Kan GA, Anker SD, Bauer JM, Bernabei R, et al. Frailty consensus: a call to action. J Am Med Dir Assoc. (2013) 14:392–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jamda.2013.03.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Rockwood K, Song X, MacKnight C, Bergman H, Hogan DB, McDowell I, et al. A global clinical measure of fitness and frailty in elderly people. CMAJ Can Med Assoc J. (2005) 173:489–95. doi: 10.1503/cmaj.050051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Bishop NA, Lu T, Yankner BA. Neural mechanisms of ageing and cognitive decline. Nature. (2010) 464:529–35. doi: 10.1038/nature08983

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Panza F, D'Introno A, Colacicco AM, Capurso C, Parigi AD, Capurso SA, et al. Cognitive frailty: predementia syndrome and vascular risk factors. Neurobiol Aging. (2006) 27:933–40. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.05.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Woods AJ, Cohen RA, Pahor M. Cognitive frailty: frontiers and challenges. J Nutr Health Aging. (2013) 17:741–3. doi: 10.1007/s12603-013-0398-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Kelaiditi E, Cesari M, Canevelli M, van Kan GA, Ousset PJ, Gillette-Guyonnet S, et al. Cognitive frailty: rational and definition from an (I.A.N.A./I.A.G.G.) international consensus group. J Nutr Health Aging. (2013) 17:726–34. doi: 10.1007/s12603-013-0367-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Panza F, Solfrizzi V, Barulli MR, Santamato A, Seripa D, Pilotto A, et al. Cognitive frailty: a systematic review of epidemiological and neurobiological evidence of an age-related clinical condition. Rejuv Res. (2015) 18:389–412. doi: 10.1089/rej.2014.1637

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Rietman ML, Spijkerman AMW, Wong A, van Steeg H, Burkle A, Moreno-Villanueva M, et al. Antioxidants linked with physical, cognitive and psychological frailty: analysis of candidate biomarkers and markers derived from the MARK-AGE study. Mech Ageing Dev. (2018) 177:135–43. doi: 10.1016/j.mad.2018.04.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Rietman ML, van der AD, van Oostrom SH, Picavet HSJ, Dolle MET, van Steeg H, et al. The association between BMI and different frailty domains: a U-shaped curve? J Nutr Health Aging. (2018) 22:8–15. doi: 10.1007/s12603-016-0854-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Picavet HSJ, Blokstra A, Spijkerman AMW, Verschuren WMM. Cohort profile update: the Doetinchem Cohort Study 1987-2017: lifestyle, health and chronic diseases in a life course and ageing perspective. Int J Epidemiol. (2017) 46:1751–g. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyx103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Yaffe K, Kanaya A, Lindquist K, Simonsick EM, Harris T, Shorr RI, et al. The metabolic syndrome, inflammation, and risk of cognitive decline. JAMA. (2004) 292:2237–42. doi: 10.1001/jama.292.18.2237

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Verschuren WM, Blokstra A, Picavet HS, Smit HA. Cohort profile: the Doetinchem Cohort Study. Int J Epidemiol. (2008) 37:1236–41. doi: 10.1093/ije/dym292

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Van der Elst W, van Boxtel MP, van Breukelen GJ, Jolles J. Rey's verbal learning test: normative data for 1855 healthy participants aged 24-81 years and the influence of age, sex, education, and mode of presentation. J Int Neuropsychol Soc JINS. (2005) 11:290–302. doi: 10.1017/S1355617705050344

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Van der Elst W, Van Boxtel MP, Van Breukelen GJ, Jolles J. The Stroop color-word test: influence of age, sex, and education; and normative data for a large sample across the adult age range. Assessment. (2006) 13:62–79. doi: 10.1177/1073191105283427

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Van der Elst W, Van Boxtel MP, Van Breukelen GJ, Jolles J. The concept shifting test: adult normative data. Psychol Assess. (2006) 18:424–32. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.18.4.424

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. van der Elst W, van Boxtel MP, van Breukelen GJ, Jolles J. The letter digit substitution test: normative data for 1,858 healthy participants aged 24-81 from the Maastricht Aging Study (MAAS): influence of age, education, and sex. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. (2006) 28:998–1009. doi: 10.1080/13803390591004428

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Nooyens AC, Bueno-de-Mesquita HB, van Boxtel MP, van Gelder BM, Verhagen H, Verschuren WM. Fruit and vegetable intake and cognitive decline in middle-aged men and women: the Doetinchem Cohort Study. Br J Nutr. (2011) 106:752–61. doi: 10.1017/S0007114511001024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Van der Zee KL, Sanderman R. Het Meten Van de Algemeng Gezondheidsutestand Met de RAND-36. Een Handleiding [Measuring General Health Status With the RAND-36. Users Manual]. Groningen: Northern Center of Health Care Research (1994).

Google Scholar

21. Ware JE Jr, Sherbourne CD. The MOS 36-item short-form health survey (SF-36). I. Conceptual framework and item selection. Med Care. (1992) 30:473–83. doi: 10.1097/00005650-199206000-00002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Wareham NJ, Jakes RW, Rennie KL, Schuit J, Mitchell J, Hennings S, et al. Validity and repeatability of a simple index derived from the short physical activity questionnaire used in the European Prospective Investigation into Cancer and Nutrition (EPIC) study. Publ Health Nutr. (2003) 6:407–13. doi: 10.1079/PHN2002439

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Hulsegge G, Spijkerman AMW, van der Schouw YT, Bakker SJL, Gansevoort RT, Smit HA, et al. Trajectories of metabolic risk factors and biochemical markers prior to the onset of type 2 diabetes: the population-based longitudinal Doetinchem study. Nutr Diabetes. (2017) 7:e270. doi: 10.1038/nutd.2017.23

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Walker KA, Walston J, Gottesman RF, Kucharska-Newton A, Palta P, Windham BG. Midlife systemic inflammation is associated with frailty in later life: the ARIC study. J Gerontol Ser A. (2019) 74:343–9. doi: 10.1093/gerona/gly045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Yaffe K, Lindquist K, Penninx BW, Simonsick EM, Pahor M, Kritchevsky S, et al. Inflammatory markers and cognition in well-functioning African-American and white elders. Neurology. (2003) 61:76–80. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000073620.42047.D7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Engelhart MJ, Geerlings MI, Meijer J, Kiliaan A, Ruitenberg A, van Swieten JC, et al. Inflammatory proteins in plasma and the risk of dementia: the rotterdam study. Arch Neurol. (2004) 61:668–72. doi: 10.1001/archneur.61.5.668

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Schmidt R, Schmidt H, Curb JD, Masaki K, White LR, Launer LJ. Early inflammation and dementia: a 25-year follow-up of the Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. Ann Neurol. (2002) 52:168–74. doi: 10.1002/ana.10265

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Bettcher BM, Kramer JH. Longitudinal inflammation, cognitive decline, and Alzheimer's disease: a mini-review. Clin Pharmacol Therap. (2014) 96:464–9. doi: 10.1038/clpt.2014.147

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Wichmann MA, Cruickshanks KJ, Carlsson CM, Chappell R, Fischer ME, Klein BE, et al. Long-term systemic inflammation and cognitive impairment in a population-based cohort. J Am Geriatr Soc. (2014) 62:1683–91. doi: 10.1111/jgs.12994

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Soysal P, Stubbs B, Lucato P, Luchini C, Solmi M, Peluso R, et al. Inflammation and frailty in the elderly: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Ageing Res Rev. (2016) 31:1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2016.08.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Solomon A, Kareholt I, Ngandu T, Wolozin B, Macdonald SW, Winblad B, et al. Serum total cholesterol, statins and cognition in non-demented elderly. Neurobiol Aging. (2009) 30:1006–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2007.09.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Gotsman I, Zwas D, Planer D, Admon D, Lotan C, Keren A. The significance of serum urea and renal function in patients with heart failure. Medicine. (2010) 89:197–203. doi: 10.1097/MD.0b013e3181e893ee

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Jiang S, Jiang D, Tao Y. Role of gamma-glutamyltransferase in cardiovascular diseases. Exp Clin Cardiol. (2013) 18:53–6.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

34. Lim JS, Yang JH, Chun BY, Kam S, Jacobs DR Jr, Lee DH. Is serum gamma-glutamyltransferase inversely associated with serum antioxidants as a marker of oxidative stress? Free Rad Biol Med. (2004) 37:1018–23. doi: 10.1016/j.freeradbiomed.2004.06.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Soysal P, Isik AT, Carvalho AF, Fernandes BS, Solmi M, Schofield P, et al. Oxidative stress and frailty: a systematic review and synthesis of the best evidence. Maturitas. (2017) 99:66–72. doi: 10.1016/j.maturitas.2017.01.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Praetorius Bjork M, Johansson B. Gamma-Glutamyltransferase (GGT) as a biomarker of cognitive decline at the end of life: contrasting age and time to death trajectories. Int Psychogeriatr. (2018) 30:981–90. doi: 10.1017/S1041610217002393

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG, Kokmen E. Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Arch Neurol. (1999) 56:303–8. doi: 10.1001/archneur.56.3.303

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Panza F, Lozupone M, Solfrizzi V, Sardone R, Dibello V, Di Lena L, et al. Different cognitive frailty models and health- and cognitive-related outcomes in older age: from epidemiology to prevention. J Alzheimer's Dis JAD. (2018) 62:993–1012. doi: 10.3233/JAD-170963

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cognitive frailty, markers, biomarkers, trajectories, Doetinchem Cohort Study

Citation: Rietman ML, Hulsegge G, Nooyens ACJ, Dollé MET, Picavet HSJ, Bakker SJL, Gansevoort RT, Spijkerman AMW and Verschuren WMM (2019) Trajectories of (Bio)markers During the Development of Cognitive Frailty in the Doetinchem Cohort Study. Front. Neurol. 10:497. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2019.00497

Received: 19 October 2018; Accepted: 24 April 2019;
Published: 31 May 2019.

Edited by:

Ding Ding, Fudan University, China

Reviewed by:

Jorge Matias-Guiu, Complutense University of Madrid, Spain
David Della-Morte, University of Miami, United States
Giancarlo Logroscino, University of Bari Aldo Moro, Italy

Copyright © 2019 Rietman, Hulsegge, Nooyens, Dollé, Picavet, Bakker, Gansevoort, Spijkerman and Verschuren. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: M. Liset Rietman, liset.rietman@rivm.nl

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.