- 1Pró-Reitoria de Graduação, Reitoria da Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais, Belo Horizonte, Brazil
- 2Grupo de Pesquisa em Neurologia Cognitiva e do Comportamento, Departamento de Clínica Médica, Faculdade de Medicina da Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais, Belo Horizonte, Brazil
- 3Departamento Médico do América Futebol Clube, Belo Horizonte, Brazil
- 4Departamento de Análise de Desempenho do Clube Atlético Mineiro, Belo Horizonte, Brazil
- 5Departamento Médico do Clube Atlético Mineiro, Belo Horizonte, Brazil
- 6Faculdade de Ciências Médicas de Minas Gerais, Belo Horizonte, Brazil
Although the scientific community has focused on the effects of concussions in contact sports, the role of subconcussive impacts, as it can occur during soccer heading, has recently gained attention, considering that it may represent an additional mechanism of cumulative brain injury. The aim of this study is to investigate the effects of soccer heading on cognitive functioning in active professional soccer players. Male soccer players (n = 44), from two soccer teams that play in the Brazilian A Series Championship, and non-athletes (n = 47), comparable in age and education, were submitted to cognitive assessment, consisting of computerized and conventional neuropsychological testing (Neupsilin battery). In the computerized cognitive assessment, soccer players performed better than controls on reaction time measures in general motor coordination, executive functioning and memory tests, and on accuracy measures in executive functioning tests. There were no significant differences between groups on the Neupsilin battery. A comparison between two sub-groups of soccer players, based on the self-reported number of headings, did not show significant differences on tests performance. No significant correlations were found between an estimate of exposure to heading during professional soccer career and cognitive performance. Our data demonstrate no evidence of cognitive impairment in soccer players, compared to non-athletes, and no association between heading exposure and performance on neuropsychological tests. Longitudinal investigations, including neuroimaging assessment, will help to clarify whether soccer heading may be associated with brain injury and cognitive dysfunction.
Introduction
Soccer is the most popular sport in the world and is unique, in comparison to other sports, as it is the only sport in which participants purposely use their head to hit the ball. Heading is considered an offensive or defensive move whereby the player's unprotected head is used to deliberately impact the ball and direct it during play. A soccer player can be subjected to an average of 6–12 headings per competitive game, where the ball reaches high velocities (1). Moreover, heading training is common in practice sessions. Although the scientific community has focused on the effects of concussion in contact sports, the possible consequences of subconcussive impacts, as it can occur during heading, have recently gained attention. The term “subconcussive” was proposed to describe the impact to the head that may cause neuronal dysfunction in the absence of concussive symptoms (2). Since repetitive subconcussive impacts are substantial in soccer, their clinical significance as a potential cause of traumatic brain injury and a risk of later development of neurodegenerative disease may represent a public health problem.
Research on the effects of soccer heading on brain structure and function has produced mixed results (3). A small number of studies have investigated the impact of heading on brain structure by using neuroimaging techniques. While some of these studies have found evidence of abnormalities (4–6), others have failed to show any evidence of brain damage in soccer players (7–9). Also, another line of research has specifically investigated biochemical markers of brain injury and their relationship with head impacts in soccer players. However, while some authors have demonstrated associations between heading and biochemical signs of brain damage (10–15), others have not corroborated these findings (16, 17).
Similarly to the research addressing the effects of soccer heading on brain structure, investigations on the effects of subconcussive impacts on brain function has produced unclear results. A recent study demonstrated an association between soccer heading and immediate and transient changes in electrophysiological measures of brain function (18). There has been a growing number of studies focusing on cognitive functioning in soccer players, but their results are also ambiguous, with some suggesting an association between heading practice and cognitive impairment (5, 6, 18–27) and others finding no evidence of association (8, 9, 15, 28–36). However, it is important to emphasize that these studies vary considerably in sample size, age of subjects, soccer practice level, estimation of heading frequency, cognitive assessment methods, presence and composition of control groups, and study design.
Only two cross-sectional studies conducted cognitive assessment of soccer groups composed entirely by active professional soccer players, with contradictory results. An investigation conducted by Matser et al. (20) compared male professional soccer players with a group of elite non-contact sport athletes, by using an extensive neuropsychological test battery, and showed poorer performance on verbal and visual memory, planning, and visuoperceptual processing tasks in the soccer group. Moreover, an increasing number of headings incurred during soccer participation was associated negatively with cognitive functioning. Field position also influenced performance on neuropsychological testing, as forward and defensive players performed significantly poorer than midfield players and goalkeepers on some tasks. On the other hand, a study conducted by Straume-Naesheim et al. (32) examined performance of male professional soccer players on computerized neuropsychological tests, measuring motor function, decision-making, simple, divided and complex attention, working memory, and learning and memory, and showed no association between estimated match or lifetime heading exposure and cognitive performance. Only 1.5% of the players qualified as outliers for one or more subtasks when compared with the normal range.
As described above, the research about the effects of heading on brain has produced intriguing results, but the findings are still inconclusive. It has been estimated that professional soccer players head the ball more than 2,000 times during their careers (28). However, as mentioned, few studies focused on active professional players (7, 8, 11–14, 20, 32), and only one involved computerized cognitive assessment (32). To our knowledge, there is currently no study about the possible deleterious effects of heading on brain functioning involving concomitantly a sample of active professional soccer players and a combination of computerized and conventional cognitive assessment.
Also, only two studies (20, 32) evaluating active professional players mentioned the educational level of the participants, a variable that can significantly influence cognitive performance (37). Furthermore, it is noteworthy the absence of research about the effects of soccer heading on brain in developing countries, as is the case of Brazil, where the schooling level of the population tends to be lower when compared to developed countries. Given the role of education in improving cognitive reserve (38), it is particularly important to investigate individuals who, besides being subjected to repetitive head impacts in their professional activity, may be more cognitively vulnerable, due to low educational level.
In this cross-sectional study, we aimed to investigate the effects of soccer heading on cognitive functioning, by comparing the performance of active professional soccer players and control subjects on computerized and conventional neuropsychological tests.
Materials and Methods
Participants
The study sample consisted of two groups of male participants: 44 active professional soccer players (mean age = 24.6 ± 4.5 years; mean educational level = 10.5 ± 1.7 years), from two soccer teams that play in the Brazilian A Series Championship, based in Belo Horizonte, Brazil—América Futebol Clube and Clube Atlético Mineiro—and 47 non-athletes control subjects (mean age = 25.9 ± 4.2 years; mean educational level = 11.0 ± 1.5 years). The groups were comparable in terms of age [t(89) = −1.38; p = 0.171] and education (U = 846.00; p = 0.112). The group of soccer players consisted of nine forwards, 14 midfielders, 15 defenders, and 6 goalkeepers. We decided not to exclude any playing position, in order to investigate a wide range of exposure to headings among soccer players. All athletes perform ~15 h of training sessions per week and usually play one or, during most of the soccer season, two competitive matches weekly. The control group was composed by non-athletes, who work as doormen or guards and do not practice soccer, or practice it occasionally and at a recreational level. All subjects provided written informed consent to participate in the study, which was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Federal University of Minas Gerais.
Clinical Information
We applied a sociodemographic questionnaire, to characterize each subject, and parts of the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (39), to investigate possible psychiatric disorders. Nine soccer players (20.4%) and four controls (8.5%) reported a lifetime history of concussion, but there was no difference in rate of concussed subjects between groups [ = 2.64; p = 0.104]. None of the participants reported using drugs with effects on central nervous system, nor exhibited major depressive episode or alcohol dependence.
Heading Exposure
The self-reported number of headings per game among soccer players ranged from 0 to 22.5 (mean = 7.5 ± 5.8 headings), with median value of 5.75 headings, which allowed us to analyze two sub-groups of players (below and above the median number of headings per game), comparable in terms of age [t(42) = 0.72; p = 0.471], education [t(42) = −0.78; p = 0.437] and rate of concussion [ = 1.25; p = 0.262]. The number of competitive matches played by each athlete during his professional soccer career until 2016, obtained from a sport database, varied from 0 to 517 (mean = 98.8 ± 127.4 competitive matches). The values of these two variables were then multiplied in order to estimate the exposure to heading during professional soccer career of each player. The estimated total number of headings ranged from 0 to 6105 (mean = 601.5 ± 1056.7 headings). Since the self-report on the number of headings per game is not an accurate measure and may not correspond to the actual values, we objectively counted the number of headings for each player in some competitive matches. The number of headings performed by 16 athletes in 42 games was directly measured by an observer. Considering this sub-sample of players, a comparison between the self-reported and the actual number of headings per game revealed no significant difference [t(30) = 0.00; p = 1.000]. Also, a significant correlation was found between the self-reported and the actual values (Pearson correlation coefficient = 0.777; p = 0.000).
Cognitive Assessment
The first part of the cognitive assessment consisted of computerized neuropsychological tests, measuring general motor coordination, memory, attention and executive functioning. All tests were designed on the E-Prime software (40), which has been used for investigations of many cognitive functions [e.g., (41)]. The participants accomplished the tasks with the preferred hand, and the answers were registered on the computer keyboard. In all tests, reaction time was measured in milliseconds (ms) and accuracy was measured as the percentage of correct responses. The tests were preceded by standardized instructions, which included answering as quickly as possible, as well as some practice trials, except in the memory tests. This assessment lasted approximately 35 min (min). Below, we present a brief description of the computerized tests, applied in the same order for all subjects.
Simple Reaction Time Test
In this test, which measures general motor coordination, the subject is asked to press the “1” key in response to the symbol “*,” which is flashed on the center of the screen at varying time intervals, ranging from 0.5 to 2.5 seconds (s). The test contains 40 trials and a single block.
Immediate Memory Test
In this test, which measures immediate verbal memory, 20 words are sequentially presented on the center of the screen for memorization. The list includes two words from each of the following categories—animal, body, clothing, device, food, furniture, nature, object, place, and transport—and each word remains on the screen for 2 s. Subsequently, sets composed by four words, numbered 1–4, are exhibited, with each set containing only one word previously presented in the list. For each set, the subject is asked to respond which word was shown in the list of 20 words, by pressing the “1,” “2,” “3,” or “4” keys. The test contains three blocks of 20 trials, each one presenting the same 20 words for memorization, but in a different display order. Reaction time and accuracy are calculated as the average values from the three blocks.
Attention Test
This test, which measures visual selective attention, consists of the presentation of sets composed by five figures, sequentially displayed, each one containing a target figure on the upper part of the screen and, below it, a group of four figures similar to one another and to the target figure. The subject is required to indicate whether the target figure is or is not present in the group below it, by pressing the “Yes” or “No” keys, respectively. The test contains 60 trials and a single block.
Executive Functioning Tests
These tests assess the three core components of executive functioning—task switching, working memory, and response inhibition, respectively by the Number-letter test, the Two-back test, and the Stroop test.
Number-letter-test
In this test, adapted from Miyake et al. (42), a number-letter pair is presented in one of four quadrants on the screen. The subject is instructed to indicate whether the number is odd or even (2, 4, 6, and 8 for even; 3, 5, 7, and 9 for odd) when the number-letter pair is presented in the top two quadrants, by pressing the “Even” or “Odd” keys, and whether the letter is a consonant or a vowel (G, K, M, and R for consonant; A, E, I, U for vowel) when the number-letter pair is presented in the bottom two quadrants, by pressing the “Consonant” or “Vowel” keys. The number-letter pair is exhibited only in the top two quadrants in the first block, only in the bottom two quadrants in the second block, and randomly around all four quadrants in the third block. Thus, only the third block requires subjects to shift between the two types of categorization operations. The test contains 32 trials in the first two blocks and 64 trials in the third block. Reaction time and accuracy are calculated as the difference between the values from the third block and the average values from the first two blocks, which means the cost of task switching.
Two-back-test
This test, adapted from Zheng et al. (43), consists of the presentation of quickly changing numbers, from 1 through 9, on the center of the screen. The subject is asked to monitor the sequence of numbers, by pressing the “Enter” key whenever a stimulus is the same as the one presented two trials before, which requires working memory. The target numbers account for 25% of the total amount of numbers. The test contains 160 trials and a single block.
Stroop-test
In this test, adapted from Zheng et al. (43), the words “Green,” Blue,” “Red,” and “Yellow” are sequentially displayed on the center of the screen in one of two possible conditions: congruence, when the meaning of the word matches the ink color (e.g., the word “Red” written in red), and incongruence, when the meaning of the word differs from the ink color (e.g., the word “Red” written in blue). Each condition accounts for 50% of the total amount of words. The subject is required to name the color in which each word is written, refraining from reading the word, by pressing the “Green,” Blue,” “Red,” and “Yellow” keys. The test contains 72 trials and a single block. Reaction time and accuracy are calculated as the difference between the values from the incongruent condition and those from the congruent condition, which means the cost of response inhibition.
Delayed Memory Test
In this test, which is applied ~30 min after the immediate memory test and measures delayed verbal memory, 40 words, from which 50% were displayed previously, are sequentially presented on the center of the screen. The subject is instructed to respond whether each word was or was not present in the list of 20 words exhibited in the first memory test, by pressing the “Yes” or “No” keys. The test contains 40 trials and a single block.
The second part of the cognitive assessment, applied immediately after the first part, involved the Brief Neuropsychological Assessment Battery Neupsilin (44), a previously validated instrument for use in the Brazilian population (45), which briefly examines the neuropsychological profile of both clinical and healthy populations. The battery, composed by 32 short tasks, evaluates time and spatial orientation, attention, perception, memory, arithmetic abilities, language, praxis, problem solving, and verbal fluency. Overall, the tasks involve vocal, written or sign responses, and each one receives a specific score, referred as the measured variable. The tasks were preceded by standardized instructions and this assessment lasted ~35 min.
Statistical Analysis
After testing the normality of each variable, utilizing the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test, mean comparisons between groups were performed using Student's t-test for independent samples for variables with normal distribution and the Mann-Whitney U-test for non-normally distributed variables. Correlations between the performance of soccer players in tests and estimate of exposure to heading during professional soccer career were calculated using Pearson's correlation test, controlling for lifetime history of concussion. Categorical variables were compared using the Chi-square test. The significance level was set to 5% (p < 0.05) for all tests. All statistical analyses were performed with SPSS (Version 19.0. IBM Corp).
Results
Data comparing performance of soccer players and control subjects are shown in Tables 1–3. On computerized cognitive assessment (Table 1), soccer players performed better on reaction time measures in the simple reaction time test [t(89) = −2.91; p = 0.004], the immediate memory test [t(89) = −2.01; p = 0.048], the number-letter test [t(89) = −3.35; p = 0.001], the two-back test [t(89) = −2.05; p = 0.043], and the delayed memory test [t(89) = −2.33; p = 0.022]. Moreover, soccer players performed better on accuracy measures in the number-letter test (U = 747.50; p = 0.022) and in the two-back test [t(89) = 3.77; p = 0.000]. The performance in the other variables of the computerized neuropsychological tests was similar between groups.
Table 1. Comparison between soccer players and control subjects on computerized cognitive assessment.
With regard to the Neupsilin battery (Table 2), there were no significant differences between soccer players and control subjects. As presented in Table 3, a comparison involving the rate of subjects with deficit scores, i.e., values below normative cut-off points for age- and education-matched population, which are calculated by subtracting 1.5 standard deviation from the mean values, also did not reveal any difference between groups.
Table 3. Comparison between soccer players and control subjects with regard to the rate of subjects with deficit scores on Neupsilin battery.
A comparison between two sub-groups of soccer players, separated by the median value of the self-reported number of headings per game, did not show significant differences on test results (Tables 4,5).
As shown in Tables 6, 7, no significant correlations were found between estimate of exposure to heading during professional soccer career and cognitive performance.
Table 6. Correlations between performance of soccer players on computerized cognitive assessment and estimate of exposure to heading during professional soccer career.
Table 7. Correlations between performance of soccer players on Neupsilin battery and estimate of exposure to heading during professional soccer career.
Moreover, as presented in Tables 8, 9, cognitive performance was similar between soccer players with and without history of concussion, comparable in terms of age [t(9.63) = 1.36; p = 0.204] and education (U = 122.50; p = 0.282), except in the perception test of Neupsilin battery, with athletes with a lifetime history of concussion showing a slightly lower score (U = 89.00; p = 0.032).
Table 8. Comparison between soccer players with and without history of concussion on computerized cognitive assessment.
Table 9. Comparison between soccer players with and without history of concussion on Neupsilin battery.
Discussion
To our knowledge, this is the first study investigating the effects of soccer heading on cognitive functioning involving concomitantly a sample of active professional soccer players and a combination of computerized and conventional cognitive assessment. Our data demonstrated better performance of soccer players, compared to controls, on computerized neuropsychological tests, namely reaction time measures in general motor coordination, executive functioning and memory tests, and accuracy measures in executive functioning tests. No significant difference between groups emerged on Neupsilin battery.
Interestingly, from the seven variables showing significant differences between groups on computerized cognitive assessment, five variables refer to reaction time and only two refer to accuracy. The advantage of soccer players on reaction time measures, when compared to controls, could be at least partially explained by better general motor coordination, since athletes had shorter reaction times also in the simple reaction time test. There is evidence that participation in sports relates to faster processing speed on measures of simple reaction time (46). The better performance of soccer players on accuracy measures in executive functioning tests, specifically assessing task switching and working memory, may be associated to team sport practice, which demands strategic decision-making in complex and quickly changing contexts. Previous research, although not focusing on the effects of soccer heading on brain function, has demonstrated better executive functioning in soccer players in comparison to general population (47).
It is also noteworthy that while computerized cognitive assessment revealed some differences between soccer players and controls, conventional neuropsychological tests did not show any difference between groups. This is in line with studies investigating the reliability of computerized cognitive assessment, which have suggested that measures of response speed are more sensitive in detecting neuropsychological changes than measures of response accuracy in healthy young adults (48).
Considering the literature investigating the effects of soccer heading on cognition (5, 6, 8, 9, 15, 18–36), as pointed out earlier, only two cross-sectional studies involved cognitive assessment exclusively of active professional soccer players. One study (20) demonstrated an association between soccer heading and cognitive impairment, but the other (32) did not corroborate this finding. However, it is important to emphasize a distinction between these two studies with regard to the control groups. While Matser et al. (20) included elite noncontact sport athletes as controls, Straume-Naesheim et al. (32) compared the performance of soccer players with normative values for the general population. Taking into account the evidence of positive effects of physical exercise practice on cognition (49), a comparison involving a control group composed by non-athletes, as is also the case of our study, could difficult to some extent the detection of possible deficits on cognitive performance of soccer players when compared to controls. Moreover, another relevant observation is that our control group was composed by doormen and guards, professions that do not normally involve high cognitive demand, which may also have influenced the results.
A limitation of our study refers to the composition of the control group, which involved some subjects who practiced soccer, albeit at recreational level. It was not possible to measure the amount of irregular exposure to headings within the control group. However, it is important to emphasize that soccer is an extremely popular sport in Brazil, among all age ranges and socioeconomic conditions. Therefore, it would not be surprising that the control group involved subjects who practiced soccer as a leisure activity, since that is part of the country's culture. We do not believe that such a limitation has significantly affected our results, considering the difference in sport practice level between professional soccer players and non-athletes who play soccer recreationally.
There were no significant differences on test variables between soccer players who reported larger or smaller number of headings per game. Similarly, no significant correlations were found between estimate of exposure to heading during professional soccer career and cognitive performance, in contrast with the study by Matser et al. (20) and in accordance with the results by Straume-Naesheim et al. (32). However, another important distinction between these two studies refers to the estimate of heading exposure. In the first study, the authors estimated the total number of headings in only one season, whereas in the second one, as in our study, the estimate involved the entire professional soccer career. In any case, some considerations must be taken with respect to the estimate of exposure to heading. In most studies, as in our investigation, such an estimate is based on self-reported information provided by soccer players, a method with unknown validity. The self-report on the number of headings may be quite variable and potentially biased, which may explain the high standard deviation of the variable regarding long-term heading exposure. In addition, even assuming a correspondence between the self-reported and the actual values, the number of headings may, in fact, vary across matches and sport seasons, an aspect that was not addressed in our estimate. Moreover, when estimating the exposure to heading during professional soccer career, we considered only the number of competitive matches played by each athlete, excluding the training sessions over time, which may considerably underestimate heading exposure. Also, heading technique may vary among soccer players (5), resulting in situations of greater or lesser risk for head injury, a variable that was not controlled for in our study. Even though few soccer players in our sample reported a lifetime history of concussion, the results showed a relative homogeneity of cognitive performance in the soccer group, since only one test variable showed a significant difference between athletes with and without history of concussion.
Although no evidence of brain function impairment in soccer players has been found, we cannot draw conclusions about the issue involving soccer heading and brain injury from cross-sectional data. Moreover, the selection of soccer players from only two soccer teams may also limit the generalizability of the findings. Further longitudinal studies involving larger samples will be crucial for better understanding of this matter, which remains controversial. The first longitudinal study assessing brain structure and function in active professional soccer players did not show evidence of abnormalities or deficits over a period of 5 years (8), but these results need corroboration. It could be argued that soccer heading does not involve enough force to represent a subconcussive impact and, therefore, does not affect cognitive function. In order to investigate this hypothesis, future research should also combine heading exposure, cognitive and biomechanical data, and their potential relationships. As emphasized by some authors (50), although a spectrum of chronic neurological injuries associated with sports-related concussive and subconcussive brain trauma may occur, a primary concern involves chronic traumatic encephalopathy, which has already been reported in studies involving soccer players (51, 52), as well as other contact sport athletes (53, 54). The prevalence of chronic traumatic encephalopathy is still unknown and in vivo diagnosis is not currently possible, which may enhance the concern about potential consequences of soccer heading.
In summary, our data demonstrated no evidence of cognitive dysfunction in active professional soccer players compared to non-athletes. Conversely, soccer players even outperformed control subjects on some computerized neuropsychological tests. Among soccer players, there were no significant differences between those who reported larger or smaller number of headings per game and no significant correlations between estimate of exposure to heading during professional soccer career and cognitive performance. Longitudinal investigations will help to clarify whether heading may be associated with brain injury and cognitive dysfunction.
Data Availability
The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.
Ethics Statement
This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the Ethics Committee of the Federal University of Minas Gerais, with written informed consent from all subjects. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Federal University of Minas Gerais.
Author Contributions
AR and PC are responsible for the conception and design of the study. AR was responsible for cognitive assessment. MDL, CF, CM, MVL, and RL contributed to data collection. LG counted the number of headings per player in competitive matches. AR and MDL performed the statistical analysis. AR drafted the manuscript. LdS and PC revised it critically. All authors read and approved the submitted version.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
We greatly thank the soccer players and the technical commissions from América Futebol Clube and Clube Atlético Mineiro, Brazil, and all non-athletes who composed the control group for their essential contribution to this study. PC is supported by CNPq, Brazil (bolsa de produtividade em pesquisa).
References
1. Spiotta AM, Bartsch AJ, Benzel EC. Heading in soccer: dangerous play? Neurosurgery. (2012) 70:1–11. doi: 10.1227/NEU.0b013e31823021b2
2. Bailes JE, Petraglia AL, Omalu BI, Nauman E, Talavage T. Role of subconcussion in repetitive mild traumatic brain injury. J Neurosurg. (2013) 119:1235–45. doi: 10.3171/2013.7.JNS121822
3. Rodrigues AC, Lasmar RP, Caramelli P. Effects of soccer heading on brain structure and function. Front Neurol. (2016) 7:38. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2016.00038
4. Sortland O, Tysvaer AT. Brain damage in former association football players: an evaluation by cerebral computed tomography. Neuroradiology. (1989) 31:44–8.
5. Lipton ML, Kim N, Zimmerman ME, Kim M, Stewart WF, Branch CA, et al. Soccer heading is associated with white matter microstructural and cognitive abnormalities. Radiology. (2013) 268:850–7. doi: 10.1148/radiol.13130545
6. Koerte IK, Mayinger M, Muehlmann M, Kaufmann D, Lin AP, Steffinger D, et al. Cortical thinning in former professional soccer players. Brain Imaging Behav. (2016) 10:792–8. doi: 10.1007/s11682-015-9442-0
7. Jordan SE, Green GA, Galanty HL, Mandelbaum BR, Jabour BA. Acute and chronic brain injury in United States National Team soccer players. Am J Sports Med. (1996) 24:205–10. doi: 10.1177/036354659602400216
8. Kemp S, Duff A, Hampson N. The neurological, neuroimaging and neuropsychological effects of playing professional football: results of the UK five-year follow-up study. Brain Inj. (2016) 30:1068–74. doi: 10.3109/02699052.2016.1148776
9. Chrisman SPD, Mac Donald CL, Friedman S, Andre J, Rowhani-Rahbar A, Drescher S, et al. Head impact exposure during a weekend youth soccer tournament. J Child Neurol. (2016) 31:971–8. doi: 10.1177/0883073816634857
10. Mussack T, Dvorak J, Graf-Baumann T, Jochum M. Serum S-100B protein levels in young amateur soccer players after controlled heading and normal exercise. Eur J Med Res. (2003) 8:457–64.
11. Stalnacke BM, Tegner Y, Sojka P. Playing soccer increases serum concentrations of the biochemical markers of brain damage S-100B and neuron-specific enolase in elite players: a pilot study. Brain Inj. (2004) 18:899–909. doi: 10.1080/02699050410001671865
12. Stalnacke BM, Ohlsson A, Tegner Y, Sojka P. Serum concentrations of two biochemical markers of brain tissue damage S-100B and neurone specific enolase are increased in elite female soccer players after a competitive game. Br J Sports Med. (2006) 40:313–6. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.2005.021584
13. Straume-Naesheim TM, Andersen TE, Jochum M, Dvorack J, Bahr R. Minor head trauma in soccer and serum levels of S-100B. Neurosurgery. (2008) 62:1297–306. doi: 10.1227/01.neu.0000333301.34189.3d
14. Bamaç B, Tamer GS, Colak T, Colak E, Seyrek E, Duman C, et al. Effects of repeatedly heading a soccer ball on serum levels of two neurotrophic factor of brain tissue, BDNF and NGF, in professional soccer players. Biol Sport. (2011) 28:177–81. doi: 10.5604/959284
15. Koerte IK, Lin AP, Muehlmann M, Merugumala S, Liao H, Starr T, et al. Altered neurochemistry in former professional soccer players without a history of concussion. J Neurotrauma. (2015) 32:1–7. doi: 10.1089/neu.2014.3715
16. Zetterberg H, Jonsson M, Rasulzada A, Popa C, Styrud E, Hietala MA, et al. No neurochemical evidence for brain injury caused by heading in soccer. Br J Sports Med. (2007) 41:574–7. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.2007.037143
17. Stalnacke BM, Sojka P. Repeatedly heading a soccer ball does not increase serum levels of S-100B, a biochemical marker of brain tissue damage: an experimental study. Biomark Insights. (2008) 3:87–91. doi: 10.4137/BMI.S359
18. Di Virgilio TG, Hunter A, Wilson L, Stewart W, Goodall S, Howatson G, et al. Evidence for acute electrophysiological and cognitive changes following routine soccer heading. EBioMedicine. (2016) 13:66–71. doi: 10.1016/j.ebiom.2016.10.029
19. Tysvaer AT, Lochen EA. Soccer injuries to the brain: a neuropsychologic study of former soccer players. Am J Sports Med. (1991) 19:56–60. doi: 10.1177/036354659101900109
20. Matser JT, Kessels AG, Jordan BD, Lezak MD, Troost J. Chronic traumatic brain injury in professional soccer players. Neurology. (1998) 51:791–6. doi: 10.1212/WNL.51.3.791
21. Downs DS, Abwender D. Neuropsychological impairment in soccer athletes. J Sports Med Phys Fitness. (2002) 42:103–7.
22. Webbe FM, Ochs SR. Recency and frequency of soccer heading interact to decrease neurocognitive performance. Appl Neuropsychol. (2003) 10:31–41. doi: 10.1207/S15324826AN1001_5
23. Rutherford A, Stephens R, Potter D, Fernie G. Neuropsychological impairment as a consequence of football (soccer) play and football heading: preliminary analyses and report on university footballers. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. (2005) 27:299–319. doi: 10.1080/13803390490515504
24. Zhang MR, Red SD, Lin AH, Patel SS, Sereno AB. Evidence of cognitive dysfunction after soccer playing with ball heading using a novel tablet-based approach. PLoS ONE. (2013) 8:e57364. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0057364
25. Koerte IK, Nichols E, Tripodis Y, Schultz V, Lehner S, Igbinoba R, et al. Impaired cognitive performance in youth athletes exposed to repetitive head impacts. J Neurotrauma. (2017) 34:2389–95. doi: 10.1089/neu.2016.4960
26. Levitch CF, Zimmerman ME, Lubin N, Kim N, Lipton RB, Stewart WF, et al. Recent and long-term soccer heading exposure is differentially associated with neuropsychological function in amateur players. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2017) 24:147–55. doi: 10.1017/S1355617717000790
27. Stewart WF, Kim N, Ifrah C, Sliwinski M, Zimmerman ME, Kim M, et al. Heading frequency is more strongly related to cognitive performance than unintentional head impacts in amateur soccer players. Front Neurol. (2018) 9:240. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2018.00240
28. Tysvaer A, Storli O. Association football injuries to the brain: a preliminary report. Br J Sports Med. (1981) 15:163–6. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.15.3.163
29. Putukian M, Echemendia RJ, Mackin S. The acute neuropsychological effects of heading in soccer: a pilot study. Clin J Sport Med. (2000) 10:104–9. doi: 10.1097/00042752-200004000-00004
30. Janda DH, Bir CA, Cheney AL. An evaluation of the cumulative concussive effect of soccer heading in the youth population. Inj Cont Saf Promot. (2002) 9:25–31. doi: 10.1076/icsp.9.1.25.3324
31. Stephens R, Rutherford A, Potter D, Fernie G. Neuropsychological impairment as a consequence of football (soccer) play and football heading: a preliminary analysis and report on school students (13-16 years). Child Neuropsychol. (2005) 11:513–26. doi: 10.1080/092970490959629
32. Straume-Naesheim TM, Andersen TE, Dvorak J, Bahr R. Effects of heading exposure and previous concussions on neuropsychological performance among Norwegian elite footballers. Br J Sports Med. (2005) 39:70–7. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.2005.019646
33. Kaminski TW, Wikstrom AM, Gutierrez GM, Glutting JJ. Purposeful heading during a season does not influence cognitive function or balance in female soccer players. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. (2007) 29:742–51. doi: 10.1080/13825580600976911
34. Kaminski TW, Cousino ES, Glutting JJ. Examining the relationship between purposeful heading in soccer and computerized neuropsychological test performance. Res Q Exerc Sport. (2008) 79:235–44. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2008.10599486
35. Kontos AP, Dolese A, Elbin RJ, Covassin T, Warren BL. Relationship of soccer heading to computerized neurocognitive performance and symptoms among female and male youth soccer players. Brain Inj. (2011) 25:1234–41. doi: 10.3109/02699052.2011.608209
36. Vann Jones SA, Breakey RW, Evans PJ. Heading in football, long-term cognitive decline and dementia: evidence from screening retired professional footballers. Br J Sports Med. (2014) 48:159–61. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2013-092758
37. Shen X, Cox SR, Adams MJ, Howard DM, Lawrie SM, Ritchie SJ, et al. Resting-state connectivity and its association with cognitive performance, educational attainment, and household income in the UK Biobank. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. (2018) 3:878–86. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2018.06.007
38. Baldivia B, Andrade VM, Bueno OFA. Contribution of education, occupation and cognitively stimulating activities to the formation of cognitive reserve. Dement Neuropsychol. (2008) 2:173–82. doi: 10.1590/S1980-57642009DN20300003
39. Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, et al. The Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatr. (1998) 59:22–33.
40. Schneider W, Eschman A, Zuccolotto A. E-Prime User's Guide. Pittsburgh, PA: Psychology Software Tools Inc. (2002).
41. Huang Z, Zhang JX, Yang Z, Dong G, Wu J, Chan AS, et al. Verbal memory retrieval engages visual cortex in musicians. Neuroscience. (2010) 168:179–89. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.03.027
42. Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognit Psychol. (2000) 41:49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734
43. Zheng D, Dong X, Sun H, Xu Y, Ma Y, Wang X. The overall impairment of core executive function components in patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment: a cross-sectional study. BMC Neurol. (2012) 12:138. doi: 10.1186/1471-2377-12-138
44. Fonseca RP, Salles JF, Parente MAMP. Instrumento de Avaliação Neuropsicológica Breve NEUPSILIN. São Paulo: Vetor (2009).
45. Fonseca RP, Salles JF, Parente MAMP. Development and content validity of the Brazilian brief neuropsychological assessment battery neupsilin. Psychol Neurosci. (2008) 1:55–62. doi: 10.3922/j.psns.2008.1.009
46. Chaddock L, Neider MB, Voss MW, Gaspar JG, Kramer AF. Do athletes excel at everyday tasks? Med Sci Sports Exerc. (2011) 43:1920–6. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0b013e318218ca74
47. Vestberg T, Gustafson R, Maurex L, Ingvar M, Petrovic P. Executive functions predict the success of top-soccer players. PLoS ONE. (2012) 7:e34731. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0034731
48. Collie A, Maruff P, Makdissi M, McCrory P, McStephen M, Darby D. CogSport: reliability and correlation with conventional cognitive tests used in postconcussion medical evaluations. Clin J Sport Med. (2003) 13:28–32. doi: 10.1097/00042752-200301000-00006
49. Loprinzi PD, Kane CJ. Exercise and cognitive function: a randomized controlled trial examining acute exercise and free-living physical activity and sedentary effects. Mayo Clin Proc. (2015) 90:450–60. doi: 10.1016/j.mayocp.2014.12.023
50. Montenigro PH, Corp DT, Stein TD, Cantu RC, Stern RA. Chronic traumatic encephalopathy: historical origins and current perspective. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. (2015) 11:309–30. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032814-112814
51. Grinberg LT, Anghinah R, Nascimento CF, Amaro E, Leite RP, Martin Mda G, et al. Chronic traumatic encephalopathy presenting as Alzheimer's dementia in a retired soccer player. J Alzheimers Dis. (2016) 54:169–74. doi: 10.3233/JAD-160312
52. Ling H, Morris HR, Neal JW, Lees AJ, Hardy J, Holton JL, et al. Mixed pathologies including chronic traumatic encephalopathy account for dementia in retired association football (soccer) players. Acta Neuropathol. (2017) 133:337–52. doi: 10.1007/s00401-017-1680-3
53. Omalu BI, Fitzsimmons RP, Hammers J, Bailes J. Chronic traumatic encephalopathy in a professional American wrestler. J Forensic Nurs. (2010) 6:130–6. doi: 10.1111/j.1939-3938.2010.01078.x
Keywords: soccer, heading, injury, brain, cognitive functioning
Citation: Rodrigues AC, Lima MDM, de Souza LC, Furtado C, Marques CE, Gonçalves L, Lima MV, Lasmar RP and Caramelli P (2019) No Evidence of Association Between Soccer Heading and Cognitive Performance in Professional Soccer Players: Cross-Sectional Results. Front. Neurol. 10:209. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2019.00209
Received: 21 October 2018; Accepted: 18 February 2019;
Published: 12 March 2019.
Edited by:
Jack Tsao, University of Tennessee Health Science Center (UTHSC), United StatesReviewed by:
Giuseppe Lazzarino, Università degli Studi di Catania, ItalyEdwin Arthur Shores, Macquarie University, Australia
Copyright © 2019 Rodrigues, Lima, de Souza, Furtado, Marques, Gonçalves, Lima, Lasmar and Caramelli. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Paulo Caramelli, Y2FyYW1lbGxpQHVmbWcuYnI=