- Neurobiology and Genetics, Theodor-Boveri Institute, Biocentre, Am Hubland, University of Würzburg, Würzburg, Germany
The fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster is an established model organism in chronobiology, because genetic manipulation and breeding in the laboratory are easy. The circadian clock neuroanatomy in D. melanogaster is one of the best-known clock networks in insects and basic circadian behavior has been characterized in detail in this insect. Another model in chronobiology is the honey bee Apis mellifera, of which diurnal foraging behavior has been described already in the early twentieth century. A. mellifera hallmarks the research on the interplay between the clock and sociality and complex behaviors like sun compass navigation and time-place-learning. Nevertheless, there are aspects of clock structure and function, like for example the role of the clock in photoperiodism and diapause, which can be only insufficiently investigated in these two models. Unlike high-latitude flies such as Chymomyza costata or D. ezoana, cosmopolitan D. melanogaster flies do not display a photoperiodic diapause. Similarly, A. mellifera bees do not go into “real” diapause, but most solitary bee species exhibit an obligatory diapause. Furthermore, sociality evolved in different Hymenoptera independently, wherefore it might be misleading to study the social clock only in one social insect. Consequently, additional research on non-model insects is required to understand the circadian clock in Diptera and Hymenoptera. In this review, we introduce the two chronobiology model insects D. melanogaster and A. mellifera, compare them with other insects and show their advantages and limitations as general models for insect circadian clocks.
Introduction: The History of Insect Models in Chronobiology
Chronobiology is a field of biology that examines cyclic phenomena in living organisms and their adaptation to solar- and lunar-related rhythms. These cycles are known as biological rhythms and the best known are daily, annual and lunar rhythms. Daily rhythms are controlled by the circadian clock, which has a period of about (circa) a day (dian), but is synchronized to a period of 24 h by the environmental rhythms (= Zeitgeber) on earth. Chronobiologists also say that the circadian clock entrains to the 24 h Zeitgeber. The circadian clock is ubiquitous in living organisms of our planet. Circadian clocks help individual insects and other organisms to anticipate the 24 h environmental cycles and insect populations to synchronize crucial steps in their life (such as eclosion from the pupal case or mating) at the optimal time of the day. In addition, they enable individual insects to measure time, which is important for a time memory. They also provide an internal time reference for insects that orient themselves via a sun compass, which is necessary to compensate for the sun's predictable daily motion. Furthermore, the circadian clock is needed to measure day length and to prepare in time for the coming season (e.g., to reproduce or to hibernate). Since insects live at different latitudes (from the equator to the poles), at different habitats (conditions of the surrounding environment) and have developed different life styles (e.g., solitary or social), several adaptations of the circadian clock evolved that are just beginning to be investigated.
Different insect models help to elucidate various aspects of circadian clock function. Nevertheless, many concepts in chronobiology, like the interplay of daily and annual time keeping in photoperiodism and hibernation (in insects called diapause) or the influence of inter-individual behavior and social insect communities on the clock, are still not well-understood.
The honey bee was one of the first insect models in chronobiology. Reports of daily foraging behavior in the beginning of the twentieth century inspired research on the biological relevance of clock regulated behavior (Kleber, 1935; Galizia et al., 2011). Studies on the role of the clock in complex behaviors like sun-compass orientation of the honey bee followed (Frisch and Lindauer, 1954; Medugorac and Lindauer, 1967; Lehmann et al., 2011; Cheeseman et al., 2012) and are still an intensely studied topic, since many other insects of different orders, for example the monarch butterfly, desert locusts and the desert ant Cataglyphis, use sun- or sky-compass orientation (e.g., Fent and Wehner, 1985; Merlin et al., 2009, 2012; Homberg et al., 2011; Homberg, 2015).
On the search for the location of the circadian clock in the insect brain, first evidence of a circadian pacemaker (= master clock) in the lateral brain was given by surgical removal and transplantation of the optic lobes in cockroaches (Nishiitsutsuji-Uwo and Pittendrigh, 1968; Page, 1982). Later, more specific tissue transplantation studies identified the accessory medulla, a small neuropil in the optic lobe, as the master clock in cockroaches (Reischig and Stengl, 2003). In comparative studies, master clocks in the lateral and/or dorsal brain could be identified in many different species, for example flies, bugs, bees and some moth species (Siwicki et al., 1988; Nässel et al., 1993; Helfrich-Förster et al., 1998; Wise et al., 2002; Závodská et al., 2003; Vafopoulou et al., 2009; Ikeno et al., 2014; Kobelková et al., 2015; Fuchikawa et al., 2017).
With the isolation of the first clock gene mutants in Drosophila melanogaster the molecular basis of the circadian clock was unraveled and subsequently the first functional studies were introduced to insect chronobiology (Konopka and Benzer, 1971). Systematic genetic manipulations of the clock system led to a detailed knowledge about the insect clock in this fly [reviewed in (Hall, 2003)] and therefore the best description of basic concepts of the insect circadian clock so far is found in Drosophila.
Lately, new arising methods for genetic manipulation offer the possibility to study circadian clock components and function in detail also in many other insects. RNA interference has been successfully applied in different insects (Moriyama et al., 2008; Lee et al., 2009; for example: Ikeno et al., 2010; Takekata et al., 2012; Kotwica-Rolinska et al., 2017) and genome editing via CRISPR (Clustered Regularly Interspaced Short Palindromic Repeats)—Cas (CRISPR associated protein) may provide clock gene manipulation suitable for further insects (Kotwica-Rolinska et al., 2019).
The Molecular Clock—the Central Negative Feedback Loop
In 2017, the Nobel Prize in Physiology/Medicine was awarded to Jeffrey Hall, Michael Young, and Michael Rosbash for their work that led to the understanding of the molecular basis of circadian rhythms in D. melanogaster, a work that was pioneered by Konopka and Benzer in the 70ties of the last century by the isolation of the period mutants (Konopka and Benzer, 1971). The period gene (per) was the first clock gene that was ever isolated and it turned out to be highly conserved in the animal kingdom (Table 1). Similarly conserved are the general mechanisms of molecular rhythm generation that involve several other clock genes and proteins that interact in transcriptional/translational feedback loops. Nevertheless, a few features are unique to D. melanogaster, or better to say to higher flies (Brachycera) (Sandrelli et al., 2008; Tomioka and Matsumoto, 2015; Chahad-Ehlers, 2017; Bertolini et al., 2018). For example, the second discovered fly clock gene, timeless1 (tim1 or dtim) has a unique function in the first transcriptional/translational feedback loop of higher flies, where its protein product TIM1 dimerizes with PER (the protein product of the period gene) and the dimer enters the nucleus (Sehgal et al., 1994; Myers et al., 1996; Saez and Young, 1996) (see Figure 1). In other animals, for example the honey bee, tim1 is substituted by a cryptochrome (cry) gene that codes for a specific light-insensitive form of CRY2 also called mammalian type CRY (mCRY) (Yuan et al., 2007). Another cry gene [Drosophila cry (dcry) or insect type cry1 (cry1)] codes for a light-sensitive CRY1 and usually forms no dimers with PER (Emery et al., 1998), although PER-CRY1 interactions have been found in vitro (Rosato et al., 2001; Schlichting et al., 2018). In the fruit fly, CRY1 interacts with TIM1 (insect type TIM1) after it has been activated by light and leads to the degradation of TIM1 in the proteasome (Ceriani et al., 1999; Naidoo, 1999), a feature that makes flies very sensitive to light (see below). TIM1 and CRY1 are also present in for example mosquitoes, aphids, crickets, butter flies and moths (Iwai et al., 2006; Gentile et al., 2009; Cortés et al., 2010; Danbara et al., 2010; Kobelková et al., 2015; Rodriguez-Sanchez et al., 2015; Shaikevich et al., 2016; Barberà et al., 2017). However, in these insects they exist in addition to CRY2 (insect CRY2 or mammalian type CRY) and it is not completely clear, whether and how TIM1 interacts with PER. Tim1 knock-down studies in crickets gave evidence that indeed tim1 seems not essential for the central feedback mechanism in crickets, because the knock-down did not destroy rhythmic behavior in the animals (Danbara et al., 2010). Following knock-down studies revealed that circadian behavior of crickets is maintained when either cry2 or tim are rhythmically expressed and that there appear to exist two interconnected negative feedback loops, in which cry1 and cry2 apart from per and tim1 are important to maintain clock function (Tokuoka et al., 2017) (see Figure 1). Unlike in Drosophila, CRY1 does not act as a blue light photoreceptor and light entrainment in the cricket relies purely on photoreception via the compound eyes (Komada et al., 2015; Kutaragi et al., 2018). In other species that lack cry1 (i.e., beetles and cockroaches), tim1 may play a different role in the circadian rhythm generation (Werckenthin et al., 2012, 2020; Li C-J et al., 2018), which stresses the complexity in evolution of divers sets of clock genes in different insects. Recently, a model which includes two different pacemakers with different clock gene sets was proposed for the cockroach to explain per, cry2, and tim1 function in parallel (Werckenthin et al., 2020) (see Figure 1). The cockroach clock may work with three regulatory loops, because knock-down of neither per, nor tim1 nor cry2 alone was successful to induce arrhythmic behavior in the animals (Werckenthin et al., 2020). Interestingly, Hymenoptera are so far the only insect group that lack both, cry1 and tim1 (only cry2 and tim2 are present; see also section “The relevance of Zeitgebers differs between flies and bees”), and they display a clock gene set that is more similar to the mammalian clock (Rubin et al., 2006; Sandrelli et al., 2008). In the following, we will describe the principal transcriptional/translational feedback loops that lead to circadian oscillations with a focus on Diptera and Hymenoptera (see for more insight: Hardin, 2011; Brown et al., 2012; Özkaya and Rosato, 2012; Hardin and Panda, 2013; Helfrich-Förster, 2017; Top and Young, 2018).
Figure 1. Scheme of four different models for the central negative feedback loop(s). Models based on different clock gene sets have been described for (A) Drosophila and other Brachycera, (B) the cricket Gryllus bimaculatus, (C) the cockroach Rhyoparobia maderae and (D) the honey bee Apis mellifera and other Hymenoptera. Different sets of clock proteins (left side of the schemes) build different combinations of dimers, which regulate the cyclic expression of clock gene mRNAs, which in turn provide the basis of clock protein production. In all four systems, the rhythms are generated by a negative feedback of some clock proteins on their transcriptional activators CLK/CYC, but there are differences in the composition of these clock proteins and their properties. In Brachycera, CRY1 acts as a blue-light receptor and can bind TIM1 (A), causing degradation of TIM1 in a light dependent manner. In contrast, the cricket relies purely on photoreception in the compound eye and CRY1 acts together with different isoforms of CRY2 (CRY2c and CRY2f) as part of a second central negative feedback loop (B). In all models, except for the Brachycera model, cyc was expressed rhythmically and Clk constitutively. Nonetheless, cyc knock-down studies showed, that Clk expression cycles in the absence of CYC, which implies that Clk expression is rhythmically regulated in crickets and rhythms are masked in the natural state (Uryu et al., 2013). It is highly likely that more than four different insect clock models for the central negative feedback loop exist, because in sand flies for example, both Clk and cyc are rhythmically expressed (Meireles-Filho et al., 2006a,b; Meireles-Filho and Kyriacou, 2013).
The clock genes per and tim1/cry2 and their respective protein products participate in a first negative feedback loop, in which the proteins inhibit the transcription of their own genes. This involves two further clock genes, cycle (cyc), and Clock (Clk), and their respective products CYC and CLK. CLK and CYC form heterodimers and bind to E-box regulatory elements in the promoters of per and tim1/cry2, activating their transcription. Consequently, per and tim1/cry2 mRNA levels rise and are translated in the cytoplasm, where their products PER and TIM1/CRY2 are subjected to posttranslational modification, dimerize, and after a while enter the nucleus as a complex. In the nucleus, PER-TIM1 or PER-CRY2 complexes bind to CLK-CYC and repress their transcriptional activity. Doing so, they negatively regulate their own expression with a time delay. This delay is provoked by the posttranslational modifications of PER and TIM1/CRY2 and it is essential for provoking stable circadian oscillations. Subsequent PER and TIM1/CRY2 destabilization and degradation stops the repression on CLK-CYC activity, and a new transcriptional-translational cycle restarts.
The basic negative feedback mechanism is very similar in all animals (although gene sets differ), but again there are unique features in higher flies. While CYC (also called BMAL1 in mammals) is the component that binds to the E-boxes and activates transcription of per and tim1/cry2 in the great majority of animals (including bees), CLK is the relevant transcriptional activator in higher flies (Bae et al., 1998; Chang et al., 2003; Rubin et al., 2006; Yuan et al., 2007; Sandrelli et al., 2008; Tomioka and Matsumoto, 2015; Chahad-Ehlers, 2017) (see Figure 1). Most interestingly, the transcription of the clock factor that possesses the transactivation domain is controlled in a rhythmic manner through a second feedback loop, while the one without transactivation domain is not rhythmically controlled. However, cyclic Clk expression has been observed for example in sandflies and in jewel wasps and crickets under certain conditions, although cyc encodes the transactivation domain (Meireles-Filho and Kyriacou, 2013; Uryu et al., 2013; Dalla Benetta et al., 2019) (see below).
The Molecular Clock—Further Feedback Loops
There is a second feedback loop that leads to a circadian oscillation in the abundance of CLK in flies and of CYC (BMAL1) in the other animals (Cyran et al., 2003; Meireles-Filho et al., 2006a; Rubin et al., 2006; Sandrelli et al., 2008; Bertolini et al., 2018). This second feedback loop is so far best described in D. melanogaster. It involves the clock genes, cycle (cyc), Clock (Clk), vrille (vri), and PAR domain protein 1 (pdp1), and their respective products. Vri and pdp1 carry E-box regulatory elements in their promoters, therefore their expression is also activated by the active CLK-CYC complex. VRI accumulates earlier than PDP1 and it represses the expression of Clk, acting at the level of VP-boxes (Emery and Reppert, 2004) present in its promoter region. PDP1 accumulates later than VRI and finally promotes Clk expression. The synergistic activity of VRI and PDP1 generates circadian transcription of clk. However, different functions of the genes pdp1 and vri of this second feedback loop are still largely undiscovered, because null-mutation studies are limited by the fact that the null-mutants exhibit developmental lethality. Besides their role in development of the fly, pdp1 and vri were proposed to regulate output function of the clock downstream of the central oscillator, because changes in levels of PDP1 and the loss of vri in clock cells (tim- and pdf-expressing cells) caused arrhythmic behavior but did not affect core clock function (Benito et al., 2007; Gunawardhana and Hardin, 2017). The inhibition of the activity of a specific isoform of PDP1 (PDP1ε) and missing vri expression in clock cells furthermore displayed a role in regulation of clock neuron morphology and neuropeptide accumulation (Lim et al., 2007a; Gunawardhana and Hardin, 2017). In contrast, Zheng et al. (2009) confirmed Clk activation by PDP1ε and suggested that pdp1 functions in both, core clock and behavioral output pathways.
As already mentioned, in Hymenoptera, cyc and not Clk appears to be rhythmically controlled, but exact mechanisms remain to be elucidated (Rubin et al., 2006; Ingram et al., 2012; Sadd et al., 2015). For example, in the jewel wasp, Nasonia vitripennis, a rhythmic control of Clk expression was found additionally to that of cyc in one study (Dalla Benetta et al., 2019) and only in cyc in another study (Davies and Tauber, 2016). Since Clk expression was only cycling in a Nasonia strain from Northern regions and only under long day conditions, but cycling was lacking in Southern species, Dalla Benetta et al. (2019) concluded that this may be due to an adaptation mechanism in the clock to photoperiods at higher latitudes. Overall expression levels of Clk and cyc were lower in northern Nasonia species (Dalla Benetta et al., 2019). Interestingly, there are parallels to the cyc knock-down studies in crickets, which also showed cycling in Clk expression when cyc levels are diminished (Uryu et al., 2013). This may point to a general mechanism of the circadian clock that promotes rhythmic Clk expression in case of low CYC levels.
With cry1 and tim1 lacking in the Hymenoptera clock, it may be that another transcription regulator, clockwork orange (cwo), plays a rather important role for core clock function, which is not yet understood (Ingram et al., 2012; Rodriguez-Zas et al., 2012). In Drosophila, cwo participates in a third feedback loop that influences CLK-CYC mediated transcription and regulates the amplitude of circadian oscillations in other clock genes (Kadener et al., 2007; Lim et al., 2007b; Matsumoto et al., 2007; Richier et al., 2008). The CWO protein promotes the PER-dependent removal of CLK-CYC complexes from E-boxes, which may be achieved by a binding competition between CWO and CLK-CYC-PER on E-boxes (Zhou et al., 2016). A CWO protein domain that is highly conserved amongst insects but different in the mammalian CWO orthologs DEC1 and DEC2 indicates that cwo function may be similar in all insect clocks (Ingram et al., 2012).
There may be more clock regulation factors in the fruit fly, which are still unknown. For example, only recently, a function in circadian clock regulation was postulated for the nuclear receptors ecdysone induced protein 75 (E75) and Unfulfilled (UNF), which may be conserved among different animals (Kumar et al., 2014; Jaumouillé et al., 2015).
The great advantage of D. melanogaster as a model in chronobiology is its well-described molecular mechanisms of the circadian clock and these mechanisms start to emerge also in other Diptera (Codd et al., 2007; Gentile et al., 2009; Rund et al., 2011; Meireles-Filho and Kyriacou, 2013; Kyriacou, 2014a; Gesto et al., 2015; Meuti et al., 2015; Kaiser et al., 2016; Bazalova and Dolezel, 2017; Bertolini et al., 2018; Noreen et al., 2018; Rivas et al., 2018). On the other hand, it is not possible to transfer all clock functions to other insect models as we illustrated above. Therefore, studies on various different insect clocks are needed to elucidate the role of the clock in complex behaviors, as we find it in other insects such as Hymenopteran species. In the following section, we will focus on the neuronal network of D. melanogaster and A. mellifera and show basic similarities and differences in these insect clock networks.
The Circadian Clock Network of Fruit Flies and Honey Bees
In D. melanogaster, the central clock is located in dorsal and lateral neurons that express the core clock genes and form an extensive neuropeptidergic network in the brain (Figure 2). In addition, the clock ticks in many glia cells (Zerr et al., 1990). The clock neurons are traditionally divided into seven groups–three dorsal ones (DN1−3) in the dorsal brain, three lateral ones (LNd, l-LNv, and s-LNv) in the anterior lateral brain, and one additional lateral group in the posterior brain that is called LPN [reviewed in Helfrich-Förster et al. (2007a), Hermann-Luibl and Helfrich-Förster (2015), Helfrich-Förster (2017), Schubert et al. (2018)]. The clock neurons have two main projection targets: (1) the accessory medulla (aMe), a small neuropil situated between the central brain and the optic lobes and that had been identified as pacemaker center in many insect species and (2) the dorsal brain that houses the hormonal center (pars intercerebralis (PI) of insects and also has connections to most brain areas. The clock neurons form a well-defined fiber network in these two brain areas, putatively allowing considerable crosstalk between them. The aMe is not only invaded by the clock neurons but also by aminergic, glutaminergic, acetylcholinergic, and Glycin- and GABA-ergic inputs from non-clock neurons [reviewed in Helfrich-Förster (2017), Top and Young (2018)]. This emphasizes the role of the aMe in intercellular communication—both among clock neurons and between extrinsic cells and clock neurons. Furthermore, the aMe appears to get light information from the eyes and from the Hofbauer-Buchner eyelets (H-B eyelets), small extra retinal eyelets that are located beneath the compound eyes and are remnants of the larval stemmata [reviewed in Helfrich-Förster (2020)]. The aMe can be regarded as a hub to channel retinal and extra retinal inputs to the central circadian clock entraining it to the periodic environmental cycles (Li M-T et al., 2018). In the dorsal brain, the clock neurons' fibers terminate close to regions that have been shown to be involved in the control of locomotion, sleep, and metabolism, such as the PI, the mushroom bodies, and the central complex (see below under “Behavior controlled by the circadian clock of fruit flies and honey bees”).
Figure 2. Schematic representation of the PER and PDF expressing neurons in the fruit fly (left) and honey bee brain (right). The two brains differ in overall size, but especially in the size of the mushroom bodies of which the Calyces (CA) and the vertical lobes (VL) are highlighted. The vertical lobe is also called α-lobe in Drosophila. While the honey bee has two large calyces per brain hemisphere, the fruit fly has only one rather small one. In Drosophila, the only PDF-fibers that run into the dorsal brain, terminate anterior, and slightly dorsally of the CA. In the honey bee, the PDF fibers terminate anterior and ventrally of the CAs and they run into many more central brain areas than in Drosophila. PER is not only present in the lateral (LN) and dorsal neurons, but also in the photoreceptor cells of the compound eyes and ocelli and in numerous glia cells that are only indicated in the lamina (LA) and the distal medulla (ME) in Drosophila and in few other brain regions in the honey bee. Note that the nuclei of the photoreceptor cells are only shown in the dorsal half of the compound eye in Drosophila. The honey bee scheme is modified from Beer et al. (2018). For details see text.
One of the best conserved and most important neuropeptides in the insect circadian clock is the Pigment-Dispersing Factor (PDF) (Renn et al., 1999; Helfrich-Förster et al., 2000; Helfrich-Förster, 2014; Ikeno et al., 2014; Shafer and Yao, 2014; Wei et al., 2014; Beer et al., 2018). In D. melanogaster, PDF is expressed in four small ventro-lateral neurons (s-LNv) and in four large ventro-lateral neurons (l-LNv), which have different roles in the clock network. While the l-LNv are dispensable for rhythmic activity, they signal to the s-LNv (Klose et al., 2016; Schlichting et al., 2016; Menegazzi et al., 2017) and they are part of the light-input pathway to the clock (Helfrich-Förster, 2020).
The s-LNv are major pacemaker neurons that are essential for robust rhythmic activity under constant darkness. Although the s-LNv projections that terminate in the dorsolateral protocerebrum anteriorly of the mushroom body calyces look relatively simple, they appear to signal to different neuropils and downstream neurons via paracrine secretion of PDF. The s-LNv terminals broaden and PDF accumulates in them in the morning (Park et al., 2000; Fernández et al., 2008) suggesting that it is also released at this time into the dorsolateral brain. PDF-receptors are for example on the ellipsoid body of the central complex (Pírez et al., 2013), which is a higher coordination center in the insect brain responsible for motor control and orientation and recently the ellipsoid body has been established as important output circuit downstream of the circadian clock neurons (Liang et al., 2019) (see also below).
However, PDF is not only a putative output factor of the clock but also the most important communication factor within the clock (Lin et al., 2004; Yoshii et al., 2009a; Helfrich-Förster, 2014; Shafer and Yao, 2014). The PDF receptor is expressed on many clock neurons, including the PDF-positive s-LNv (Shafer et al., 2008; Im and Taghert, 2010; Choi et al., 2012; Klose et al., 2016). PDF is able to couple the molecular oscillations of individual clock neurons by speeding them up or slowing them down (Lin et al., 2004; Yoshii et al., 2009a). Thus, PDF is the most powerful neuropeptide in the clock network, leading to the hypothesis that the PDF neurons are dominant circadian pacemakers governing the other clock neurons by setting phase and period of their molecular clocks and shaping the activity pattern of the flies (Stoleru et al., 2005; Guo et al., 2014; Chatterjee et al., 2018).
In comparison to the fly, the honey bee brain possesses many more clock neurons (~400 compared to 150 in the fly brain), which are nevertheless clustered similarly like in the fly brain (Fuchikawa et al., 2017; Beer et al., 2018) (Figure 2). There are two rather dorsally located clusters, the DN (dorsal neurons) and the DLN (dorsolateral neurons), and two clusters (LN1, LN2) in the lateral brain between the protocerebrum and the optic lobe. The cell bodies of the LN2 neurons (~15 per hemisphere) are very closely located to the LN1, but are a little bit bigger and produce PDF (Fuchikawa et al., 2017; Beer et al., 2018). Like in Drosophila, the PDF neurons seem to take on a communication function between different clock cells and brain regions for control of downstream behavior, because injections of artificial PDF peptide have been successful to shift the locomotion rhythms in bees (Beer et al., 2018). The PDF neurons build a highly complex network of arborizations widespread throughout the honey bee brain. Like in other Hymenoptera, classification of PDF neurons in the honey bee into different functional groups, has not been possible so far (Bloch et al., 2003; Weiss et al., 2009; Sumiyoshi et al., 2011; Fuchikawa et al., 2017; Beer et al., 2018; Kay et al., 2018). Nevertheless, a functional subdivision of neurons projecting into different brain areas, like it is the case in Drosophila or cockroaches (Reischig et al., 2004; Helfrich-Förster et al., 2007b), is highly likely. Similar to other insects, the PDF neurons in the honey bee brain project into an highly dense fiber hub in the lateral brain close to the optic lobe (Homberg et al., 1991; Helfrich-Förster et al., 1998; Závodská et al., 2003). This “communication center” of the circadian clock may be analog to the aMe in Drosophila and other insects, with a small difference in location: it seems rather less associated with the Medulla than with the Lobula (Beer et al., 2018). This was suggested to be related to the fact that Hymenoptera have no stemmata, which are the precursor of the HB-eyelet in Drosophila development, and the developing honey bee clock may be consequently less associated with photic inputs (Beer et al., 2018).
Additionally to the clock neurons, numerous glia cells expressing PER (which were similarly observed in Drosophila (Siwicki et al., 1988; Helfrich-Förster, 1995) in various brain areas are closely connected via the PDF neuronal network (Fuchikawa et al., 2017; Beer et al., 2018). This fact and evidence from different per expression studies in nursing and foraging bees indicates a crucial role of glia cells in the circadian plasticity of the honey bee clock as we will explain later [see section “Behavior controlled by the circadian clock of fruit flies and honey bees” and review (Beer and Bloch, 2020)].
The Relevance of Zeitgebers Differs Between Flies and Bees
Circadian clocks have to be synchronized to the 24-h day of the earth by Zeitgebers. The most reliable Zeitgebers are the daily light-dark (LD) and temperature cycles, but also social interactions, periodic vibration signals and the availability of food can serve as Zeitgeber. For most adult insects, light is the most important Zeitgeber, which is followed by temperature and social interactions, while the impact of food is only studied in some insects (see below).
The effectiveness of Zeitgebers is very different for developing insects that receive no light input at all, because they nest in cavities or mature underground, such as onion flies (Watari and Tanaka, 2010; Miyazaki et al., 2016), tsetse flies (Ždárek and Denlinger, 1995) and solitary bees (Tweedy and Stephen, 1970; Yocum et al., 2016; Bennett et al., 2018; Beer et al., 2019). For these insects the daily temperature cycle is the most important Zeitgeber for emerging rhythmically from their pupal case and at least solitary bees do not entrain to LD cycles when they are present (Tweedy and Stephen, 1970; Beer et al., 2019). Even fruit flies, which can perceive light through their pupal case and which are nicely entrainable by LD cycles in the lab are very sensitive to temperature cycles (Zimmerman et al., 1968), and under natural conditions, the daily phase of eclosion and the robustness of rhythmicity depends strongly on the environmental temperature conditions (Ruf et al., 2019). This indicates that temperature cycles may be generally more important for entraining the endogenous clock of developing insects than they are for entraining the clock of adult insects. During development the sensitivity to light-dark cycles appears to increase (Watari, 2005; Beer et al., 2019). In some flies, this switch to light sensitivity of the clock may occur earlier than in onion flies or solitary bees [see discussion in Beer et al. (2019)].
Furthermore, although adult insects are all very sensitive to LD cycles, for social insects, such as honey bees, social Zeitgebers can be more important than light (Beer et al., 2016; Fuchikawa et al., 2016). Thereby, different members of a colony can serve as social Zeitgebers: a group of foraging/forager-aged bees or even the single queen was shown to determine the colony rhythm in honey bees (Moritz and Sakofski, 1991; Frisch and Koeniger, 1994; Beer et al., 2016; Fuchikawa et al., 2016). Laboratory experiments with small groups of worker bees showed that the relative number of bees is important for social synchronization and that individual bees preferably adapt the rhythm of larger groups (Moritz and Kryger, 1994). The cues by which social synchronization is mediated between individual honey bees is so far unknown. It appears that pheromones, micro-climate and vibration signals could play a role, while direct tactile and visual contact between con-specifics could be excluded (Eban-Rothschild et al., 2012; Beer et al., 2016; Fuchikawa et al., 2016). Similar factors appear to influence the rhythms of flies. Although, flies are not classified as social, they form groups, interact with each other, adjusting their interactive behavior to group size (Rooke et al., 2020) and their clocks can be entrained by pheromones (Levine et al., 2002; Krupp et al., 2008) and vibrations (Simoni et al., 2014). Clearly, in flies, social synchronization has not the same significance as it has in bees, but studying it might help to unravel the underlying mechanisms.
In the following, we will give an overview about the effectiveness of light and temperature cycles as Zeitgebers in adult flies and bees (Figure 3). For a detailed insight into light-input pathways to the circadian clock of adult insects with an emphasis on the fruit fly the reader is referred to a recent review (Helfrich-Förster, 2020).
Figure 3. Input and Output of the circadian clock in D. melanogaster (left) and A. mellifera (right). Relevance of input signals to the clocks differs between species, so that light appears to be most important for the fly clock, but social signals out rule light signals in entraining the bee clock. Many output behaviors are common among different insects (e.g., locomotion, sleep, mating, metabolism, and foraging), but others are limited to or more pronounced in some species (e.g., eclosion in Drosophila and time-place-memory, sun compass orientation or task-related plasticity in circadian behavior in Apis). Although D. melanogaster does not exhibit pronounced diapause, basic mechanisms in clock regulation involved in dormancy appear to be shared between this fly and diapausing fly species. Similarly, A. mellifera does not go into diapause, but it may be that the honey bee exhibits some residual photoperiodic function in initiating dormancy behavior, because solitary bees display pronounced diapause.
Adult fruit flies entrain to LD and temperature cycles [reviewed in Helfrich-Förster (2017)]. When both Zeitgebers are combined in a natural-like way with the highest temperature occurring after noon, locomotion rhythms are very precise and the molecular clock cycles with high amplitude in all clock neurons (Currie et al., 2009; Yoshii et al., 2009b). When LD and temperature cycles are completely out of phase with each other, wild-type flies entrain to the LD cycles, while mutants without CRY1 entrain to the temperature cycles, suggesting that light is the predominant Zeitgeber and light entrainment is mediated via CRY1 (Yoshii et al., 2010). However, this conclusion is slightly premature. When the two Zeitgeber cycles are <4 h out of phase, the temperature cycles strongly influence the phase of the activity rhythms, meaning that light and temperature interact in the entrainment of wild-type flies and that light only dominates when the two Zeitgebers are too much out of phase (Harper et al., 2016). That the compound eyes and not only CRY1 contribute to light entrainment under conflicting Zeitgeber cycles was shown by Busza et al. (2007). The compound eyes together with CRY1 actively suppress temperature input to the clock neurons in wild-type flies and by this way make sure that the flies are not too sensitive to sudden temperature fluctuations that can easily happen in nature (Busza et al., 2007; Gentile et al., 2013). Most interestingly, different clock neurons are responsible for mediating light- and temperature entrainment in fruit flies: the Dorsal Neurons are more important for temperature entrainment than the Lateral Neurons, which mediate predominantly light entrainment [reviewed in Helfrich-Förster (2017), Yadlapalli et al. (2018), Lamaze and Stanewsky (2020)].
The light-input pathway to the circadian clock in honey bees is less well-studied. Honey bees miss in comparison to the fruit fly the blue light receptor CRY1 and the HB-eyelet. Similar to CRY1-missing Drosophila mutants, the honey bee clock may be less susceptible to light than other environmental cues (and at least for social cues this theory seems to hold true). With tim1 absent in the molecular clock of honey bees, the light input pathway in bees may utilize another mechanism. Tim2 (timeout) was identified as part of a photo-entrainment mechanism in Drosophila (Benna et al., 2010), which only plays an residual role in photoreception compared to tim1 in Drosophila. The relevance of the mammalian ortholog of tim2 for the circadian system is highly debated until today (Gotter et al., 2000; Barnes, 2003; Gotter, 2006). However, with cry1 missing, a tim2 mediated photoreception in the compound eyes may be the major photo-entrainment pathway in Hymenoptera (Benna et al., 2010). Furthermore, a vertebrate-like opsin called pteropsin, may be part of the light-input pathway to the clock in honey bees besides the compound eyes and the ocelli (Velarde et al., 2005). The expression pattern of pteropsin strongly resembles the location of PER expressing clock cells (Fuchikawa et al., 2017; Beer et al., 2018).
Behavior Controlled by the Circadian Clock of Fruit Flies and Honey Bees
Basic circadian clock output behavior is similar between fruit flies and honey bees. However, honey bees have an extraordinary rich behavioral repertoire and, due to their age- and caste-dependent differences in behavior, they are perfect models to study circadian clock development and plasticity as well as socially regulated clock output. Therefore, we will first review the general rhythmic behavior of flies and bees and then concentrate on honey bee behavior.
Output Rhythms in Flies and Bees
The best studied daily rhythms in flies and bees are those of locomotion and of sleeping/waking [reviewed for flies by Dubowy and Sehgal (2017), King and Sehgal (2020), and for bees by Moore (2001), Eban-Rothschild and Bloch (2012)]. The honey bee has been found to be a very good model for sleep, because of its detailed description of sleep architecture (Kaiser, 1988; Sauer et al., 2003; Klein et al., 2014), while the fruit fly has been very helpful to unravel the underlying molecular and recently also neuronal mechanism of sleep [reviewed in Helfrich-Förster (2017), Guo et al. (2018)]. In both insects, disturbances of the sleep-wake-rhythm result in reduced learning ability (Hussaini et al., 2009; Toda et al., 2018; Donlea, 2019) and in honey bees additionally in reduced communication ability, which is very similar in humans (Klein et al., 2010), suggesting that sleep is essential to maintain neuronal plasticity, learning and memory in all animals.
The daily rhythm in movement (locomotion), which is best studied in all insects investigated so far, can serve different purposes. Insects may be active for foraging, for seeking mates, for nesting/brood care activity or just because their circadian clock dictates them to be active. In most laboratory systems that record movements of insects, it is impossible to distinguish between these different possibilities. Here, natural studies with honey bee foragers are of great advantage. Indeed, their foraging rhythms are the first behavioral rhythms described in honey bees (Beling, 1929; Wahl, 1933; Kleber, 1935; Frisch and Aschoff, 1987; Moore et al., 1989). These studies showed that honey bees forage throughout the day depending on the available food sources and that they have an excellent memory about time and location of open flowers. When trained in restricted feeding cycles, honey bee foragers can remember up to nine time-points per day (Koltermann, 1971). Although, entrainment via feeding has been deployed in many studies, the link between the clock and time-place-learning in foraging behavior of bees is largely unknown (Pahl et al., 2007; Moore and Doherty, 2009; Mulder et al., 2013). Only recently, it was shown that restricted feeding indeed can phase shift the molecular clock of honey bees (Jain and Brockmann, 2018).
When recorded in isolation and under controlled conditions in the lab, honey bee foragers show activity throughout the day and few activity during the night (Moore and Rankin, 1985). Fruit flies have a completely different activity pattern under such conditions. They exhibit bimodal activity rhythms with activity bouts in the morning and evening and a pronounced siesta in between, and this is true for both sexes (Helfrich-Förster, 2000). Nevertheless, there are differences between the sexes. Mated females show a greatly reduced siesta, probably because they search for places for depositing their eggs. Oviposition occurs rhythmically in female flies starting in the middle of the day and reaching a maximum in the evening (McCabe and Birley, 1998; Sheeba et al., 2001; Manjunatha et al., 2008), which fits to the high activity of isolated mated females during this time. In contrast, isolated males begin activity significantly earlier in the morning than females, which can be explained by a search for female mating partners (Helfrich-Förster, 2000). Indeed, male sex drive behavior has been shown to be controlled by the circadian clock (Fujii et al., 2007, 2017; Fujii and Amrein, 2010) and mating occurs rhythmically with a maximum in the early morning hours (Sakai and Ishida, 2001; Lin et al., 2014). Nevertheless, sex drive is influenced by the presence of females and is not generally restricted to the morning: male flies that are housed together with females become highly active throughout the night and early morning (Fujii et al., 2007). Similar to activity, feeding occurs at slightly different times of the day in male and female flies. While males feed maximally in the early morning, females do so from the middle of the day until the evening (Seay and Thummel, 2011; Xu et al., 2011; Liu et al., 2020; Schäbler et al., 2020). In summary, general activity, mating and feeding occur at different times of the day and additionally show sex differences in timing. Thus, these three rhythms may be controlled by different neuronal pathways from the clock neurons to the effector organs.
Unlike in the honey bee, no studies under real natural conditions have been performed in fruit flies and only few studies have addressed fly behavior under quasi natural conditions (Vanin et al., 2012; Green et al., 2015). Thus, we don't know yet, which activities are performed by fruit flies in nature and when they occur naturally. Nevertheless, many more rhythms are known from fruit flies studied under laboratory conditions, of which some have been also found in honey bees, while others appear absent in honey bees. For example, fruit flies do not only lay their eggs in a rhythmical manner, but they also eclose rhythmically from their pupal case, with a peak of eclosion in the subjective morning (Pittendrigh, 1954; Lin et al., 2014). Similar rhythms in the queen's oviposition or rhythms in emergence of newly eclosed honey bees could not be detected in honey bees (Free et al., 1992; Harano et al., 2007; Johnson et al., 2010). Most probably, such rhythms have no selective advantage in the protected beehive (but they do so in solitary bees, see above). Nevertheless, mating between drones and queens happens in a rhythmic manner as it does in fruit flies. Mating of bees always occurs in the afternoon (Lensky and Demter, 1985), while the exact timing of mating flights can be altered by selective forces (e.g., the presence of sympatric species within the same location). Generally, honey bees strongly avoid an overlap in flight times between sympatric species, either to avoid interspecific hybrids or a reduction in the efficiency of mating (Koeniger and Koeniger, 2000). We expect that such selective forces will also alter the timing of the different rhythms in fruit flies under natural conditions.
As do bees, flies can also remember the time of day, at least to some degree (Chouhan et al., 2015). Chouhan et al. (2015) showed that flies can remember two time points per day, as long as the two are at least 6 h apart. This memory depended on a functional circadian clock and did neither persist in the absence of the PER protein nor in the absence of the neuropeptide PDF. Time memory in honey bees is much more sophisticated than in flies, but it may relay on the same connections of the circadian clock neurons with the centers of memory, the mushroom bodies (see below). Furthermore, in flies and bees, the ability to learn is modulated by the circadian clock and different at different times of day (Sakai et al., 2004; Lehmann et al., 2011). Again, this requires a functional relationship between the clock neurons and the mushroom bodies.
Sun Compass Orientation
Honey bees and other insects are famous for their remarkable spatial orientation, which relies on a time-compensated sun-compass (Lindauer, 1960). Because of the earth's rotation the relative position of the sun changes during the day and the bee has to compensate for the past time during flight. In several studies, in which the honey bee clock was phase shifted, it was shown that the circadian clock is essential for sun-compass orientation (Medugorac and Lindauer, 1967; Cheeseman et al., 2012, Cheeseman et al., 2014). Recently, putative input neurons of the clock to the sky compass orientation pathway in the honey bee brain have been identified (Zeller et al., 2015; Beer et al., 2018). Transmedulla neurons of the sky compass pathway originating at the dorsal rim area of the medulla run in close proximity to PDF neurons (Zeller et al., 2015). PDF neurons, on the other hand may communicate with the central complex, which generally controls orientation and navigation in insects (e.g., Pegel et al., 2019).
Also flies can fly straight over long distances, indicating that they can perceive celestial cues and might even be able to compensate for the movements of the sun although this is not yet proven (Giraldo et al., 2018; Mathejczyk and Wernet, 2019; Warren et al., 2019). If true, research on flies may help to decipher the neuronal pathway from polarization vision to the circadian clock and from there to the brain centers controlling orientation such as the central complex. In any case, good interdisciplinary communication between fly and bee researchers will strongly support the successful elucidation of sun-compass orientation mechanisms.
Task Related Plasticity of the Clock in Honey Bees
Honey bees have an age-related division of labor displayed by worker bees. At the age of 2–10 days bees assume brood care (nursing) functions in the colony, later they take over other duties such as storing and processing food or guarding the hive and around 3 weeks of age they begin to forage pollen or nectar and are now called foragers (Free, 1965). This division of labor is associated with pronounced changes in rhythmic behavior (Crailsheim et al., 1996; Moore et al., 1998; Bloch and Robinson, 2001). Typically, young bees tend the brood without a rhythm in locomotion, which is supposed to be beneficial for optimizing brood care and colony growth. They also display more and less pronounced sleep-bouts scattered over the day (Eban-Rothschild and Bloch, 2008; Klein et al., 2008). Older foraging bees on the other hand, display robust day-night rhythms of activity and sleep. This behavior is highly plastic and bees can start prematurely rhythmic foraging or revert back to nursing without rhythms, all according to the need of the colony (Bloch and Robinson, 2001). The cues establishing arrhythmic behavior in bees are largely unknown, but contact to the brood is essential (Nagari and Bloch, 2012; Nagari et al., 2017a). The plasticity in this behavior is further demonstrated by the fact that nurse bees displayed rhythmic behavior (locomotion) shortly after removing them from the colony (Shemesh et al., 2007, 2010; Eban-Rothschild et al., 2012; Fuchikawa et al., 2016; Nagari et al., 2017b). Moreover, molecular studies were in line with the behavioral experiments and showed attenuation of the circadian rhythm in mRNA levels of the clock genes per, cry2, cyc, and cwo in nurses compared to foragers (Bloch et al., 2001; Shemesh et al., 2007, Shemesh et al., 2010; Rodriguez-Zas et al., 2012). This implies a major reorganization of the circadian clock system. But does the clock of nurse bees actually stop? It was puzzling to see that the clock of nurses drove activity rhythms in the laboratory that were in phase with the ambient day-night rhythm of the colony, they had been removed from. Furthermore, PER protein levels were cycling in brains of behaviorally arrhythmic nurse bees (Fuchikawa et al., 2017). That means that the clock in nurse bees keeps ticking even when they are behaviorally arrhythmic! Still, why is it that mRNA oscillation of clock genes is attenuated in comparison to forager bees? As we have already mentioned, the numerous PER expressing glia cells may play an essential role in the honey bee clock. In Drosophila and mammals, it was demonstrated that non-neuronal cells are part of the circadian clock and contribute to formation of rhythmic behavior (Ng et al., 2011; Jackson et al., 2015; Brancaccio et al., 2017). Similarly, the PER expressing glia cells could be involved in circadian plasticity in honey bees. A differential coupling of clock cells in nurses and foragers may also be possible in the highly complex circadian clock network of honey bees. Future studies may help to elucidate the regulation mechanism of circadian plasticity in honey bees.
Interestingly, also queens display circadian plasticity in their behavior. In the colony, they were observed to be behaviorally arrhythmic while laying eggs (Free et al., 1992; Johnson et al., 2010). However, when isolated in the laboratory, they show rhythms in locomotion in phase with the ambient day-night cycle (Harano et al., 2007).
Neuronal Output Pathways From the Clock to Downstream Neurons
Although circadian clock output behaviors have been studied in numerous insects, the output pathways from the circadian clock in the brain to rhythmic behavior have been revealed only lately and in many cases are not well-understood yet. Here, again Drosophila with its manifold genetic tools has been the pioneer model. In the following, we will briefly review the different possible neuronal pathways from the clock to downstream neurons in the brain that may in turn communicate with the body.
Starting with development and eclosion from the pupal case, Selcho et al. (2017) showed that the s-LNv transfer timing information via the neuropeptide sNPF (small neuropeptide F), which is co-produced with PDF, to neurosecretory cells in the dorso-lateral brain that produce the neuropeptide PTTH (Prothoracotropic hormone). PTTH then forwards time information to the prothoracic gland, which secretes the steroid molting hormone, ecdysone. The titer of ecdysone gates subsequent eclosion. The mating rhythm of adult flies appears to be correlated with pheromone release from the oenocyctes and the latter is coupled to the circadian clock in the brain via PDF (Krupp et al., 2013). The precise pathways of this regulation are however not yet known.
Sleep is controlled by the central complex (besides other brain areas) and a connection from the clock neurons to the ellipsoid body of the central complex has been identified by Guo et al. (2018), Lamaze et al. (2018), and Lamaze et al. (2018) [nicely summarized by Hsu and Sehgal (2018)]. These authors show that specific DN1 clock neurons that get input from the s-LNv contact so-called tubercular-bulbar (TuBu) neurons that in turn are connected to ellipsoid body ring neurons that promote sleep. Most importantly, these ellipsoid body ring neurons are different from those involved in spatial orientation (see above). They are also different from ring neurons that have been recently shown to be implicated in the control of the flies' bimodal activity in the morning and evening (Liang et al., 2019). Thus, there are several parallel pathways ending in specific neurons of the ellipsoid body. Liang et al. (2019) demonstrated that other specific ring neurons of the ellipsoid body display spontaneous morning and evening neural activity peaks that coincide with the bouts of locomotion and that get inputs from circadian clock neurons that control morning and evening activity, respectively. This input is indirect and occurs via specific dopaminergic neurons that also arborize in the ellipsoid body. The s-LNv control morning activity and PDF is able to activate the dopaminergic neurons as well as the ellipsoid body ring neurons (Liang et al., 2019). Thus, PDF may be one of the clock factors that signals directly to the ellipsoid body [see Pírez et al. (2013) and above], while the connection from the evening clock neurons to the ellipsoid body neurons is still unknown [for more information on morning and evening clock neurons see Yoshii et al. (2012)]. Nevertheless, activity is controlled also by other parallel pathways that circumvent the ellipsoid body and run via the hormonal center, the PI in the middle dorsal brain [reviewed in King and Sehgal (2020)]. One important humoral pathway runs via six PI neurons that produce Diuretic Hormone 44 (DH44) a homolog of the mammalian stress hormone releasing factor (Cavanaugh et al., 2014). DH44 neurons receive synaptic inputs from the DN1 clock neurons and DH44 is important for strong activity rhythms under constant darkness. However DH44 neurons are most active during mid-day (Bai et al., 2018) making it likely that they promote activity for other reasons. Possibly, they elevate activity for searching food or egg-laying places in females. In addition to the DH44 neurons, there are further neurons in the dorsal brain that are contacted by the clock neurons and contribute to shaping activity (Pírez et al., 2019).
Feeding rhythms per se are controlled by a different set of PI neurons, the four SIFamide positive neurons (Dreyer et al., 2019). The SIFamide neurons are also contacted by the DN1 clock neurons and project to the subesophageal ganglion that is involved in gustatory processing and contains feeding-related motor neurons. Indeed, stimulation of the SIFamide neurons increases food intake (Martelli et al., 2017). Feeding is additionally controlled by leucokinin-positive neurons that are downstream of the s-LNv and other clock neurons (Cavey et al., 2016) and that mediate hunger signals to promote locomotion (Zandawala et al., 2018; Yurgel et al., 2019). Finally, the PI contains 14 Insulin-like-Peptide positive neurons (IPCs) that are contacted by the DN1 and the s-LNv clock neurons and control circadian gene expression in the fat body (Barber et al., 2016) and general metabolism (see next chapter).
In summary, there are multiple output pathways from the circadian clock neurons that all originate in the dorsal brain (reaching from lateral to mid-central brain areas) (Figure 4). In the honey bee, these output pathways have not been elucidated in detail, but one can easily see in the pattern of the PDF arborizations that these neurons alone can easily establish contacts with the relevant brain areas mentioned for Drosophila (Figure 2). It will be most interesting to reveal the arborizations of the other clock neurons of the honey bee, especially with regard to the integration of the honey bee complex behaviors in the clock output network.
Figure 4. Simplified scheme showing the output pathways of the clock neurons in the brain of D. melanogaster. The somata of the clock neurons in the lateral and dorsal brain are depicted in gray with the s-LNv and DN1 highlighted. The clock neurons are connected with each other and most of them have neurites in the accessory medulla (aMe), where they receive major light-input, as well as in the dorsal brain where they connect different neuropeptidergic neurons. The prothoracotrophic hormone (PTTH) positive neurons receive rhythmic signals via sNPF from the s-LNv and control the timing of eclosion by triggering the release of ecdysone from the prothoracotropic glands (shown in the left brain hemisphere). The leukokine (LK) positive neurons receive also rhythmic signals from the s-LNv and perhaps from other lateral neurons and contact via tubercular-bulbar neurons (not shown) the bulb (Bu) where ring neurons of the ellipsoid body (EB) have their dendrites (shown in the right brain hemisphere). The corresponding EB ring neurons contribute to the rhythmic control of activity. In addition, the morning neurons (s-LNv) and evening neurons (not highlighted) signal via PDF (stippled) and unknown pathways (E?) to dopaminergic neurons (not shown) that arborize in the Bu and the EB and contact putatively the same activity promoting ring neurons in the EB in order to stimulate morning and evening activity. Other ring neurons in the EB receive rhythmic signals from the DN1 and control sleep, while still others may get rhythmic input from the clock neurons via still unknown pathways and control orientation. Furthermore, activity, feeding and metabolism are controlled by diuretic hormone 44 (DH44), SIFamide (SIFa), and Insulin-like peptide (ILP) expressing neurons in the pars intercerebralis (PI), which receive rhythmic signals from the clock neurons. Finally, the clock neurons control learning and contribute to time memory via still unknown pathways (?) to the mushroom bodies (MB).
The Role of the Clock in Photoperiodism and Diapause Needs New Insect Models
A central question in chronobiology is how endogenous clocks changed in order to anticipate vastly different cyclical environmental conditions on earth, especially such that exist close to the poles. Organisms like D. melanogaster, and also Homo sapiens, are assumed to have developed in tropical regions that are characterized by regular 24 h cycles in irradiance and temperature that remain the same throughout the seasons. However, in northern and southern hemispheres of the earth photoperiods vary throughout the year causing the well-known seasons of spring, summer, autumn and winter. All organisms including insects have to anticipate these seasons in order to be prepared in advance for the coming spring-summer and autumn-winter. Bünning (1936) hypothesized that the main purpose of the circadian clock is to provide the necessary time reference for measuring day length so that organisms can prepare in time for the winter. A failure in such a preparation will ultimately lead to death. Similarly, a failure to predict the coming warm season will lead to the death of the offspring, since a too early or too late reproduction may result in too low temperatures and/or shortages of food. Small animals, such as insects are especially sensitive to seasonal changes and need to be well-prepared.
One strategy of avoiding adverse conditions is reproductive arrest that is also called dormancy or diapause. Dormancy is a generic term covering any state of developmental arrest, which is adaptive and usually accompanied with metabolic suppression (Koštál, 2006). As soon as the adverse environmental conditions disappear, insects can terminate dormancy. Diapause is a specific subtype of dormancy, which is a more profound, endogenously and centrally mediated interruption that routes the developmental program away from direct morphogenesis into an alternative diapause program of succession of several physiological events. The start of diapause usually precedes the advent of adverse conditions. Since temperature alone is not a reliable predictor, most organism use day length (= photoperiod) as a measure for the coming season and diapause is induced as soon as day length falls beyond a certain threshold, the critical day length (Koštál, 2006). The responses to changing photoperiods are called photoperiodic responses. This is different for the obligatory diapause, which is present in insects that complete only one generation each year (Denlinger et al., 2017). Typical examples are solitary bees or Rhagoletis fruit flies. Such insects do not need a mechanism to measure day length for diapause induction, because they enter diapause at a fixed developmental stage regardless of prevailing environmental cues. However, environmental cues remain essential for regulating the timing of diapause termination, because the mechanism for terminating diapause at the appropriate time dictates the active window of the insect's life.
Tropical insects are not exposed to seasonal differences in photoperiod and don't have to undergo winter diapause. Thus, they have no need to measure day length. This does not mean that they don't undergo dormancy or even diapause, just other cues such as temperature, moisture, and changes in food quality dictate the induction of dormancy (Denlinger, 1986). Our model organisms, the honey bee, Apis mellifera, and the fruit fly, D. melanogaster, stem from tropical regions. Honey bees live additionally in hives and can actively produce heat; both protect them from the coldness of winter. Therefore, both species do not exhibit a photoperiodically induced diapause. Nevertheless, they undergo a state of dormancy in response to low temperatures and shortage of available food that can be enhanced by shortening day length (Kefuss, 1978; Saunders et al., 1989). In contrast to real diapause this state does not include a succession of defined physiological events and it can be terminated at any time when the environmental conditions improve.
Can we, nevertheless, learn something about the mechanisms of dormancy from D. melanogaster? The adult female flies exhibit a reproductive dormancy manifested by reduced metabolic activity and arrested ovarian development, which is stimulated by low temperatures and can be enhanced by short natural photoperiods and food shortage (Nagy et al., 2018, 2019; Ojima et al., 2018). As in other insects, the insulin-like peptide (ILP) producing cells (IPCs) are key regulators of this process, since they produce and release insulin-like peptides that act as diapause-antagonizing hormones. Although, fruit flies have no photoperiodically induced diapause that needs communication with the endogenous clock to determine day length, it was recently shown that the circadian clock neurons communicate with the IPC cells (Nagy et al., 2019). The s-LNv clock neurons activate the IPCs via the neuropeptides Pigment-Dispersing Factor (PDF) and short neuropeptide F (sNPF), which in turn release insulin-like peptides, antagonize dormancy and lead to reproductive growth. This result suggests that the secretion of PDF and sNPF is enhanced under long summer days and by this way keep the flies in the reproductive state. PDF is indeed secreted during the day (Park et al., 2000), but it is still unknown whether its secretion is prolonged or enhanced under long days.
That the clock communicates with the dormancy inducing centers in the central brain of D. melanogaster flies can also be inferred from the fact that some components of the molecular circadian clock affect dormancy incidence. For example, a long isoform of the clock protein TIM (L-TIM) evolved a few 100–1,000 years ago, after D. melanogaster colonized Europe (Sandrelli et al., 2007; Tauber et al., 2007; Zonato et al., 2018); reviewed in Kyriacou (2014b). This long isoform coexists with the original short form (S-TIM), and flies carrying both isoforms (“LS-TIM” flies) gradually invaded Northern Europe and North America (Pegoraro et al., 2017). LS-TIM has a reduced ability to interact with CRY, which makes the flies less light-sensitive and less likely to become arrhythmic under extreme long days. In addition, the “LS-TIM” flies enter dormancy earlier in autumn than the S-TIM flies. Both properties are advantageous for a life in the north. A very recent study supports the role of TIM in dormancy of D. melanogaster (Abrieux et al., 2020). The authors showed that tim null mutants exhibit reduced incidence of reproductive dormancy in simulated winter conditions, while flies overexpressing tim show an increased incidence of reproductive dormancy even under long photoperiods.
What about insects that exhibit a real photoperiodic diapause? Most interestingly, the tim1 or cry1 clock genes plays also a role in the photoperiodic response of such species as for example the Japanese fruit fly Chymomyza costata (Stehlík et al., 2008), the fruit fly Drosophila triauraria (Yamada and Yamamoto, 2011), the silkworm Bombyx mori (Li, 2011), the Asian Tiger mosquito Aedes albopictus (Huang et al., 2015), and the Northern house mosquito Culex pipiens (Meuti et al., 2015). In photoperiodic species that possess no TIM1 and no light-sensitive CRY1 such as the bean bug Riptortus pedestris or the Linden bug Pyrrhocoris apterus other clock genes such as per or Clk are involved in the photoperiodic response (Syrová et al., 2003; Ikeno et al., 2010, 2011a,b).
Analogous to D. melanogaster, a knockdown of the neuropeptide PDF caused female Culex pipiens that were reared under long day conditions to enter a diapause-like state (Meuti et al., 2015). Furthermore, the ablation of the PDF-positive clock neurons in the blow fly Protophormia terraenovae interferes with photoperiodic diapause induction in such a way that the flies could not discriminate long and short days and half of the flies entered diapause at both conditions (Shiga and Numata, 2009). This confirms the importance of PDF as signaling molecule from the circadian clock to the IPC cells.
In most cases, PDF signaling to the IPC cells appears to keep flies in the reproductive state. Most interestingly, several Drosophila species such as D. montana, D. littoralis, D. ezoana, and D. virilis that live in the very north lack PDF in the s-LNv clock neurons that project to the IPC cells (Bahn et al., 2009; Kauranen et al., 2012; Hermann et al., 2013; Menegazzi et al., 2017; Beauchamp et al., 2018); reviewed in Helfrich-Förster et al. (2018). These species have a high incidence of reproductive arrest already under long-day lengths, which is an adaptation to the low temperatures even under summer photoperiods at these clines. For example, D. ezoana enters diapause when day-length falls below 16 h (Vaze and Helfrich-Förster, 2016). The lack of PDF-signaling to the IPCs of these species might facilitate the termination of the reproductive state already at these relatively long days. In addition to lacking PDF in the s-LNv clock neurons, these high-altitude flies lack CRY in other clock neurons: the l-LNv [reviewed in Helfrich-Förster et al. (2018)]. This may enhance the flies' ability to enter dormancy earlier in the seasons as true for the less light-sensitive TIM-LS flies of D. melanogaster.
Nevertheless, not all fly species that are adapted to high altitudes lack PDF and CRY in certain clock neurons. For example, C. costata flies that are distributed in Eastern Siberia, Northern Lapland, Iceland, and from northern Japan to the Artic Cycle (Hackman et al., 1970) possess a D. melanogaster-like PDF network (Bertolini et al., 2019). This shows that the circadian clock of C. costata flies has found other ways to adapt to high-latitudes. PDF may just promote metabolic and reproductive activity, but there is no prove that it is really necessary for photoperiodic information. On the contrary: most photoperiodic species lack PDF signals to the IPC cells and nevertheless undergo diapause at a critical day length. For example, in the aphid Acyrthosipon pisum, which is a classic model for photoperiodism, PDF was even not found at all (Beer, Colizzi and Helfrich-Förster, unpublished). Furthermore, we still lack a detailed pathway leading from photoreception to expression of diapause. Though a functional circadian clock appears essential for the diapause response, it is not at all clear how the circadian clock and the photoperiodic timer are integrated. How are short days distinguished from long days, and how is this critical information stored in the brain to be acted upon at a later stage or even in the following generation?
Although most insects enter dormancy at some point in their life cycle, insects that currently offer the best models for genetic research lack a robust photoperiodic diapause. Further development of genetic tools for non-model species, including both loss and gain-of-function mutations, are urgently needed to advance the exciting field of insect photoperiodism. In addition, laboratory-based experiments can benefit from carefully simulated natural environments under controlled conditions, and whenever possible experiments should also be carried out in the wild. The natural world offers an incredibly rich diversity of biological clocks that can be probed for understanding the timing of seasonal activity.
Concluding Remarks on Evolution of the Circadian Clock in Insects
As we have elucidated above, the model insects D. melanogaster and A. mellifera, have remarkable advantages in representing different aspects in chronobiology: Their genomes are sequenced, and many components of the circadian clockwork are already identified. They show a variety of circadian output behaviors with species specific relevance and differing sensitivity to various inputs. Interestingly, some basic concepts may be transferable although the chronobiological function substantially differs between species (e.g., sun compass orientation, time-place-learning, emergence, and diapause). Nevertheless, restricting the research to model organisms is insufficient to understand quite a few aspects in insect clock evolution. We may learn best from insect models, when we investigate the circadian clock in parallel in various insects, which display a modification or more pronounced function of the circadian clock, like diapause in northern flies or other insects with pronounced diapause [e.g., aphids (Barberà and Martínez-Torres, 2017; Barberà et al., 2017), bugs (Kotwica-Rolinska et al., 2017), wasps (Reznik, 2011; Paolucci et al., 2019), or butter flies (Denlinger et al., 2017)], or emergence rhythms in solitary bees. This may provide us furthermore with a better insight into circadian clock evolution. Even task related plasticity in the circadian clock is not restricted to honey bees. Ants were also found to perform arrhythmic brood care and have task related plasticity in clock gene expression (Ingram et al., 2009; Fujioka et al., 2017). This indicates, that although sociality evolved several times independently in Hymenoptera (in bees, wasps and ants), there seems to be a common ground plan to the social clock of Hymenoptera [reviewed in Bloch (2009), Bloch (2010), Bloch and Grozinger (2011)].
Studying modifications to the circadian clock in social and non-social Hymenopterans may be the key to understand the concept of social clocks. Especially bees (Anthophila) provide a huge range of differently scaled social lifestyles. We find true social (eusocial) bees (e.g., honey bees), primitively social (e.g., bumble bees), facultatively social and solitary bees (Shell and Rehan, 2018). Primitively social bumble bee queens also display circadian plasticity: when founding a new colony they take care of their first brood without behavioral rhythms, but resume rhythmic activity, when the brood is removed (Eban-Rothschild et al., 2011). Contrary to honey bees, brood care of bumble bee workers is rather related to their body size than their age (Yerushalmi et al., 2006).
Regarding the molecular clock, it appears not enough to compare the clockwork of social Hymenoptera with solitary insects from other orders (like for example D. melanogaster), because the unique composition of the circadian clock gene set in Hymenoptera indicates a regulatory mechanism that is different from the one in other insect orders. Therefore, exploring the clock genes, neuronal network and clock regulated behavior in solitary bees (and bees of different social grades) appears essential in future studies on the Hymenopteran clock.
Finally, we want to mention one further topic: inter-species interactions and co-evolution in chronobiology. Apart from temporal reproductive barriers (see above: species specific mating flights in honey bees), cohabitating insects establish species specific daytime-dependent foraging activity because of competition for food resources (Krell-Westerwalbesloh et al., 2004; Gottlieb et al., 2005; Wcislo and Tierney, 2009; Bloch et al., 2017; Smith et al., 2017). For example, the solitary bee Proxylocopa olivieri forages with a bimodal activity peaking at dusk and dawn and thereby avoids interaction with other bee species like A. mellifera, which shows unimodal foraging during the day (Gottlieb et al., 2005). Or in case of different cohabitating dung beetle guilds, the superior competitors are active during the day, while beetle guilds of lower competitive status display a peak in activity around dusk (Krell-Westerwalbesloh et al., 2004). Such inter-species effects in insect interaction networks, just like species specific clock outputs, clearly can only be sufficiently researched by studying the circadian clock of both, model and non-model insects in chronobiology.
Author Contributions
All authors listed have made a substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.
Funding
Our research on the circadian clock of insects was funded by the European Union's Horizon 2020 research and innovation program CINCHRON under the Marie Sklodowska-Curie grant agreement No 765937, by an individual grant of the German Research Foundation DFG to CH-F (FO 207/15-1) and an individual grant of the University of Würzburg to KB (Postdoc researcher funding program SCIENCIA). This publication was supported by the Open Access Publication Fund of the University of Würzburg.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
The reviewer CK declared a past co-authorship with one of the authors CH-F to the handling Editor
References
Abrieux, A., Xue, Y., Cai, Y., Lewald, K. M., Nhu Nguyen, H., Zhang, Y., et al. (2020). EYES ABSENT and TIMELESS integrate photoperiodic and temperature cues to regulate seasonal physiology in Drosophila. BioRxiv 117, 15293–15304. doi: 10.1101/2020.03.05.979682
Bae, K., Lee, C., Sidote, D., Chuang, K., and Edery, I. (1998). Circadian regulation of a Drosophila homolog of the mammalian clock gene: PER and TIM function as positive regulators. Mol. Cell. Biol. 18, 6142–6151. doi: 10.1128/MCB.18.10.6142
Bahn, J. H., Lee, G., and Park, J. H. (2009). Comparative analysis of pdf-mediated circadian behaviors between Drosophila melanogaster and D. virilis. Genetics 181, 965–975. doi: 10.1534/genetics.108.099069
Bai, L., Lee, Y., Hsu, C. T., Williams, J. A., Cavanaugh, D., Zheng, X., et al. (2018). A conserved circadian function for the Neurofibromatosis 1 gene. Cell Rep. 22, 3416–3426. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2018.03.014
Barber, A. F., Erion, R., Holmes, T. C., and Sehgal, A. (2016). Circadian and feeding cues integrate to drive rhythms of physiology in Drosophila insulin-producing cells. Genes Dev. 30, 2596–2606. doi: 10.1101/gad.288258.116
Barberà, M., Collantes-Alegre, J. M., and Martínez-Torres, D. (2017). Characterisation, analysis of expression and localisation of circadian clock genes from the perspective of photoperiodism in the aphid Acyrthosiphon pisum. Insect. Biochem. Mol. Biol. 83, 54–67. doi: 10.1016/j.ibmb.2017.02.006
Barberà, M., and Martínez-Torres, D. (2017). Identification of the prothoracicotropic hormone (Ptth) coding gene and localization of its site of expression in the pea aphid Acyrthosiphon pisum. Insect. Mol. Biol. 26, 654–664. doi: 10.1111/imb.12326
Barnes, J. W. (2003). Requirement of mammalian timeless for circadian rhythmicity. Science 302, 439–442. doi: 10.1126/science.1086593
Bazalova, O., and Dolezel, D. (2017). Daily activity of the housefly, Musca domestica, is influenced by temperature independent of 3′ UTR period gene splicing. G3 7, 2637–2649. doi: 10.1534/g3.117.042374
Beauchamp, M., Bertolini, E., Deppisch, P., Steubing, J., Menegazzi, P., and Helfrich-Förster, C. (2018). Closely related fruit fly species living at different latitudes diverge in their circadian clock anatomy and rhythmic behavior. J. Biol. Rhythms 33, 602–613. doi: 10.1177/0748730418798096
Beer, K., and Bloch, G. (2020). Circadian plasticity in honey bees. Biochemist 42, 22–26. doi: 10.1042/BIO04202002
Beer, K., Kolbe, E., Kahana, N. B., Yayon, N., Weiss, R., Menegazzi, P., et al. (2018). Pigment-dispersing factor-expressing neurons convey circadian information in the honey bee brain. Open Biol. 8:170224. doi: 10.1098/rsob.170224
Beer, K., Schenk, M., Helfrich-Förster, C., and Holzschuh, A. (2019). The circadian clock uses different environmental time cues to synchronize emergence and locomotion of the solitary bee Osmia bicornis. Sci. Rep. 9:17748. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-54111-3
Beer, K., Steffan-Dewenter, I., Härtel, S., and Helfrich-Förster, C. (2016). A new device for monitoring individual activity rhythms of honey bees reveals critical effects of the social environment on behavior. J. Comp. Physiol. A 202, 555–565. doi: 10.1007/s00359-016-1103-2
Beling, I. (1929). Über das Zeitgedächtnis der Bienen. Zeitschrift Für Vergleichende Physiologie 9, 259–338. doi: 10.1007/BF00340159
Benito, J., Zheng, H., and Hardin, P. E. (2007). PDP1 functions downstream of the circadian oscillator to mediate behavioral rhythms. J. Neurosci. 27, 2539–2547. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4870-06.2007
Benna, C., Bonaccorsi, S., Wülbeck, C., Helfrich-Förster, C., Gatti, M., Kyriacou, C. P., et al. (2010). Drosophila timeless2 is required for chromosome stability and circadian photoreception. Curr. Biol. 20, 346–352. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.12.048
Bennett, M. M., Rinehart, J. P., Yocum, G. D., Doetkott, C., and Greenlee, K. J. (2018). Cues for cavity nesters: investigating relevant zeitgebers for emerging leafcutting bees, Megachile rotundata. J. Exp. Biol. 221:jeb175406. doi: 10.1242/jeb.175406
Bertolini, E., Kistenpfennig, C., Menegazzi, P., Keller, A., Koukidou, M., and Helfrich-Förster, C. (2018). The characterization of the circadian clock in the olive fly Bactrocera oleae (Diptera: Tephritidae) reveals a Drosophila-like organization. Sci. Rep. 8:816. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-24850-w
Bertolini, E., Schubert, F. K., Zanini, D., Sehadová, H., Helfrich-Förster, C., and Menegazzi, P. (2019). Life at high latitudes does not require circadian behavioral rhythmicity under constant darkness. Curr. Biol. 29, 3928–3936.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.09.032
Bloch, G. (2010). The social clock of the honeybee. J. Biol. Rhythms 25, 307–317. doi: 10.1177/0748730410380149
Bloch, G., Bar-Shai, N., Cytter, Y., and Green, R. (2017). Time is honey: circadian clocks of bees and flowers and how their interactions may influence ecological communities. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 372:20160256. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0256
Bloch, G., and Grozinger, C. M. (2011). Social molecular pathways and the evolution of bee societies. Philos. Trans. Roy. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 366, 2155–2170. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0346
Bloch, G., and Robinson, G. E. (2001). Chronobiology: reversal of honey bee behavioural rhythms. Nat. Brief Commun. 410:1048. doi: 10.1038/35074183
Bloch, G., Solomon, S. M., Robinson, G. E., and Fahrbach, S. E. (2003). Patterns of PERIOD and pigment-dispersing hormone immunoreactivity in the brain of the European honeybee (Apis mellifera): age- and time-related plasticity. J. Comp. Neurol. 464, 269–284. doi: 10.1002/cne.10778
Bloch, G., Toma, D. P., and Robinson, G. E. (2001). Behavioral rhythmicity, age, division of labor and period expression in the honey bee brain. J. Biol. Rhythms 16, 444–456. doi: 10.1177/074873001129002123
Bloch, G. (2009). “Plasticity in the circadian clock and temporal organization of insect societies,” in Organization of Insect Societies: From Genome to Sociocomplexity, eds J. Gadau and J. Fewell (Cambridge: Harvard University Press), 402–432.
Brancaccio, M., Patton, A. P., Chesham, J. E., Maywood, E. S., and Hastings, M. H. (2017). Astrocytes control circadian timekeeping in the suprachiasmatic nucleus via glutamatergic signaling. Neuron 93, 1420–1435.e5. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.02.030
Brown, S. A., Kowalska, E., and Dallmann, R. (2012). (Re)inventing the circadian feedback loop. Dev. Cell 22, 477–487. doi: 10.1016/j.devcel.2012.02.007
Bünning, E. (1936). Die endogene Tagesrhythmik als Grundlage der Photoperiodischen Reaktion (Endogenous daily rhythms as the basis of photoperiodism). Berichte Der Deutschen Botanischen Gesellschaft 54, 590–607.
Busza, A., Murad, A., and Emery, P. (2007). Interactions between circadian neurons control temperature synchronization of Drosophila behavior. J. Neurosci. 27, 10722–10733. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2479-07.2007
Cavanaugh, D. J., Geratowski, J. D., Wooltorton, J. R. A., Spaethling, J. M., Hector, C. E., Zheng, X., et al. (2014). Identification of a circadian output circuit for rest: activity rhythms in Drosophila. Cell 157, 689–701. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2014.02.024
Cavey, M., Collins, B., Bertet, C., and Blau, J. (2016). Circadian rhythms in neuronal activity propagate through output circuits. Nat. Neurosci. 19, 587–595. doi: 10.1038/nn.4263
Ceriani, M. F., Darlington, T. K., Staknis, D., Más, P., Petti, A. A., Weitz, C. J., et al. (1999). Light-dependent sequestration of timeless by cryptochrome. Science 285, 553–556. doi: 10.1126/science.285.5427.553
Chahad-Ehlers, S. (2017). Expanding the view of Clock and cycle gene evolution in Diptera. Insect. Mol. Biol. v 26, 317–331. doi: 10.1111/imb.12296
Chang, D. C., McWatters, H. G., Williams, J. A., Gotter, A. L., Levine, J. D., and Reppert, S. M. (2003). Constructing a feedback loop with circadian clock molecules from the silkmoth, Antheraea pernyi. J. Biol. Chem. 278, 38149–38158. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M306937200
Chatterjee, A., Lamaze, A., De, J., Mena, W., Chélot, E., Martin, B., et al. (2018). Reconfiguration of a multi-oscillator network by light in the Drosophila circadian clock. Curr. Biol. 28, 2007–2017.e4. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.04.064
Cheeseman, J. F., Millar, C. D., Greggers, U., Lehmann, K., Pawley, M. D., Gallistel, C. R., et al. (2014). Way-finding in displaced clock-shifted bees proves bees use a cognitive map. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 111, 8949–8954. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1408039111
Cheeseman, J. F., Winnebeck, E. C., Millar, C. D., Kirkland, L. S., Sleigh, J., Goodwin, M., et al. (2012). General anesthesia alters time perception by phase shifting the circadian clock. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 109, 7061–7066. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1201734109
Choi, C., Cao, G., Tanenhaus, A. K., McCarthy, E., v., Jung, M., Schleyer, W., et al. (2012). Autoreceptor control of peptide/neurotransmitter corelease from PDF neurons determines allocation of circadian activity in Drosophila. Cell Rep. 2, 332–344. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2012.06.021
Chouhan, N. S., Wolf, R., Helfrich-Förster, C., and Heisenberg, M. (2015). Flies remember the time of day. Curr. Biol. 25, 1619–1624. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.04.032
Codd, V., Dolezel, D., Stehlik, J., Piccin, A., Garner, K. J., Racey, S. N., et al. (2007). Circadian rhythm gene regulation in the housefly Musca domestica. Genetics 177, 1539–1751. doi: 10.1534/genetics.107.079160
Cortés, T., Ortiz-Rivas, B., and Martínez-Torres, D. (2010). Identification and characterization of circadian clock genes in the pea aphid Acyrthosiphon pisum: aphid clock genes. Insect. Mol. Biol. 2, 123–139. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2583.2009.00931.x
Crailsheim, K., Hrassnigg, N., and Stabentheiner, A. (1996). Diurnal behavioural differences in forager and nurse honey bees (Apis mellifera carnica Pollm). Apidologie 27, 235–244. doi: 10.1051/apido:19960406
Currie, J., Goda, T., and Wijnen, H. (2009). Selective entrainment of the Drosophila circadian clock to daily gradients in environmental temperature. BMC Biol. 7:49. doi: 10.1186/1741-7007-7-49
Cyran, S. A., Buchsbaum, A. M., Reddy, K. L., Lin, M.-C., Glossop, N. R., Hardin, P. E., et al. (2003). vrille, Pdp1, and dClock form a second feedback loop in the Drosophila circadian clock. Cell 112, 329–341. doi: 10.1016/S0092-8674(03)00074-6
Dalla Benetta, E., Beukeboom, L. W., and van de Zande, L. (2019). Adaptive differences in circadian clock gene expression patterns and photoperiodic diapause induction in Nasonia vitripennis. Am. Nat. 193, 881–896. doi: 10.1086/703159
Danbara, Y., Sakamoto, T., Uryu, O., and Tomioka, K. (2010). RNA interference of timeless gene does not disrupt circadian locomotor rhythms in the cricket Gryllus bimaculatus. J. Insect. Physiol. 56, 1738–1745. doi: 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2010.07.002
Davies, N. J., and Tauber, E. (2016). Deep sequencing analysis of the circadian transcriptome of the jewel wasp Nasonia vitripennis. bioRxiv [Preprint]. doi: 10.1101/048736
Denlinger, D. L. (1986). Dormancy in tropical insects. Ann. Rev. Entomol. 31, 329–364. doi: 10.1146/annurev.en.31.010186.001323
Denlinger, D. L., Hahn, D. A., Merlin, C., Holzapfel, C. M., and Bradshaw, W. E. (2017). Keeping time without a spine: what can the insect clock teach us about seasonal adaptation? Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 372:20160257. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0257
Donlea, J. M. (2019). Roles for sleep in memory: insights from the fly. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 54, 120–126. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2018.10.006
Dreyer, A. P., Martin, M. M., Fulgham, C. V., Jabr, D. A., Bai, L., Beshel, J., et al. (2019). A circadian output center controlling feeding:fasting rhythms in Drosophila. PLoS Genet. 15:e1008478. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1008478
Dubowy, C., and Sehgal, A. (2017). Circadian rhythms and sleep in Drosophila melanogaster. Genetics 205, 1373–1397. doi: 10.1534/genetics.115.185157
Eban-Rothschild, A., Belluci, S., and Bloch, G. (2011). Maternity-related plasticity in circadian rhythms of bumble-bee queens. Proc. Royal Soc. B Biol. Sci. 278, 3510–3516. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2011.0579
Eban-Rothschild, A., and Bloch, G. (2012). “Social influences on circadian rhythms and sleep in insects,” in Advances in Genetics, eds M. B. Sokolowski and S. F. Goodwin (Elsevier), 1–32. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-387687-4.00001-5
Eban-Rothschild, A., Shemesh, Y., and Bloch, G. (2012). The colony environment, but not direct contact with conspecifics, influences the development of circadian rhythms in honey bees. J. Biol. Rhythms 27, 217–225. doi: 10.1177/0748730412440851
Eban-Rothschild, A. D., and Bloch, G. (2008). Differences in the sleep architecture of forager and young honeybees (Apis mellifera). J. Exp. Biol. 211, 2408–2416. doi: 10.1242/jeb.016915
Emery, P., and Reppert, S. M. (2004). A rhythmic ror. Neuron 43, 443–446. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.08.009
Emery, P., So, W. V., Kaneko, M., Hall, J. C., and Rosbash, M. (1998). CRY, a Drosophila clock and light-regulated cryptochrome, is a major contributor to circadian rhythm resetting and photosensitivity. Cell 95, 669–679. doi: 10.1016/S0092-8674(00)81637-2
Fent, K., and Wehner, R. (1985). Ocelli: a celestial compass in the desert ant Cataglyphis. Science 228, 192–194. doi: 10.1126/science.228.4696.192
Fernández, M. P., Berni, J., and Ceriani, M. F. (2008). Circadian remodeling of neuronal circuits involved in rhythmic behavior. PLoS Biol. 6:e69. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0060069
Free, J. B., Ferguson, A. W., and Simpkins, J. R. (1992). The behaviour of queen honeybees and their attendants. Physiol. Entomol. 17, 43–55. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-3032.1992.tb00988.x
Frisch, B., and Aschoff, J. (1987). Circadian rhythms in honeybees: entainment by feeding cycles. Physiol. Entomol. 12, 41–49. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-3032.1987.tb00722.x
Frisch, B., and Koeniger, N. (1994). Social synchronization of the activity rhythms of honeybees within a colony. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 35, 91–98. doi: 10.1007/BF00171498
Frisch, K. v., and Lindauer, M. (1954). Himmel und erde in konkurrenz bei der orientierung der bienen. Naturwissenschaften 41, 245–253. doi: 10.1007/BF00634944
Fuchikawa, T., Beer, K., Linke-Winnebeck, C., Ben-David, R., Kotowoy, A., Tsang, V. W. K., et al. (2017). Neuronal circadian clock protein oscillations are similar in behaviourally rhythmic forager honeybees and in arrhythmic nurses. Open Biol. 7:170047. doi: 10.1098/rsob.170047
Fuchikawa, T., Eban-Rothschild, A., Nagari, M., Shemesh, Y., and Bloch, G. (2016). Potent social synchronization can override photic entrainment of circadian rhythms. Nat. Commun. 7:11662. doi: 10.1038/ncomms11662
Fujii, S., and Amrein, H. (2010). Ventral lateral and DN1 clock neurons mediate distinct properties of male sex drive rhythm in Drosophila. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 107, 10590–10595. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0912457107
Fujii, S., Emery, P., and Amrein, H. (2017). SIK3–HDAC4 signaling regulates Drosophila circadian male sex drive rhythm via modulating the DN1 clock neurons. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 114, E6669–E6677. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1620483114
Fujii, S., Krishnan, P., Hardin, P., and Amrein, H. (2007). Nocturnal male sex drive in Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 17, 244–251. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.11.049
Fujioka, H., Abe, M. S., Fuchikawa, T., Tsuji, K., Shimada, M., and Okada, Y. (2017). Ant circadian activity associated with brood care type. Biol. Lett. 13:20160743. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2016.0743
Galizia, C. G., Eisenhardt, D., and Giurfa, M. (2011). Honeybee neurobiology and behavior: a tribute to Randolf Menzel (Dordrecht: Springer). doi: 10.1007/978-94-007-2099-2
Gentile, C., Rivas, G. B. S., Meireles-Filho, A. C. A., Lima, J. B. P., and Peixoto, A. A. (2009). Circadian expression of clock genes in two mosquito disease vectors: cry2 is different. J. Biol. Rhythms 24, 444–451. doi: 10.1177/0748730409349169
Gentile, C., Sehadova, H., Simoni, A., Chen, C., and Stanewsky, R. (2013). Cryptochrome antagonizes synchronization of Drosophila's circadian clock to temperature cycles. Curr. Biol. 23, 185–195. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.12.023
Gesto, J. S. M., Rivas, G. B., da, S., Pavan, M. G., Meireles-Filho, A. C. A., Amoretty, P.R. de, Souza, N. A., et al. (2015). Clocks do not tick in unison: isolation of Clock and vrille shed new light on the clockwork model of the sand fly Lutzomyia longipalpis. Parasites Vectors 8:505. doi: 10.1186/s13071-015-1117-6
Giraldo, Y. M., Leitch, K. J., Ros, I. G., Warren, T. L., Weir, P. T., and Dickinson, M. H. (2018). Sun navigation requires compass neurons in Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 28, 2845–2852.e4. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.07.002
Gotter, A. L. (2006). A timeless debate: resolving TIM's noncircadian roles with possible clock function. NeuroReport 17, 1229–1233. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000233092.90160.92
Gotter, A. L., Manganaro, T., Weaver, D. R., Kolakowski, L. F., Possidente, B., Sriram, S., et al. (2000). A time-less function for mouse timeless. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 755–756. doi: 10.1038/77653
Gottlieb, D., Keasar, T., Shmida, A., and Motro, U. (2005). Possible foraging benefits of bimodal daily activity in Proxylocopa olivieri (Lepeletier) (Hymenoptera: Anthophoridae). Environ. Entomol. 34, 417–424. doi: 10.1603/0046-225X-34.2.417
Green, E. W., O'Callaghan, E. K., Hansen, C. N., Bastianello, S., Bhutani, S., Vanin, S., et al. (2015). Drosophila circadian rhythms in seminatural environments: summer afternoon component is not an artifact and requires TrpA1 channels. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 112, 8702–8707. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1506093112
Gunawardhana, K. L., and Hardin, P. E. (2017). VRILLE controls PDF neuropeptide accumulation and arborization rhythms in small ventrolateral neurons to drive rhythmic behavior in Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 27, 3442–3453.e4. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.10.010
Guo, F., Cerullo, I., Chen, X., and Rosbash, M. (2014). PDF neuron firing phase-shifts key circadian activity neurons in Drosophila. Elife 3:e02780. doi: 10.7554/eLife.02780.014
Guo, F., Holla, M., Díaz, M. M., and Rosbash, M. (2018). A circadian output circuit controls sleep-wake arousal in Drosophila. Neuron 100, 624–635.e4. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.09.002
Hackman, W., Lakvaara, S., Saura, A., Sorsa, M., and Vepsalainen, K. (1970). On the biology and karyology of Chymomyza costata Zetterstedt, with reference to the taxonomy and distribution of various species of Chymomyza (Dipt., Drosophilidae). Ann. Entom. Fenn. 36, 1–9.
Hall, J. C. (2003). Genetics and Molecular Biology of Rhythms in Drosophila and Other Insects, Vol. 48. Waltham, MA: Gulf Professional Publishing; Brandeis University.
Harano, K., Sasaki, M., and Sasaki, K. (2007). Effects of reproductive state on rhythmicity, locomotor activity and body weight in the European honeybee, Apis mellifera queens (Hymenoptera, Apini). Sociobiology 50, 189–200.
Hardin, P. E., and Panda, S. (2013). Circadian timekeeping and output mechanisms in animals. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 23, 724–731. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2013.02.018
Hardin, P. E. (2011). “Chapter 5—Molecular genetic analysis of circadian timekeeping in Drosophila,” in The Genetics of Circadian Rhythms, 141–173. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-387690-4.00005-2
Harper, R. E. F., Dayan, P., Albert, J. T., and Stanewsky, R. (2016). Sensory conflict disrupts activity of the Drosophila circadian network. Cell Rep. 17, 1711–1718. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2016.10.029
Hassaneen, E., El-Din Sallam, A., Abo-Ghalia, A., Moriyama, Y., Karpova, S. G., Abdelsalam, S., et al. (2011). Pigment-dispersing factor affects nocturnal activity rhythms, photic entrainment, and the free-running period of the circadian clock in the cricket Gryllus bimaculatus. J. Biol. Rhythms 26, 3–13. doi: 10.1177/0748730410388746
Helfrich-Förster, C. (1995). The period clock gene is expressed in central nervous system neurons which also produce a neuropeptide that reveals the projections of circadian pacemaker cells within the brain of Drosophila melanogaster. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 92, 612–616. doi: 10.1073/pnas.92.2.612
Helfrich-Förster, C. (2000). Differential control of morning and evening components in the activity rhythm of Drosophila melanogaster—sex-specific differences suggest a different quality of activity. J. Biol. Rhythms 15, 135–154. doi: 10.1177/074873040001500208
Helfrich-Förster, C. (2014). From neurogenetic studies in the fly brain to a concept in circadian biology. J. Neurogenet. 28, 329–347. doi: 10.3109/01677063.2014.905556
Helfrich-Förster, C. (2017). “The drosophila clock system,” in Biological Timekeeping: Clocks, Rhythms and Behaviour, ed V. Kumar (New Delhi: Springer India), 133–176. doi: 10.1007/978-81-322-3688-7_6
Helfrich-Förster, C. (2020). Light input pathways to the circadian clock of insects with an emphasis on the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster. J. Comp. Physiol. A 206, 259–272. doi: 10.1007/s00359-019-01379-5
Helfrich-Förster, C., Bertolini, E., and Menegazzi, P. (2018). Flies as models for circadian clock adaptation to environmental challenges. Eur. J. Neurosci. 51, 166–181. doi: 10.1111/ejn.14180
Helfrich-Förster, C., Shafer, O. T., Wülbeck, C., Grieshaber, E., Rieger, D., and Taghert, P. (2007b). Development and morphology of the clock-gene-expressing lateral neurons of Drosophila melanogaster. J. Comp. Neurol. 500, 47–70. doi: 10.1002/cne.21146
Helfrich-Förster, C., Stengl, M., and Homberg, U. (1998). Organization of the circadian system in insects. Chronobiol. Int. 15, 567–594. doi: 10.3109/07420529808993195
Helfrich-Förster, C., Täuber, M., Park, J. H., Mühlig-Versen, M., Schneuwly, S., and Hofbauer, A. (2000). Ectopic expression of the neuropeptide pigment-dispersing factor alters behavioral rhythms in Drosophila melanogaster. J. Neurosci. 20, 3339–3353. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-09-03339.2000
Helfrich-Förster, C., Yoshii, T., Wülbeck, C., Grieshaber, E., Rieger, D., Bachleitner, W., et al. (2007a). “The lateral and dorsal neurons of Drosophila melanogaster: new insights about their morphology and function,” Cold Spring Harbor Symposia on Quantitative Biology, Vol. 72 (Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press). doi: 10.1101/sqb.2007.72.063
Hermann, C., Saccon, R., Senthilan, P. R., Domnik, L., Dircksen, H., Yoshii, T., et al. (2013). The circadian clock network in the brain of different Drosophila species. J. Comp. Neurol. 521, 367–388. doi: 10.1002/cne.23178
Hermann-Luibl, C., and Helfrich-Förster, C. (2015). Clock network in Drosophila. Curr. Opin. Insect. Sci. 7, 65–70. doi: 10.1016/j.cois.2014.11.003
Homberg, U. (2015). Sky compass orientation in desert locusts—evidence from field and laboratory studies. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9:346. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00346
Homberg, U., Heinze, S., Pfeiffer, K., Kinoshita, M., and el Jundi, B. (2011). Central neural coding of sky polarization in insects. Philos. Trans. Roy. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 366, 680–687. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0199
Homberg, U., Würden, S., Dircksen, H., and Rao, K. R. (1991). Comparative anatomy of pigment-dispersing hormone-immunoreactive neurons in the brain of orthopteroid insects. Cell Tissue Res. 266, 343–357. doi: 10.1007/BF00318190
Hsu, C. T., and Sehgal, A. (2018). Circadian and sleep circuits ring together. Neuron 100, 514–516. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.10.030
Huang, X., Poelchau, M. F., and Armbruster, P. A. (2015). Global transcriptional dynamics of diapause induction in non-blood-fed and blood-fed Aedes albopictus. PLoS Negl. Trop. Dis. 9, 1–28. doi: 10.1371/journal.pntd.0003724
Hussaini, S. A., Bogusch, L., Landgraf, T., and Menzel, R. (2009). Sleep deprivation affects extinction but not acquisition memory in honeybees. Learn. Memory 16, 698–705. doi: 10.1101/lm.1578409
Ikeno, T., Numata, H., and Goto, S. G. (2011a). Circadian clock genes period and cycle regulate photoperiodic diapause in the bean bug Riptortus pedestris males. J. Insect. Physiol. 57, 935–938. doi: 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2011.04.006
Ikeno, T., Numata, H., and Goto, S. G. (2011b). Photoperiodic response requires mammalian-type cryptochrome in the bean bug Riptortus pedestris. Biochem. Biophys. Res. Commun. 410, 394–397. doi: 10.1016/j.bbrc.2011.05.142
Ikeno, T., Numata, H., Goto, S. G., and Shiga, S. (2014). Involvement of the brain region containing pigment-dispersing factor-immunoreactive neurons in the photoperiodic response of the bean bug, Riptortus pedestris. J. Exp. Biol. 217, 453–462. doi: 10.1242/jeb.091801
Ikeno, T., Tanaka, S. I., Numata, H., and Goto, S. G. (2010). Photoperiodic diapause under the control of circadian clock genes in an insect. BMC Biol. 8:116. doi: 10.1186/1741-7007-8-116
Im, S. H., and Taghert, P. H. (2010). PDF receptor expression reveals direct interactions between circadian oscillators in Drosophila. J. Comp. Neurol. 518, 1925–1945. doi: 10.1002/cne.22311
Ingram, K. K., Krummey, S., and LeRoux, M. (2009). Expression patterns of a circadian clock gene are associated with age-related polyethism in harvester ants, Pogonomyrmex occidentalis. BMC Ecol. 9:7. doi: 10.1186/1472-6785-9-7
Ingram, K. K., Kutowoi, A., Wurm, Y., Shoemaker, D., Meier, R., and Bloch, G. (2012). The molecular clockwork of the fire ant Solenopsis invicta. PLoS ONE 7:e45715. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0045715
Iwai, S., Fukui, Y., Fujiwara, Y., and Takeda, M. (2006). Structure and expressions of two circadian clock genes, period and timeless in the commercial silkmoth, Bombyx mori. J. Insect Physiol. 52, 625–637. doi: 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2006.03.001
Jackson, F. R., Ng, F. S., Sengupta, S., You, S., and Huang, Y. (2015). Glial cell regulation of rhythmic behavior. Methods Enzymol. 552, 45–73. doi: 10.1016/bs.mie.2014.10.016
Jain, R., and Brockmann, A. (2018). Time-restricted foraging under natural light/dark condition shifts the molecular clock in the honey bee, Apis mellifera. Chronobiol. Int. 1723–1734. doi: 10.1080/07420528.2018.1509867
Janssen, T., Husson, S. J., Lindemans, M., Mertens, I., Rademakers, S., Donck, K. V., et al. (2008). Functional characterization of three G protein-coupled receptors for pigment dispersing factors in Caenorhabditis elegans. J. Biol. Chem. 283, 15241–15249. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M709060200
Jaumouillé, E., Almeida, P. M., Stähli, P., Koch, R., and Nagoshi, E. (2015). Transcriptional regulation via nuclear receptor crosstalk required for the Drosophila circadian clock. Curr. Biol. 25, 189–200. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.04.017
Johnson, J. N., Hardgrave, E., Gill, C., and Moore, D. (2010). Absence of consistent diel rhythmicity in mated honey bee queen behavior. J. Insect. Physiol. 56, 761–773. doi: 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2010.01.004
Kadener, S., Stoleru, D., McDonald, M., Nawathean, P., and Rosbash, M. (2007). Clockwork Orange is a transcriptional repressor and a new Drosophila circadian pacemaker component. Genes Dev. 21, 1675–1686. doi: 10.1101/gad.1552607
Kaiser, T. S., Poehn, B., Szkiba, D., Preussner, M., Sedlazeck, F. J., Zrim, A., et al. (2016). The genomic basis of circadian and circalunar timing adaptations in a midge. Nature 540, 69–73. doi: 10.1038/nature20151
Kaiser, W. (1988). Busy bees need rest, too: behavioural and electromyographical sleep signs in honeybees. J. Comp. Physiol. 163, 565–584. doi: 10.1007/BF00603841
Kauranen, H., Menegazzi, P., Costa, R., Helfrich-Förster, C., Kankainen, A., and Hoikkala, A. (2012). Flies in the North: locomotor behavior and clock neuron organization of Drosophila montana. J. Biol. Rhythms 27, 377–387. doi: 10.1177/0748730412455916
Kay, J., Menegazzi, P., Mildner, S., Roces, F., and Helfrich-Förster, C. (2018). The circadian clock of the ant Camponotus floridanus is localized in dorsal and lateral neurons of the brain. J. Biol. Rhythms 33, 255–271. doi: 10.1177/0748730418764738
Kefuss, J. A. (1978). Influence of photoperiod on the behaviour and brood-rearing activities of honeybees in a flight room. J. Apicult. Res. 17, 137–151. doi: 10.1080/00218839.1978.11099919
King, A. N., and Sehgal, A. (2020). Molecular and circuit mechanisms mediating circadian clock output in the Drosophila brain. Eur. J. Neurosci. 51, 268–281. doi: 10.1111/ejn.14092
Kleber, E. (1935). Hat das Zeitgedächtnis der bienen biologische Bedeutung? Zeitschrift Für Vergleichende Physiologie 22, 221–262. doi: 10.1007/BF00586500
Klein, B. A., Klein, A., Wray, M. K., Mueller, U. G., and Seeley, T. D. (2010). Sleep deprivation impairs precision of waggle dance signaling in honey bees. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 107, 22705–22709. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1009439108
Klein, B. A., Olzsowy, K. M., Klein, A., Saunders, K. M., and Seeley, T. D. (2008). Caste-dependent sleep of worker honey bees. J. Exp. Biol. 211, 3028–3040. doi: 10.1242/jeb.017426
Klein, B. A., Stiegler, M., Klein, A., and Tautz, J. (2014). Mapping sleeping bees within their nest: spatial and temporal analysis of worker honey bee sleep. PLoS ONE 9:e102316. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0102316
Klose, M., Duvall, L. B., Li, W., Liang, X., Ren, C., Steinbach, J. H., et al. (2016). Functional PDF signaling in the Drosophila circadian neural circuit is gated by Ral A-dependent modulation. Neuron 90, 781–794. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.04.002
Kobelková, A., Závodská, R., Sauman, I., Bazalová, O., and Dolezel, D. (2015). Expression of clock genes period and timeless in the central nervous system of the mediterranean flour moth, Ephestia kuehniella. J. Biol. Rhythms 30, 104–116. doi: 10.1177/0748730414568430
Koeniger, N., and Koeniger, G. (2000). Reproductive isolation among species of the genus Apis. Apidol. 31, 313–339. doi: 10.1051/apido:2000125
Koltermann, R. (1971). 24-Std-periodik in der Langzeiterinnerung an Duft- und Farbsignale bei der Honigbiene. Z. vergl. Physiol. 75, 49–68. doi: 10.1007/BF00335137
Komada, S., Kamae, Y., Koyanagi, M., Tatewaki, K., Hassaneen, E., Saifullah, A. S. M., et al. (2015). Green-sensitive opsin is the photoreceptor for photic entrainment of an insect circadian clock. Zool. Lett. 1:11. doi: 10.1186/s40851-015-0011-6
Konopka, R. J., and Benzer, S. (1971). Clock mutants of Drosophila melanogaster. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 68, 2112–2116. doi: 10.1073/pnas.68.9.2112
Koštál, V. (2006). Eco-physiological phases of insect diapause. J. Insect. Physiol. 52, 113–127. doi: 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2005.09.008
Kotwica-Rolinska, J., Chodakova, L., Chvalova, D., Kristofova, L., Fenclova, I., Provaznik, J., et al. (2019). CRISPR/Cas9 genome editing introduction and optimization in the non-model insect Pyrrhocoris apterus. Front. Physiol. 10:891. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2019.00891
Kotwica-Rolinska, J., Pivarciova, L., Vaneckova, H., and Dolezel, D. (2017). The role of circadian clock genes in the photoperiodic timer of the linden bug Pyrrhocoris apterus during the nymphal stage: nymphal photoperiodic timer. Physiol. Entomol. 42, 266–273. doi: 10.1111/phen.12197
Krell-Westerwalbesloh, S., Krell, F.-T., and Eduard Linsenmair, K. (2004). Diel separation of Afrotropical dung beetle guilds—avoiding competition and neglecting resources (Coleoptera: Scarabaeoidea). J. Nat. Hist. 38, 2225–2249. doi: 10.1080/00222930310001618921
Krupp, J. J., Billeter, J.-C., Wong, A., Choi, C., Nitabach, M. N., and Levine, J. D. (2013). Pigment-dispersing factor modulates pheromone production in clock cells that influence mating in Drosophila. Neuron 79, 54–68. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.05.019
Krupp, J. J., Kent, C., Billeter, J.-C., Azanchi, R., So, A. K.-C., Schonfeld, J. A., et al. (2008). Social experience modifies pheromone expression and mating behavior in male Drosophila melanogaster. Curr. Biol. 18, 1373–1383. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.07.089
Kumar, S., Chen, D., Jang, C., Nall, A., Zheng, X., and Sehgal, A. (2014). An ecdysone-responsive nuclear receptor regulates circadian rhythms in Drosophila. Nat. Commun. 5:5697. doi: 10.1038/ncomms6697
Kutaragi, Y., Tokuoka, A., Tomiyama, Y., Nose, M., Watanabe, T., Bando, T., et al. (2018). A novel photic entrainment mechanism for the circadian clock in an insect: involvement of c-fos and cryptochromes. Zool. Lett. 4:26. doi: 10.1186/s40851-018-0109-8
Kyriacou, C. P. (2014a). Sex and rhythms in sandflies and mosquitoes: an appreciation of the work of alexandre afranio peixoto (1963–2013). Infect. Genet. Evol. 28, 662–665. doi: 10.1016/j.meegid.2014.06.016
Kyriacou, C. P. (2014b). “Functional analysis of natural clock gene variation,” in Behavioral Genetics of the Fly, ed J. Dubnau (New York, NY: Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory; Cambridge University Press), 183–191.
Lam, V. H., and Chiu, J. C. (2019). “Evolution and design of invertebrate circadian clocks,” in The Oxford Handbook of Invertebrate Neurobiology, ed J.H. Byrne (Oxford University Press), 594–614. doi: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780190456757.013.25
Lamaze, A., Krätschmer, P., Chen, K.-F., Lowe, S., and Jepson, J. E. C. (2018). A wake-promoting circadian output circuit in Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 28, 3098–3105.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.07.024
Lamaze, A., and Stanewsky, R. (2020). DN1p or the “fluffy” cerberus of clock outputs. Front. Physiol. 10:1540. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2019.01540
Lee, C.-M., Su, M.-T., and Lee, H.-J. (2009). Pigment dispersing factor: an output regulator of the circadian clock in the German cockroach. J. Biol. Rhythms 24, 35–43. doi: 10.1177/0748730408327909
Lehmann, M., Gustav, D., and Galizia, C. G. (2011). The early bee catches the flower - circadian rhythmicity influences learning performance in honey bees, Apis mellifera. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 65, 205–215. doi: 10.1007/s00265-010-1026-9
Lensky, Y., and Demter, M. (1985). Mating flights of the queen honeybee (Apis mellifera) in a subtropical climate. Comp. Physiol. A Physiol. 81, 229–241. doi: 10.1016/0300-9629(85)90127-6
Levine, J. D., Funes, P., Dowse, H. B., and Hall, J. C. (2002). Resetting the circadian clock by social experience in Drosophila melanogaster. Science 298, 2010–2012. doi: 10.1126/science.1076008
Li, C.-J., Yun, X.-P., Yu, X.-J., and Li, B. (2018). Functional analysis of the circadian clock gene timeless in Tribolium castaneum: functions of timeless in Tribolium castaneum. Insect. Science 25, 418–428. doi: 10.1111/1744-7917.12441
Li, M.-T., Cao, L.-H., Xiao, N., Tang, M., Deng, B., Yang, T., et al. (2018). Hub-organized parallel circuits of central circadian pacemaker neurons for visual photoentrainment in Drosophila. Nat. Commun. 9:4247. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-06506-5
Li, X. (2011). Timeless is a critical gene in the diapause of silkworm, Bombyx mori. Afr. J. Biotechnol. 10:2522. doi: 10.5897/AJB10.2522
Liang, X., Ho, M. C. W., Zhang, Y., Li, Y., Wu, M. N., Holy, T. E., et al. (2019). Morning and evening circadian pacemakers independently drive premotor centers via a specific dopamine relay. Neuron 102, 843–857.e4. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.03.028
Lim, C., Chung, B. Y., Pitman, J. L., McGill, J. J., Pradhan, S., Lee, J., et al. (2007b). clockwork orange encodes a transcriptional repressor important for circadian-clock amplitude in Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 17, 1082–1089. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.05.039
Lim, C., Lee, J., Koo, E., and Choe, J. (2007a). Targeted inhibition of Pdp1ε abolishes the circadian behavior of Drosophila melanogaster. Biochem. Biophys. Res. Commun. 364, 294–300. doi: 10.1016/j.bbrc.2007.10.009
Lin, Q.-C., Zhai, Y.-F., Zhou, C.-G., Li, L.-L., Zhuang, Q.-Y., Zhang, X.-Y., et al. (2014). Behavioral rhythms of Drosophila suzukii and Drosophila melanogaster. Florida Entomologist 97, 1424–1433. doi: 10.1653/024.097.0417
Lin, Y., Stormo, G. D., and Taghert, P. H. (2004). The neuropeptide pigment-dispersing factor coordinates pacemaker interactions in the Drosophila circadian system. J. Neurosci. 24, 7951–7957. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2370-04.2004
Lindauer, M. (1960). Time-compensated sun orientation in bees. Cold Spring Harbor Symposia on Quant. Biol. 25, 371–377. doi: 10.1101/SQB.1960.025.01.039
Liu, X., Blaženović, I., Contreras, A. J., Pham„ T. M., Tabuloc, C. A., Li, Y. H., et al. Hexosamine biosynthetic pathway integrates circadian metabolic signals to regulate daily rhythms in protein O-linked N-acetylglucosaminylation. bioRxiv [Preprint] (2020). doi: 10.1101/2020.08.18.256636
Manjunatha, T., Dass, S. H., and Sharma, V. K. (2008). Egg-laying rhythm in Drosophila melanogaster. J. Genet. 87, 495–504. doi: 10.1007/s12041-008-0072-9
Martelli, C., Pech, U., Kobbenbring, S., Pauls, D., Bahl, B., Sommer, M. V., et al. (2017). SIFamide translates hunger signals into appetitive and feeding behavior in Drosophila. Cell Rep. 20, 464–478. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2017.06.043
Mathejczyk, T. F., and Wernet, M. F. (2019). Heading choices of flying Drosophila under changing angles of polarized light. Sci. Rep. 9:16773. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-53330-y
Matsumoto, A., Ukai-Tadenuma, M., Yamada, R. G., Houl, J., Uno, K. D., Kasukawa, T., et al. (2007). A functional genomics strategy reveals clockwork orange as a transcriptional regulator in the Drosophila circadian clock. Genes Dev. 21, 1687–1700. doi: 10.1101/gad.1552207
McCabe, C., and Birley, A. (1998). Oviposition in the Period genotypes of Drosophila Melanogaster. Chronobiol. Int. 15, 119–133. doi: 10.3109/07420529808998677
Medugorac, I., and Lindauer, M. (1967). Das Zeitgedächtnis der Bienen unter dem Einfluß von Narkose und von sozialen Zeitgebern. J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol. 55, 450–474. doi: 10.1007/BF00302625
Meireles-Filho, A. C., Amoretty, P. R., Souza, N. A., Kyriacou, C. P., and Peixoto, A. A. (2006a). Rhythmic expression of the cycle gene in a hematophagous insect vector. BMC Mol. Biol. 7:38. doi: 10.1186/1471-2199-7-38
Meireles-Filho, A. C. A., da, S., Rivas, G. B., Gesto, J. S. M., Machado, R. C., Britto, C., et al. (2006b). The biological clock of an hematophagous insect: locomotor activity rhythms, circadian expression and downregulation after a blood meal. FEBS Lett. 580, 2–8. doi: 10.1016/j.febslet.2005.11.031
Meireles-Filho, A. C. A., and Kyriacou, C. P. (2013). Circadian rhythms in insect disease vectors. Mem. Inst. Oswaldo Cruz. 108, 48–58. doi: 10.1590/0074-0276130438
Menegazzi, P., Dalla Benetta, E., Beauchamp, M., Schlichting, M., Steffan-Dewenter, I., and Helfrich-Förster, C. (2017). Adaptation of circadian neuronal network to photoperiod in high-latitude European drosophilids. Curr. Biol. 27, 833–839. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.01.036
Merlin, C., Gegear, R. J., and Reppert, S. M. (2009). Antennal circadian clocks coordinate sun compass orientation in migratory monarch butterflies. Science 325, 1700–1704. doi: 10.1126/science.1176221
Merlin, C., Heinze, S., and Reppert, S. M. (2012). Unraveling navigational strategies in migratory insects. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 22, 353–361. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2011.11.009
Meuti, M. E., Stone, M., Ikeno, T., and Denlinger, D. L. (2015). Functional circadian clock genes are essential for the overwintering diapause of the Northern house mosquito, Culex pipiens. J. Exp. Biol. 218, 412–422. doi: 10.1242/jeb.113233
Miyazaki, Y., Watari, Y., Tanaka, K., and Goto, S. G. (2016). Temperature cycle amplitude alters the adult eclosion time and expression pattern of the circadian clock gene period in the onion fly. J. Insect. Physiol. 86, 54–59. doi: 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2016.01.002
Moore, D. (2001). Honey bee circadian clocks: behavioral control from individual workers to whole-colony rhythms. J. Insect. Physiol. 47, 843–857. doi: 10.1016/S0022-1910(01)00057-9
Moore, D., Angel, J. E., Cheeseman, I. M., Fahrbach, S. E., and Robinson, G. E. (1998). Timekeeping in the honey bee colony: integration of circadian rhythms and division of labor. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 43, 147–160. doi: 10.1007/s002650050476
Moore, D., and Doherty, P. (2009). Acquisition of a time-memory in forager honey bees. J. Comp. Physiol. A 195, 741–751. doi: 10.1007/s00359-009-0450-7
Moore, D., and Rankin, M. A. (1985). Circadian locomotor rhythms in individual honeybees. Physiol. Entomol. 10, 191–197. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-3032.1985.tb00034.x
Moore, D., Siegfried, D., Wilson, R., and Rankin, M. A. (1989). The influence of time of day on the foraging behavior of the honeybee, Apis mellifera. J. Biol. Rhythms 4, 305–325. doi: 10.1177/074873048900400301
Moritz, R. F., and Kryger, P. (1994). Self-organization of circadian rhythms in groups of honeybees (Apis mellifera L.). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 34, 211–215. doi: 10.1007/BF00167746
Moritz, R. F., and Sakofski, F. (1991). The role of the queen in circadian rhythms of honeybees (Apis mellifera L.). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 29, 361–365. doi: 10.1007/BF00165961
Moriyama, Y., Kamae, Y., Uryu, O., and Tomioka, K. (2012). Gb'Clock is expressed in the optic lobe and is required for the circadian clock in the cricket Gryllus bimaculatus. J. Biol. Rhythms 27, 467–477. doi: 10.1177/0748730412462207
Moriyama, Y., Sakamoto, T., Karpova, S. G., Matsumoto, A., Noji, S., and Tomioka, K. (2008). RNA interference of the clock gene period disrupts circadian rhythms in the cricket Gryllus bimaculatus. J. Biol. Rhythms 23, 308–318. doi: 10.1177/0748730408320486
Mulder, C. K., Gerkema, M. P., and Van der Zee, E. A. (2013). Circadian clocks and memory: time-place learning. Front. Mol. Neurosci. 6:8. doi: 10.3389/fnmol.2013.00008
Myers, M. P., Wager-Smith, K., Rothenfluh-Hilfiker, A., and Young, M. W. (1996). Light-induced degradation of TIMELESS and entrainment of the Drosophila circadian clock. Science 271, 1736–1740. doi: 10.1126/science.271.5256.1736
Nagari, M., and Bloch, G. (2012). The involvement of the antennae in mediating the brood influence on circadian rhythms in “nurse” honey bee (Apis mellifera) workers. J. Insect. Physiol. 58, 1096–1103. doi: 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2012.05.007
Nagari, M., Brenner, Y., and Bloch, G. (2017a). Nurse honeybee workers tend capped-brood, which does not require feeding, around-the-clock. J. Exp. Biol. 220, 4130–4140. doi: 10.1242/jeb.166884
Nagari, M., Szyszka, P., Galizia, G., and Bloch, G. (2017b). Task-related phasing of circadian rhythms in antennal responsiveness to odorants and pheromones in honeybees. J. Biol. Rhythms 32.6, 593–608. doi: 10.1177/0748730417733573
Nagy, D., Andreatta, G., Bastianello, S., Martín Anduaga, A., Mazzotta, G., Kyriacou, C. P., et al. (2018). A semi-natural approach for studying seasonal diapause in Drosophila melanogaster reveals robust photoperiodicity. J. Biol. Rhythms 33, 117–125. doi: 10.1177/0748730417754116
Nagy, D., Cusumano, P., Andreatta, G., Anduaga, A. M., Hermann-Luibl, C., Reinhard, N., et al. (2019). Peptidergic signaling from clock neurons regulates reproductive dormancy in Drosophila melanogaster. PLoS Genet 15, 1–25. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1008158
Naidoo, N. (1999). A role for the proteasome in the light response of the Timeless clock protein. Science 285, 1737–1741. doi: 10.1126/science.285.5434.1737
Narasaki-Funo, Y., Tomiyama, Y., Nose, M., Bando, T., and Tomioka, K. (2020). Functional analysis of Pdp1 and vrille in the circadian system of a cricket. J. Insect. Physiol. 2020:104156 doi: 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2020.104156
Nässel, D. R., Shiga, S., Mohrherr, C. J., and Rao, K. R. (1993). Pigment-dispersing hormone-like peptide in the nervous system of the flies Phormia and Drosophila: immunocytochemistry and partial characterization. J. Comp. Neurol. 331, 183–198. doi: 10.1002/cne.903310204
Ng, F. S., Tangredi, M. M., and Jackson, F. R. (2011). Glial cells physiologically modulate clock neurons and circadian behavior in a calcium-dependent manner. Curr. Biol. 21, 625–634. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.03.027
Nishiitsutsuji-Uwo, J., and Pittendrigh, C. S. (1968). Central nervous system control of circadian rhythmicity in the cockroach. J. Comp. Neurol. 58, 14–46. doi: 10.1007/BF00302434
Noreen, S., Pegoraro, M., Nouroz, F., Tauber, E., and Kyriacou, C. P. (2018). Interspecific studies of circadian genes period and timeless in Drosophila. Gene 648, 106–114. doi: 10.1016/j.gene.2018.01.020
Nose, M., Tokuoka, A., Bando, T., and Tomioka, K. (2018). timeless2 plays an important role in reproduction and circadian rhythms in the cricket Gryllus bimaculatus. J. Insect. Physiol. 105, 9–17. doi: 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2017.12.007
Ojima, N., Hara, Y., Ito, H., and Yamamoto, D. (2018). Genetic dissection of stress-induced reproductive arrest in Drosophila melanogaster females. PLoS Genetics 14, 1–15. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1007434
Özkaya, Ö., and Rosato, E. (2012). The circadian clock of the fly: a neurogenetics journey through time. Adv. Genet. 77, 79–123. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-387687-4.00004-0
Page, T. L. (1982). Transplantation of the cockroach circadian pacemaker. Science 216:73. doi: 10.1126/science.216.4541.73
Pahl, M., Zhu, H., Pix, W., Tautz, J., and Zhang, S. (2007). Circadian timed episodic-like memory a bee knows what to do when, and also where. J. Exp. Biol. 210, 3559–3567. doi: 10.1242/jeb.005488
Paolucci, S., Dalla Benetta, E., Salis, L., DoleŽel, D., van de Zande, L., and Beukeboom, L. (2019). Latitudinal variation in circadian rhythmicity in Nasonia vitripennis. Behav. Sci. 9:115. doi: 10.3390/bs9110115
Park, J. H., Helfrich-Förster, C., Lee, G., Liu, L., Rosbash, M., and Hall, J. C. (2000). Differential regulation of circadian pacemaker output by separate clock genes in Drosophila. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 97, 3608–3613. doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.7.3608
Pegel, U., Pfeiffer, K., Zittrell, F., Scholtyssek, C., and Homberg, U. (2019). Two compasses in the central complex of the locust brain. J. Neurosci. 39, 3070–3080. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0940-18.2019
Pegoraro, M., Zonato, V., Tyler, E. R., Fedele, G., Kyriacou, C. P., and Tauber, E. (2017). Geographical analysis of diapause inducibility in European Drosophila melanogaster populations. J. Insect Physiol. 98, 238–244. doi: 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2017.01.015
Petri, B., and Stengl, M. (1997). Pigment-dispersing hormone shifts the phase of the circadian pacemaker of the cockroach Leucophaea maderae. J. Neurosci. 17, 4087–4093. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-11-04087.1997
Pírez, N., Bernabei-Cornejo, S. G., Fernandez-Acosta, M., Duhart, J. M., and Ceriani, M. F. (2019). Contribution of non-circadian neurons to the temporal organization of locomotor activity. Biol. Open 8:bio039628. doi: 10.1242/bio.039628
Pírez, N., Christmann, B. L., and Griffith, L. C. (2013). Daily rhythms in locomotor circuits in Drosophila involve PDF. J. Neurophysiol. 110, 700–708. doi: 10.1152/jn.00126.2013
Pittendrigh, C. S. (1954). On temperature independence in the clock system controlling emergence time in Drosophila. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 40, 1018–1029. doi: 10.1073/pnas.40.10.1018
Reischig, T., Petri, B., and Stengl, M. (2004). Pigment-dispersing hormone (PDH)-immunoreactive neurons form a direct coupling pathway between the bilaterally symmetric circadian pacemakers of the cockroach Leucophaea maderae. Cell Tissue Res. 318, 553–564. doi: 10.1007/s00441-004-0927-1
Reischig, T., and Stengl, M. (2003). Ectopic transplantation of the accessory medulla restores circadian locomotor rhythms in arrhythmic cockroaches (Leucophaea maderae). J. Exp. Biol. 206, 1877–1886. doi: 10.1242/jeb.00373
Renn, S. C. P., Park, J. H., Rosbash, M., Hall, J. C., and Taghert, P. H. (1999). A pdf neuropeptide gene mutation and ablation of PDF neurons each cause severe abnormalities of behavioral circadian rhythms in Drosophila. Cell 99, 791–802. doi: 10.1016/S0092-8674(00)81676-1
Reppert, S. M., Guerra, P. A., and Merlin, C. (2016). Neurobiology of monarch butterfly migration. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 61, 25–42. doi: 10.1146/annurev-ento-010814-020855
Reznik, S. (2011). Ecological and evolutionary aspects of photothermal regulation of diapause in Trichogrammatidae. J Evol Biochem Phys 47, 512–523. doi: 10.1134/S0022093011060020
Richier, B., Michard-Vanhée, C., Lamouroux, A., Papin, C., and Rouyer, F. (2008). The Clockwork Orange Drosophila protein functions as both an activator and a repressor of clock gene expression. J. Biol. Rhythms 23, 103–116. doi: 10.1177/0748730407313817
Rivas, G. B. S., Teles-de-Freitas, R., Pavan, M. G., Lima, J. B. P., Peixoto, A. A., and Bruno, R. V. (2018). Effects of light and temperature on daily activity and clock gene expression in two mosquito disease vectors. J. Biol. Rhythms. 33, 272–288. doi: 10.1177/0748730418772175
Rodriguez-Sanchez, I. P., Villanueva-Segura, O. K., Gonzalez-Alvarez, R., Flores-Suarez, A. E., Garza-Rodriguez, M., de, L., et al. (2015). Molecular cloning and characterization of the circadian clock Timeless gene in Culex quinquefasciatus Say (Diptera: Culicidae). South. Entomol. 40, 53–70. doi: 10.3958/059.040.0105
Rodriguez-Zas, S. L., Southey, B. R., Shemesh, Y., Rubin, E. B., Cohen, M., Robinson, G. E., et al. (2012). Microarray analysis of natural socially regulated plasticity in circadian rhythms of honey bees. J. Biol. Rhythms 27, 12–24. doi: 10.1177/0748730411431404
Rooke, R., Rasool, A., Schneider, J., and Levine, J. D. (2020). Drosophila melanogaster behaviour changes in different social environments based on group size and density. Commun. Biol. 3:304. doi: 10.1038/s42003-020-1024-z
Rosato, E., Codd, V., Mazzotta, G., Piccin, A., Zordan, M., Costa, R., et al. (2001). Light-dependent interaction between Drosophila CRY and the clock protein PER mediated by the carboxy terminus of CRY. Curr. Biol. 11, 909–917. doi: 10.1016/S0960-9822(01)00259-7
Rubin, E. B., Shemesh, Y., Cohen, M., Elgavish, S., Robertson, H. M., and Bloch, G. (2006). Molecular and phylogenetic analyses reveal mammalian-like clockwork in the honey bee (Apis mellifera) and shed new light on the molecular evolution of the circadian clock. Genome Res. 16, 1352–1365. doi: 10.1101/gr.5094806
Ruf, F., Mitesser, O., Mungwa, S. T., Horn, M., Rieger, D., Hovestadt, T., et al. (2019). Natural Zeitgebers cannot compensate for the loss of a functional circadian clock in timing of a vital behaviour in Drosophila. Anim. Behav. Cogn. bioRxiv [Preprint]. doi: 10.1101/2019.12.22.886309
Rund, S. S. C., Hou, T. Y., Ward, S. M., Collins, F. H., and Duffield, G. E. (2011). Genome-wide profiling of diel and circadian gene expression in the malaria vector Anopheles gambiae. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 108:E421. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1100584108
Sadd, B. M., Barribeau, S. M., Bloch, G., de Graaf, D. C., Dearden, P., Elsik, C. G., et al. (2015). The genomes of two key bumblebee species with primitive eusocial organization. Genome Biol. 16:76. doi: 10.1186/s13059-015-0623-3
Saez, L., and Young, M. W. (1996). Regulation of nuclear entry of the Drosophila clock proteins Period and Timeless. Neuron 17, 911–920. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80222-6
Sakai, T., and Ishida, N. (2001). Circadian rhythms of female mating activity governed by clock genes in Drosophila. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 98, 9221–9225. doi: 10.1073/pnas.151443298
Sakai, T., Tamura, T., Kitamoto, T., and Kidokoro, Y. (2004). A clock gene, period, plays a key role in long-term memory formation in Drosophila. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 101, 16058–16063. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0401472101
Sandrelli, F., Costa, R., Kyriacou, C. P., and Rosato, E. (2008). Comparative analysis of circadian clock genes in insects. Insect. Mol. Biol. 17, 447–463. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2583.2008.00832.x
Sandrelli, F., Tauber, E., Pegoraro, M., Mazzotta, G., Cisotto, P., Landskron, J., et al. (2007). A molecular basis for natural selection at the timeless locus in Drosophila melanogaster. Science 316, 1898–1900. doi: 10.1126/science.1138426
Sato, S., Chuman, Y., Matsushima, A., Tominaga, Y., Shimohigashi, Y., and Shimohigashi, M. (2002). A circadian neuropeptide, pigment-dispersing factor–PDF, in the last-summer cicada Meimuna opalifera: cDNA cloning and immunocytochemistry. Zool. Sci. 19, 821–828. doi: 10.2108/zsj.19.821
Sauer, S., Kinkelin, M., Herrmann, E., and Kaiser, W. (2003). The dynamics of sleep-like behaviour in honey bees. J. Comp. Physiol. A Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol. 189, 599–607. doi: 10.1007/s00359-003-0436-9
Saunders, D. S., Vincent, C. H., and Lawrence, I. G. (1989). Induction of diapause in Drosophila melanogaster: photoperiodic regulation and the impact of arrhythmic clock mutations on time measurement. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 86, 3748–3752. doi: 10.1073/pnas.86.10.3748
Schäbler, S., Amatobi, K. M., Horn, M., Rieger, D., Helfrich-Förster, C., Mueller, M. J., et al. (2020). Loss of function in the Drosophila clock gene period results in altered intermediary lipid metabolism and increased susceptibility to starvation. Cell. Mol. Life Sci. 77, 1–18. doi: 10.1007/s00018-019-03441-6
Schlichting, M., Menegazzi, P., Lelito, K. R., Yao, Z., Buhl, E., Dalla Benetta, E., et al. (2016). A neural network underlying circadian entrainment and photoperiodic adjustment of sleep and activity in Drosophila. J. Neurosci. 36, 9084–9096. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0992-16.2016
Schlichting, M., Rieger, D., Cusumano, P., Grebler, R., Costa, R., Mazzotta, G. M., et al. (2018). Cryptochrome interacts with actin and enhances eye-mediated light sensitivity of the circadian clock in Drosophila melanogaster. Front. Mol. Neurosci. 11:238. doi: 10.3389/fnmol.2018.00238
Schubert, F. K., Hagedorn, N., Yoshii, T., Helfrich-Förster, C., and Rieger, D. (2018). Neuroanatomical details of the lateral neurons of Drosophila melanogaster support their functional role in the circadian system. J. Comp. Neurol. 526, 1209–1231. doi: 10.1002/cne.24406
Seay, D. J., and Thummel, C. S. (2011). The circadian clock, light, and cryptochrome regulate feeding and metabolism in Drosophila. J. Biol. Rhythms. 26, 497–506. doi: 10.1177/0748730411420080
Sehgal, A., Price, J., Man, B., and Young, M. (1994). Loss of circadian behavioral rhythms and per RNA oscillations in the Drosophila mutant timeless. Science 263, 1603–1606. doi: 10.1126/science.8128246
Selcho, M., Millán, C., Palacios-Muñoz, A., Ruf, F., Ubillo, L., Chen, J., et al. (2017). Central and peripheral clocks are coupled by a neuropeptide pathway in Drosophila. Nat. Commun. 8:15563. doi: 10.1038/ncomms15563
Shafer, O. T., Kim, D. J., Dunbar-Yaffe, R., Nikolaev, V. O., Lohse, M. J., and Taghert, P. H. (2008). Widespread receptivity to neuropeptide PDF throughout the neuronal circadian clock network of Drosophila revealed by real-time cyclic AMP imaging. Neuron 58, 223–237. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.02.018
Shafer, O. T., and Yao, Z. (2014). Pigment-dispersing factor signaling and circadian rhythms in insect locomotor activity. Curr. Opin. Insect Sci. 1, 73–80. doi: 10.1016/j.cois.2014.05.002
Shaikevich, E. V., Zakharov, I. S., and Fedorova, M. V. (2016). Comparative analysis of the circadian rhythm genes period and timeless in Culex pipiens Linnaeus, 1758 (Diptera, Culicidae). CCG 10, 483–504. doi: 10.3897/CompCytogen.v10i4.7582
Sheeba, V., Chandrashekaran, M. K., Joshi, A., and Kumar Sharma, V. (2001). A case for multiple oscillators controlling different circadian rhythms in Drosophila melanogaster. J. Insect. Physiol. 47, 1217–1225. doi: 10.1016/S0022-1910(01)00107-X
Shell, W. A., and Rehan, S. M. (2018). Behavioral and genetic mechanisms of social evolution: insights from incipiently and facultatively social bees. Apidologie 49, 13–30. doi: 10.1007/s13592-017-0527-1
Shemesh, Y., Cohen, M., and Bloch, G. (2007). Natural plasticity in circadian rhythms is mediated by reorganization in the molecular clockwork in honeybees. FASEB J. 21, 2304–2311. doi: 10.1096/fj.06-8032com
Shemesh, Y., Eban-Rothschild, A., Cohen, M., and Bloch, G. (2010). Molecular dynamics and social regulation of context-dependent plasticity in the circadian clockwork of the honey bee. J. Neurosci. 30, 12517–12525. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1490-10.2010
Shiga, S., and Numata, H. (2009). Roles of PER immunoreactive neurons in circadian rhythms and photoperiodism in the blow fly, Protophormia terraenovae. J. Exp. Biol. 212, 867–877. doi: 10.1242/jeb.027003
Simoni, A., Wolfgang, W., Topping, M. P., Kavlie, R. G., Stanewsky, R., and Albert, J. T. (2014). A mechanosensory pathway to the Drosophila circadian clock. Science 343, 525–528. doi: 10.1126/science.1245710
Singaravel, M., Fujisawa, Y., Hisada, M., Saifullah, A. S. M., and Tomioka, K. (2003). Phase shifts of the circadian locomotor rhythm induced by pigment-dispersing factor in the cricket Gryllus bimaculatus. Zool. Sci. 20, 1347–1354. doi: 10.2108/zsj.20.1347
Siwicki, K. K., Petersen, G., Rosbash, M., and Hall, J.C. (1988). Antibodies to the period gene product of Drosophila reveal diverse tissue distribution and rhythmic changes in the visual system. Neuron 1, 141–150. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(88)90198-5
Smith, A. R., Kitchen, S. M., Toney, R. M., and Ziegler, C. (2017). Is nocturnal foraging in a tropical bee an escape from interference competition? J. Insect Sci. 17:30. doi: 10.1093/jisesa/iex030
Stehlík, J., Závodská, R., Shimada, K., Šauman, I., and Koštál, V. (2008). Photoperiodic induction of diapause requires regulated transcription of timeless in the larval brain of Chymomyza costata. J. Biol. Rhythms 23, 129–139. doi: 10.1177/0748730407313364
Stoleru, D., Peng, Y., Nawathean, P., and Rosbash, M. (2005). A resetting signal between Drosophila pacemakers synchronizes morning and evening activity. Nature 438, 238–242. doi: 10.1038/nature04192
Sumiyoshi, M., Sato, S., Takeda, Y., Sumida, K., Koga, K., Itoh, T., et al. (2011). A circadian neuropeptide PDF in the honeybee, Apis mellifera : cDNA cloning and expression of mRNA. Zool. Sci. 28, 897–909. doi: 10.2108/zsj.28.897
Syrová, Z., DoleŽel, D., Šaumann, I., and Hodková, M. (2003). Photoperiodic regulation of diapause in linden bugs: are period and Clock genes involved? Cell. Mol. Life Sci. 60, 2510–2515. doi: 10.1007/s00018-003-3227-0
Takekata, H., Matsuura, Y., Goto, S. G., Satoh, A., and Numata, H. (2012). RNAi of the circadian clock gene period disrupts the circadian rhythm but not the circatidal rhythm in the mangrove cricket. Biol. Lett. 8, 488–491. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2012.0079
Tauber, E., Zordan, M., Sandrelli, F., Pegoraro, M., Osterwalder, N., Breda, C., et al. (2007). Natural selection favors a newly derived timeless allele in Drosophila melanogaster. Science 316, 1895–1898. doi: 10.1126/science.1138412
Toda, H., Shi, M., Williams, J. A., and Sehgal, A. (2018). Genetic mechanisms underlying sleep. Cold. Spring Harb. Symp. Quant. Biol. 83, 57–61. doi: 10.1101/sqb.2018.83.037705
Tokuoka, A., Itoh, T. Q., Hori, S., Uryu, O., Danbara, Y., Nose, M., et al. (2017). cryptochrome genes form an oscillatory loop independent of the per/tim loop in the circadian clockwork of the cricket Gryllus bimaculatus. Zool. Lett 3:5. doi: 10.1186/s40851-017-0066-7
Tomioka, K., and Matsumoto, A. (2015). Circadian molecular clockworks in non-model insects. Curr. Opin. Insect Sci. 7, 58–64. doi: 10.1016/j.cois.2014.12.006
Top, D., and Young, M. W. (2018). Coordination between differentially regulated circadian clocks generates rhythmic behavior. Cold Spring Harb. Perspect. Biol. 10:a033589. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a033589
Tweedy, D. G., and Stephen, W. P. (1970). Light refractive emergence rhythm in leafcutter bee, Megachile rotundata (F.) (Hymenoptera:Apoidea). Cell. Mol. Life Sci. 26, 377–379. doi: 10.1007/BF01896898
Uryu, O., Karpova, S. G., and Tomioka, K. (2013). The clock gene cycle plays an important role in the circadian clock of the cricket Gryllus bimaculatus. J. Insect Physiol. 59, 697–704. doi: 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2013.04.011
Vafopoulou, X., Terry, K. L., and Steel, C. G. H. (2009). The circadian timing system in the brain of the fifth larval instar of Rhodnius prolixus (Hemiptera). J. Comp. Neurol. 518, 1264–1282. doi: 10.1002/cne.22274
Vanin, S., Bhutani, S., Montelli, S., Menegazzi, P., Green, E. W., Pegoraro, M., et al. (2012). Unexpected features of Drosophila circadian behavioural rhythms under natural conditions. Nature 484, 371–375. doi: 10.1038/nature10991
Vaze, K. M., and Helfrich-Förster, C. (2016). Drosophila ezoana uses an hour-glass or highly damped circadian clock for measuring night length and inducing diapause: Diapause induction in D. ezoana. Physiol. Entomol. 41, 378–389. doi: 10.1111/phen.12165
Veenstra, J. A. (2019). Coleoptera genome and transcriptome sequences reveal numerous differences in neuropeptide signaling between species. PeerJ 7:e7144. doi: 10.7717/peerj.7144
Velarde, R. A., Sauer, C. D. O., Walden, K. K., Fahrbach, S. E., and Robertson, H.M. (2005). Pteropsin: a vertebrate-like non-visual opsin expressed in the honey bee brain. Insect Biochem. Mol. Biol. 35, 1367–1377. doi: 10.1016/j.ibmb.2005.09.001
Wahl, O. (1933). Beitrag zur Frage der biologischen Bedeutung des Zeitgedächtnisses der Bienen. J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol. 18, 709–717.
Warren, T. L., Giraldo, Y. M., and Dickinson, M. H. (2019). Celestial navigation in Drosophila. J. Exp. Biol. 222:jeb186148. doi: 10.1242/jeb.186148
Watari, Y. (2005). Comparison of the circadian eclosion rhythm between non-diapause and diapause pupae in the onion fly, Delia antiqua: the change of rhythmicity. J. Insect Physiol. 51, 11–16. doi: 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2004.09.013
Watari, Y., and Tanaka, K. (2010). Interacting effect of thermoperiod and photoperiod on the eclosion rhythm in the onion fly, Delia antiqua supports the two-oscillator model. J. Insect. Physiol. 56, 1192–1197. doi: 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2010.03.022
Wcislo, W. T., and Tierney, S. M. (2009). Behavioural environments and niche construction: the evolution of dim-light foraging in bees. Biol. Rev. 84, 19–37. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-185X.2008.00059.x
Wei, H., Yasar, H., Funk, N. W., Giese, M., Baz, E.-S., and Stengl, M. (2014). Signaling of Pigment-Dispersing Factor (PDF) in the Madeira cockroach Rhyparobia maderae. PLoS ONE 9:e108757. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0108757
Weiss, R., Dov, A., Fahrbach, S. E., and Bloch, G. (2009). Body size-related variation in pigment dispersing factor-immunoreactivity in the brain of the bumblebee Bombus terrestris (Hymenoptera, Apidae). J. Insect. Physiol. 55, 479–487. doi: 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2009.01.016
Werckenthin, A., Derst, C., and Stengl, M. (2012). Sequence and expression of per, tim1, and cry2 genes in the madeira cockroach Rhyparobia maderae. J. Biol. Rhythms 27, 453–466. doi: 10.1177/0748730412462109
Werckenthin, A., Huber, J., Arnold, T., Koziarek, S., Plath, M. J. A., Plath, J. A., et al. (2020). Neither per, nor tim1, nor cry2 alone are essential components of the molecular circadian clockwork in the madeira cockroach. PLoS ONE. 15:e0235930. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0235930
Wise, S., Davis, N. T., Tyndale, E., Noveral, J., Folwell, M. G., Bedian, V., et al. (2002). Neuroanatomical studies of period gene expression in the hawkmoth, Manduca sexta. J. Comp. Neurol. 447, 366–380. doi: 10.1002/cne.10242
Xu, K., DiAngelo, J. R., Hughes, M. E., Hogenesch, J. B., and Sehgal, A. (2011). The circadian clock interacts with metabolic physiology to influence reproductive fitness. Cell Metabol. 13, 639–654. doi: 10.1016/j.cmet.2011.05.001
Yadlapalli, S., Jiang, C., Bahle, A., Reddy, P., Meyhofer, E., and Shafer, O. T. (2018). Circadian clock neurons constantly monitor environmental temperature to set sleep timing. Nature 555, 98–102. doi: 10.1038/nature25740
Yamada, H., and Yamamoto, M.-T. (2011). Association between circadian clock genes and diapause incidence in Drosophila triauraria. PLoS ONE 6, 1–9. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0027493
Yerushalmi, S., Bodenhaimer, S., and Bloch, G. (2006). Developmentally determined attenuation in circadian rhythms links chronobiology to social organization in bees. J. Exp. Biol. 209, 1044–1051. doi: 10.1242/jeb.02125
Yocum, G. D., Rinehart, J. P., Yocum, I. S., Kemp, W. P., and Greenlee, K. J. (2016). Thermoperiodism synchronizes emergence in the alfalfa leafcutting bee (Hymenoptera: Megachilidae). Environ. Entomol. 45, 245–251. doi: 10.1093/ee/nvv152
Yoshii, T., Hermann, C., and Helfrich-Förster, C. (2010). Cryptochrome-positive and -negative clock neurons in Drosophila entrain differentially to light and temperature. J. Biol. Rhythms 25, 387–398. doi: 10.1177/0748730410381962
Yoshii, T., Rieger, D., and Helfrich-F?rster, C. (2012). Two clocks in the brain: an update of the morning and evening oscillator model in Drosophila. Progress Brain Res. 199, 59–82. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-59427-3.00027-7
Yoshii, T., Vanin, S., Costa, R., and Helfrich-Förster, C. (2009b). Synergic entrainment of Drosophila's circadian clock by light and temperature. J. Biol. Rhythms 24, 452–464. doi: 10.1177/0748730409348551
Yoshii, T., Wulbeck, C., Sehadova, H., Veleri, S., Bichler, D., Stanewsky, R., et al. (2009a). The neuropeptide pigment-dispersing factor adjusts period and phase of Drosophila's clock. J. Neurosci. 29, 2597–2610. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5439-08.2009
Yuan, Q., Metterville, D., Briscoe, A. D., and Reppert, S. M. (2007). Insect cryptochromes: gene duplication and loss define diverse ways to construct insect circadian clocks. Mol. Biol. Evol. 24, 948–955. doi: 10.1093/molbev/msm011
Yurgel, M. E., Kakad, P., Zandawala, M., Nässel, D. R., Godenschwege, T. A., and Keene, A. C. (2019). A single pair of leucokinin neurons are modulated by feeding state and regulate sleep–metabolism interactions. PLoS Biol. 17, 1–26. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.2006409
Zandawala, M., Yurgel, M. E., Liao, S., Texada, M. J., Rewitz, K. F., Keene, A. C., et al. (2018). Orchestration of Drosophila post-feeding physiology and behavior by the neuropeptide leucokinin. BioRxiv 355107. doi: 10.1101/355107
Závodská, R., Šauman, I., and Sehnal, F. (2003). Distribution of PER protein, Pigment-Dispersing Hormone, Prothoracicotropic Hormone, and Eclosion Hormone in the cephalic nervous system of insects. J. Biol. Rhythms 18, 106–122. doi: 10.1177/0748730403251711
Ždárek, J., and Denlinger, D.L. (1995). Changes in temperature, not photoperiod control the pattern of adult eclosion in the tsetse, Glossina morsitans. Physiol. Entomol. 20, 362–366. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-3032.1995.tb00827.x
Zeller, M., Held, M., Bender, J., Berz, A., Heinloth, T., Hellfritz, T., et al. (2015). Transmedulla neurons in the sky compass network of the honeybee (Apis mellifera) are a possible site of circadian input. PLoS ONE 10:e0143244. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0143244
Zerr, D. M., Hall, J. C., Rosbash, M., and Siwicki, K. K. (1990). Circadian fluctuations of period protein immunoreactivity in the CNS and the visual system of Drosophila. J. Neurosci. 10, 2749–2762. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.10-08-02749.1990
Zheng, X., Koh, K., Sowcik, M., Smith, C. J., Chen, D., Wu, M. N., et al. (2009). An Isoform-specific mutant reveals a role of PDP1 in the circadian oscillator. J. Neurosci. 29, 10920–10927. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2133-09.2009
Zhou, J., Yu, W., and Hardin, P. E. (2016). clockwork orange enhances period mediated rhythms in transcriptional repression by antagonizing E-box Binding by clock-cycle. PLoS Genet 12:e1006430. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1006430
Zhu, H., Sauman, I., Yuan, Q., Casselman, A., Emery-Le, M., Emery, P., et al. (2008). Cryptochromes define a novel circadian clock mechanism in monarch butterflies that may underlie sun compass navigation. PLoS Biol. 6:e4. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0060004
Zhu, H., Yuan, Q., Froy, O., Casselman, A., and Reppert, S. M. (2005). The two CRYs of the butterfly. Curr. Biol. 15:2. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.11.030
Zimmerman, W. F., Pittendrigh, C. S., and Pavlidis, T. (1968). Temperature compensation of the circadian oscillation in Drosophila pseudoobscura and its entrainment by temperature cycles. J. Insect. Physiol. 14, 669–684. doi: 10.1016/0022-1910(68)90226-6
Keywords: Drosophila melanogaster, Apis mellifera, circadian clock, complex behavior, diapause, sociality
Citation: Beer K and Helfrich-Förster C (2020) Model and Non-model Insects in Chronobiology. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 14:601676. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2020.601676
Received: 02 September 2020; Accepted: 30 October 2020;
Published: 26 November 2020.
Edited by:
Maria de la Paz Fernandez, Columbia University, United StatesReviewed by:
Joanna C. Chiu, University of California, Davis, United StatesCharalambos P. Kyriacou, University of Leicester, United Kingdom
Copyright © 2020 Beer and Helfrich-Förster. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Katharina Beer, katharina.beer@uni-wuerzburg.de