Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 10 March 2020
Sec. Emotion Regulation and Processing
This article is part of the Research Topic Cognitive Control of Emotions in Challenging Contexts View all 20 articles

Trait Anxiety Attenuates Response Inhibition: Evidence From an ERP Study Using the Go/NoGo Task

\r\nLisheng XiaLisheng Xia1Licheng MoLicheng Mo2Jian WangJian Wang3Weifeng ZhangWeifeng Zhang3Dandan Zhang,*Dandan Zhang2,4*
  • 1School of Psychology, Guizhou Normal University, Guiyang, China
  • 2School of Psychology, Shenzhen University, Shenzhen, China
  • 3School of Electronics and Communication, Shenzhen Institute of Information Technology, Shenzhen, China
  • 4Shenzhen Institute of Neuroscience, Shenzhen, China

Neuropsychology and cognitive neuroscience have shown that anxious individuals have deficits in response inhibition. However, existing knowledge about the influence of trait anxiety on response inhibition is still inconsistent. The aim of this study was to investigate response inhibition between groups with high trait anxiety (HTA) and low trait anxiety (LTA). Here, we used event-related potential (ERP) indexes as biomarkers to examine the effect of trait anxiety on response inhibition using the Go/NoGo task. Behavioral results indicated that the HTA group made significantly lower accuracy than did the LTA group in the NoGo condition but not the Go condition. Meanwhile, the HTA group needed significantly longer overall response time (RT) than the LTA group did. ERP analyses revealed that the HTA group had smaller and later frontal NoGo-N2 as well as larger and later parietal NoGo-P3 compared to the LTA group. The two response inhibition-related ERP components are distinct neurophysiological indexes that, first, the NoGo-N2 is a component involved in the motor plan prior to the motor execution inhibitory process. Second, the NoGo-P3 reflects later monitoring and evaluation of the inhibition process. Accordingly, the current ERP findings suggest that HTA individuals’ response inhibition deficits are the consequence of abnormal premotor inhibition control and inefficient evaluation and monitoring. In addition, we also found that the peak amplitude of NoGo-N2 and NoGo-P3 were significantly correlated with the State–Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) scores after correction for multiple comparisons. To sum up, these results support the notion that trait anxious individuals have response inhibition deficits in the Go/NoGo task.

Introduction

According to Eysenck’s attentional control theory (Eysenck et al., 2007), anxiety might be associated with dysfunction of inhibitory control. From this perspective, neuropsychology and cognitive neuroscience studies have revealed that the medial prefrontal regions [including the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC)] are crucial substrates of the human anxiety circuitry (Sehlmeyer et al., 2009) and that deficits in these areas are associated with impaired inhibition control (Sehlmeyer et al., 2010). Although previous studies have shown that individuals with state anxiety (e.g., experimentally induced anxiety; see Aylward et al., 2017), anxiety with substance use (Karch et al., 2007), and clinical anxious patients (Grillon et al., 2017) have deficits in response inhibition, the question remains on whether and at what extent anxiety-related personality traits (e.g., trait anxiety) can also modulate response inhibition. Recent studies have demonstrated that trait anxiety interrupts top-down goal-driven processes such as response inhibition, resulting in failures in the inhibition function that enable executive control over prepotent motor responses (Pacheco-Unguetti et al., 2012; Su-Hao et al., 2014). Thus, this abnormal inhibitory function may consequently alter the level of cognitive control as well as cognitive performance in anxious population (Sehlmeyer et al., 2010) and appear to be a promising predictive marker of trait anxiety (Grillon et al., 2017). Response inhibition is a critical executive function in accordance with situation changes in everyday life, and this function involves attention and flexibility, which are largely influenced by anxiety levels (Pacheco-Unguetti et al., 2012). Moreover, investigating the response inhibition in anxiety may deepen our understanding of comorbid anxiety symptoms including impulsivity (Jakuszkowiak-Wojten et al., 2015) and substance use disorders (Karch et al., 2007). Therefore, it is important to investigate the influence of trait anxiety on response inhibition and its associated neural mechanism, which can broaden our understanding of the inhibitory control of anxious individuals and further unravel the psychological and etiological mechanisms of anxiety.

Among many inhibition tasks such as the two-choice oddball task (Wang et al., 2011; Yuan et al., 2012, 2017; Ren et al., 2019), this study employed the Go/NoGo task due to its wide application and easy performance (Helton, 2009). In this paradigm, subjects should response fast to frequently presented “Go” targets while ignoring rare “NoGo” stimuli using motor inhibition (Aron and Poldrack, 2005). Such withholding of a prepotent response generates a prototypical index of response inhibition (Helton, 2009; Bari and Robbins, 2013). To our knowledge, previous studies have examined the influence of trait anxiety on response inhibition in the Go/NoGo task and resulted in conflict results. For example, Sehlmeyer et al. (2010) found that trait anxious subjects maintain a high level of cognitive control effort reflected by electrophysiological measurements which might facilitate response inhibition. Some researchers explained that trait anxious individuals are cautious about errors and aware of their cognitive control failures. Thus, they might allocate excessive cognitive resources in response inhibition task (McWilliams and Cox, 2001; Righi et al., 2009; Sehlmeyer et al., 2010). However, some other studies found that attenuated response inhibition in trait anxiety due to the dysfunction in the frontal cortex (Yang and Li, 2014) and cognitive control deficits in anxiety resulted in impaired response inhibition (Pacheco-Unguetti et al., 2012). These results suggested that trait anxiety might attenuate rather than facilitate the response inhibition. Consistent with this idea, recent studies suggested that trait anxiety interferes with the top-down mechanisms required for the suppression of prepotent responses, resulting in failures in the response inhibition (Ansari and Derakshan, 2010, 2011). Furthermore, this abnormal response inhibition process might consequently reduce the level of cognitive control to prepare and to evaluate the outcome of actions reflected by electrophysiological measurements in trait anxious population (Yang and Li, 2014). In our opinion, the declining level of cognitive control supports the assumption that impairment of inhibitory control leads to reduced neural processing efficiency related to cognitive control in trait anxious individuals. Although the neural processing efficiency related to cognitive control in response inhibition account does not make predictions with respect to the effects of trait anxiety on behavioral performance (Basten et al., 2011), several studies proposed that less flexibility in response control in trait anxious individuals is driven by their repetitive compulsive behaviors which might induce an inability to inhibit prepotent responses reflected by behavioral performance measurements, for example, high-anxiety individuals may show an inefficient or inflexible response style with repetitive movements with rigid routines (Bannon et al., 2002; Martial et al., 2005). Thus, we assume that trait anxiety might attenuate the response inhibition that shifts motor action tendencies, resulting in cognitive failures. However, the detailed underlying mechanisms of trait anxiety modulate response inhibition related to cognitive control deficiency are far less elucidated (e.g., as mentioned above, the different patterns of electrophysiological activity related to cognitive control level in trait anxiety were not predictive of overt behavioral performance during response inhibition) and should be taken into consideration in our study.

The goal of the present study was to verify whether and at what extent the attenuated response inhibition processes in trait anxious individuals due to the impairment of cognitive control processes as shown on the electrophysiological level can also be demonstrated on the behavioral level, so as to enrich the understanding of the influence of trait anxiety on response inhibition. To this end, we chose the event-related potential (ERP) technique for its exquisite temporal resolution (Amodio et al., 2014). Two frontocentral ERP components have been associated with different subprocesses of response inhibition in the Go/NoGo task (Beste et al., 2010), based on which we compared the ERP differences between individuals with high and low trait anxiety (HTA and LTA, respectively). The first component is the frontal–midline N2, peaking approximately from 200 ms to 400 ms post stimulus. The N2 displays larger amplitudes in the NoGo compared to Go conditions (Eimer, 1993). In general, the N2 enhancement for NoGo stimuli (NoGo-N2) has been interpreted as a premotor inhibitory process that suppresses the incorrect response prior to reaction stage (Falkenstein et al., 1999). The latency of NoGo-N2 reflects the success or failure of inhibitory control (Roche et al., 2005). The amplitudes of NoGo-N2 have been found to be negatively correlated with psychiatric symptoms such as obsessive–compulsive disorder (Herrmann et al., 2003; Kim et al., 2007), depression (Katz et al., 2010), and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (Woltering et al., 2013); also, a high false alarm rate (the number of mistaken responses made on NoGo trials) is associated with small and delayed NoGo-N2 (Falkenstein et al., 1999). The second component is the parietal P3, peaking approximately from 300 ms to 600 ms post-stimulus, which also displays larger amplitudes in the NoGo compared to Go conditions (Falkenstein et al., 1999). The P3 enhancement to NoGo stimuli (NoGo-P3) has been considered as an extra enhanced cognitive control effort for later monitoring and evaluation of the outcome of inhibition process (Schmajuk et al., 2006; Huster et al., 2013). In addition, the prolonged NoGo-P3 latency might reflect the extent of evaluation processing (Roche et al., 2005). Taken together, the superior response inhibition in subjects was characterized by larger and shorter NoGo-N2 as well as smaller and shorter NoGo-P3 (Zhang et al., 2015).

Based on current knowledge in the Go/NoGo task, several studies found that the attenuated response inhibition in anxious population was due to decreased activation of the frontal area and that the hypoactivity of the frontal cortex might lower premotor inhibition but enhance the cognitive control effort for monitoring and evaluation of inhibition outcomes (Kim et al., 2007; Yang and Li, 2014). The latter two processes are reflected by the NoGo-N2 and NoGo-P3. Regarding this, we predicted in this study the following. On the behavioral level, the HTA group would exhibit lower accuracy (ACC) in the NoGo condition compared with the LTA group; on the electrophysiological level, the HTA group would show smaller and later NoGo-N2 as well as larger and later NoGo-P3 compare with the LTA group.

Materials and Methods

Participants

In view of the fact that anxiety and depression are highly comorbid (Hirsh and Inzlicht, 2008; Nelson et al., 2016) and depressive individuals also have inhibitory deficits in the Go/NoGo task (Kaiser et al., 2003; Ruchsow et al., 2008), we only recruited nondepressed participants with HTA vs. LTA in this study.

All the freshman students (n = 6,903) in Shenzhen University were required to complete the Trait form of Spielberger’s State–Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI-T; Spielberger et al., 1983; Shek, 1993). Among them, 788 questionnaires were missed. As a result, the total effective sample of questionnaires was 6,115, and the effective rate was 88.6%. In this sample, individuals with STAI-T scores in the upper and lower 25% of the distribution were considered as HTA and LTA subjects (Gu et al., 2010; Luo et al., 2014; Xia et al., 2017). The Beck Depression Inventory second edition (BDI-II; Beck et al., 1996) was used to assess self-reported symptoms of depression. Only the participants with BDI-II scores <13 were considered in this study (note that while BDI-II < 13 indicates minimal depression, BDI-II ≥ 14 indicates mild, moderate, or severe depression; see Beck et al., 1996). From those who met these criteria, we randomly recruited 56 students as paid participants (28 in the LTA group and 28 in the HTA group). There was no significant difference between the two groups with respect to age, handedness, and BDI-II scores (see Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic data of participants with high trait anxiety (HTA) and low trait anxiety (LTA).

Exclusion criteria for both groups were: (1) any Axis I and II disorders according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM-IV; APA, 1994); (2) seizure disorder; (3) history of head injury with possible neurological sequelae; and (4) substance abuse or dependence in the past 6 months.

Procedures

Each trial started with a 200- to 300-ms fixation, followed by targets (Go stimuli: M, N or O, Q) and nontargets (NoGo stimuli: O, Q or M, N) that were presented for 150 ms (see also Kim et al., 2007). A black blank screen appeared after the stimulus and lasted for 1,000 ms. Participants were required to press a button with their right index finger on the response box as quick as possible when the Go stimuli appeared while with hold the motor responses when the NoGo stimuli appeared. The Go and NoGo trials were presented in random order with a probability of 2:1 to build a prepotent response of “Go” (see also Yang et al., 2010). The total experiment consisted of two blocks, with 240 trials in each block (Go stimulus: 160 trials; NoGo stimulus: 80 trials). In order to avoid the confounding factor of letter shape, the Go and NoGo stimuli were counterbalanced across subjects (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Illustration of the Go/NoGo paradigm in this study. To counterbalance the Go and NoGo stimuli across subjects, the first half of low trait anxiety (LTA) and high trait anxiety (HTA) subjects was assigned to program (A), while the second half was assigned to program (B). LTA, the low-trait anxiety group; HTA, the high-trait anxiety group.

EEG Recording and Analysis

Brain electrical activity was recorded referentially against the left mastoid and re-referenced off-line to the average of the left and right mastoids, by a 64-channel amplifier with a sampling frequency of 250 Hz (Brain Products, Gilching, Germany). Electroencephalography (EEG) data were collected with electrode impedances kept below 5 kΩ. Ocular artifacts were removed from EEGs using a regression procedure implemented in Neuroscan software (Scan 4.3).

The recorded EEG data were filtered (0.01−30 Hz; slope 12 dB/oct; zero phase) and segmented beginning 200 ms prior to the onset of stimuli. All epochs were baseline-corrected with respect to the mean voltage over the 200 ms preceding the onset of stimuli, followed by averaging in association with Go and NoGo conditions. Trials contaminated with large artifacts (peak-to-peak deflection exceeded ±100 μV) were excluded from the averaging. As a result, 21 ± 8 trials and 13 ± 7 trials were rejected in each subject for Go and NoGo conditions, respectively. The rejected trials were less than 10% of the total trials (see also Gu et al., 2010; Xia et al., 2017). Trial numbers did not show significant difference between experimental conditions.

We analyzed the peak amplitudes and peak latencies of the frontal–midline N2 and parietal P3; the measures were averaged based on waveforms of different sets of electrodes according to grand-mean ERP topographies and relevant literatures (Kim et al., 2007; Huang et al., 2009; Righi et al., 2009). The N2 peak was detected to occur at 250–350 ms post stimuli at the electrode sites of Fz, F1, F2, FCz, FC1, FC2, Cz, C1, and C2, while the P3 peak was detected within a time window of 340–420 ms (Go condition) or 430–530 ms (NoGo condition) at the electrode sites of Pz, P1, P2, CPz, CP1, CP2, and POz.

Statistics

Statistical analysis was performed using SPSS Statistics 21.0 (IBM, Somers, NY, USA). Descriptive data were presented as mean ± standard error. The significance level was set at 0.05.

Two-way repeated-measures ANOVAs were performed on measurements of behavioral [ACC and response time (RT)] and ERP data (N2 and P3 amplitude/latency), with response assignment (Go vs. NoGo) as the within-subject factor and group (HTA vs. LTA) as the between-subject factor. Significant interactions were analyzed using simple effects model. Partial eta-squared (ηp2) was reported to demonstrate the effect size in ANOVA tests.

Two-tailed Pearson’s r correlation was performed between the two self-reported measures (BDI-II and STAI-T) and behavioral/ERP indexes. Correction for multiple comparisons was based on Holm’s stepwise method.

Results

For the sake of brevity, the experimental effects that did not reach significance were omitted.

Behaviors

Accuracy

The interaction of response assignment by group was significant (F(1,54) = 4.107; p = 0.048; ηp2 = 0.071; Figure 2A). Simple effect analysis indicated that the ACC in the NoGo trials was lower in the HTA group (76.05 ± 2.96%) compared with that in the LTA group (86.75 ± 2.96%; F(1,54) = 6.525, p = 0.013). However, this group difference did not achieve significance level in the Go trials (F(1,54) < 1; HTA = 93.07 ± 2.06% ; LTA = 95.78 ± 2.06%).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Behavioral results. (A) The Go accuracy and NoGo accuracy in the two groups. (B) The overall response time (RT) in the two groups. Bars represent standard error of the mean. LTA, the low-trait anxiety group; HTA, the high-trait anxiety group. *p < 0.05.

The main effect of group was significant (F(1,54) = 4.926; p = 0.031; ηp2 = 0.084). The HTA group (84.56 ± 2.14%) made lower ACC than the LTA group did (91.26 ± 2.14%).

Response Time

The main effect of group was significant (F(1,54) = 7.733; p = 0.007; ηp2 = 0.125; Figure 2B). The LTA group (overall RT: 274.77 ± 10.93 ms; Go RT for correct response: 295.82 ± 14.36 ms) responded much faster than the HTA group did (overall RT: 317.75 ± 10.93 ms; Go RT for correct response: 322.25 ± 14.36 ms).

ERPs

N2

For peak amplitude, the interaction effect of response assignment by group was significant (F(1,54) = 4.418; p = 0.040; ηp2 = 0.076; Figure 3). Simple effect analysis indicated that the N2 amplitude in the NoGo condition (F(1,54) = 9.567; p = 0.003) was lower in the HTA group (−1.35 ± 0.31 μV) compared with the LTA group (−2.71 ± 0.31 μV). However, this group difference did not achieve significant level in the Go condition (F(1,54) < 1; HTA = 0.76 ± 0.28 μV; LTA = 0.77 ± 0.28 μV). The main effect of group was significant (F(1,54) = 6.567; p = 0.013; ηp2 = 0.108). The HTA group (−0.29 ± 0.19 μV) had a smaller N2 than the LTA group did (−0.97 ± 0.19 μV).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. The N2 component time-locked to the Go and NoGo conditions. Event-related potentials (ERPs) were calculated by averaging the data at the electrodes of Fz, F1, F2, FCz, FC1, FC2, Cz, C1, and C2.

For peak latency, the interaction effect of response assignment by group was significant (F(1,54) = 7.946; p = 0.007; ηp2 = 0.128; Figure 3). Simple effect analysis indicated that the N2 latency in the NoGo condition (F(1,54) = 11.475; p = 0.001) was longer in the HTA group (336.71 ± 8.08 ms) compared with LTA group (298.00 ± 8.08 ms), However, this group difference did not achieve significance level in the Go condition (F(1,54) < 1; HTA = 264.50 ± 7.12 ms; LTA = 268.75 ± 7.12 ms). The main effect of group was significant (F(1,54) = 5.121; p = 0.028; ηp2 = 0.087). The HTA group (300.61 ± 5.38 ms) had a longer peak latency of the average N2 than the LTA group did (283.38 ± 5.38 ms).

P3

For peak amplitude, the interaction effect of response assignment by group was significant (F(1,54) = 4.534; p = 0.038; ηp2 = 0.077; Figure 4). Simple effect analysis indicated that the P3 amplitude in the NoGo condition (F(1,54) = 11.496; p = 0.001) was higher in the HTA group (6.63 ± 0.33 μV) compared with the LTA group (5.04 ± 0.33 μV). However, this group difference did not achieve significance level in the Go condition (F(1,54) < 1; HTA = 3.55 ± 0.33 μV; LTA = 3.37 ± 0.33 μV). The main effect of group was significant (F(1,54) = 7.125; p = 0.01; ηp2 = 0.117). The HTA group (5.09 ± 0.24 μV) had a larger P3 than the LTA group did (4.20 ± 0.24 μV).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. The P3 component time-locked to the Go and NoGo conditions. ERPs were calculated by averaging the data at the electrodes of Pz, P1, P2, CPz, CP1, CP2, and POz.

For peak latency, the interaction effect of response assignment by group was significant (F(1,54) = 6.603; p = 0.013; ηp2 = 0.109; Figure 4). Simple effect analysis indicated that the P3 latency in the NoGo condition (F(1,54) = 16.664; p < 0.001) was longer in the HTA group (473.86 ± 11.82 ms) compared with LTA group (405.64 ± 11.82 ms). However, this group difference did not achieve significance level in the Go condition (F(1,54) < 1; HTA = 376.75 ± 11.17 ms ; LTA = 372.93 ± 11.17 ms). The main effect of group was significant (F(1,54) = 12.076; p = 0.001; ηp2 = 0.183). The HTA group (425.30 ± 7.33 ms) had a longer peak latency of the average P3 than the LTA group did (389.29 ± 7.33 ms).

Correlation

According to the results reported above, we conducted Pearson correlation analyses between the two self-reported scores (STAI-T and BDI) and the five behavioral/ERP indexes which showed interaction between response assignment and group (e.g., the ACC in the NoGo condition and the peak amplitudes as well as peak latencies of NoGo-N2 and NoGo-P3). In total, we performed 10 (2 × 5) correlations.

The results showed two significant correlations after correction for multiple comparisons. The peak amplitudes of NoGo-N2 (r = 0.383, p = 0.004, corrected p = 0.036; note that since NoGo-N2 is a negative-going component, the positive correlation means that higher STAI-T scores were associated with smaller NoGo-N2 amplitudes) and NoGo-P3 were correlated with the STAI-T (r = 0.404, p = 0.002, corrected p = 0.02).

In addition, we also conducted Pearson correlation analyses between the behavior and ERP which showed interaction between response assignment and group (e.g., the ACC in the NoGo condition and the peak amplitudes as well as peak latencies of NoGo-N2 and NoGo-P3). In total, we performed 4 (1 × 4) correlations.

The results showed one significant correlation after correction for multiple comparisons. The peak amplitudes of NoGo-N2 were correlated with ACC (r = −0.409, p = 0.002, corrected p = 0.02).

Discussion

Debate exists on the influence of trait anxiety on response inhibition. This study applied the Go/NoGo paradigm to compare the response inhibition between HTA and LTA participants. On the behavioral level, we found that the HTA group made significantly lower ACC than the LTA group did in the NoGo condition. Meanwhile, the overall RT was longer in the HTA group than in the LTA group. Our behavioral results are consistent with the study conducted by Pacheco-Unguetti et al. (2012) that showed impaired response inhibition processes due to inflexibility in response control in anxiety. However, our behavioral results are inconsistent with some other studies showing no behavioral effect of trait anxiety in the Go/NoGo paradigm (Karch et al., 2007; Ruchsow et al., 2008; Righi et al., 2009). These discrepancies may be due to the difference in experimental parameters. For example, the study conducted by Righi et al. (2009) required subjects to inhibit their response to only one special stimulus, and all stimuli were presented for 250 ms. However, in this study, subjects were required to discriminate between two pairs of stimuli, and these stimuli were presented for 150 ms, which enhanced the difficulty of inhibiting the NoGo stimuli. In addition, the ratio between Go and NoGo trials may modulate the behavioral results (Kim et al., 2007). In our study, we used a 2:1 Go/NoGo ratio while Karch et al. (2007) and Ruchsow et al. (2008) used a 1:1 ratio.

As mentioned in the “Introduction” section, we speculate that the attenuated response inhibition behavioral effect in trait anxious individuals is driven by their repetitive compulsive behaviors. The core symptoms of repetitive compulsive behaviors (e.g., rigid routines, hesitation, and inflexibility) have been thought to be related to response inhibition deficits (Bannon et al., 2002), and this deficit might serve as a behavioral maker underlying inhibitory dysfunction of anxiety findings (Martial et al., 2005; Kim et al., 2007). Moreover, several evidences suggested that repetitive compulsive behaviors and trait anxiety are highly positive related (Black, 2008; Rodgers et al., 2012; Goodwin, 2015). It has been found that HTA individuals showed an inability to inhibit certain stimuli or certain prepotent responses compared with LTA individuals due to the repetitive compulsive behaviors (Martial et al., 2005). Meanwhile, HTA participants are less flexible in response control (e.g., inability to set shift) than the LTA group due to repetitive compulsive behavior inducing slowness and hesitation (Hughes et al., 2008; Dar and Iqbal, 2014; Yilmaz, 2015), resulting in a decrease in speed in the task. Note that this study also found that the Go RT was slightly longer in the HTA group than in the LTA group. In the current study, both the ACC and the RT indicate that HTA individuals are inferior to LTA individuals in response inhibition, leading to decreased NoGo ACC and longer RT. However, a non-negligible limitation of the current study is that we did not include any behavioral measure of the repetitive compulsive level, such as the Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale (Goodman et al., 1989) and Repetitive Behaviors Scale (Lam and Aman, 2007). Follow-up research is necessary to further address this issue.

On the electrophysiological level, the first ERP finding is that trait anxious participants showed smaller and later NoGo-N2 compared to low-anxiety participants. Consistent with this result, previous Go/NoGo studies found that a smaller and/or later NoGo-N2 was evoked in individuals with obsessive–compulsive disorder (Herrmann et al., 2003; Kim et al., 2007), depression (Katz et al., 2010), and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (Woltering et al., 2013). As mentioned in the introduction, the NoGo-N2 reflects the inhibitory process of a motor plan prior to the motor execution stage (Eimer, 1993; Falkenstein et al., 1999; Herrmann et al., 2003; Karch et al., 2007; Kim et al., 2007; Huster et al., 2013), and this inhibitory process is usually located at the premotor level rather than at the motor level (Falkenstein et al., 1999). The N2 enhancement to the NoGo stimulus has been suggested to reflect the suppression of incorrect response prior to the motor action (Falkenstein et al., 1999; Zhang et al., 2015), and a shorter-latency NoGo-N2 has been observed in successful withholding to NoGo stimuli compared with unsuccessful attempts to inhibit (Roche et al., 2005). In addition, neuroimaging studies have revealed that the neural sources of NoGo-N2 are located in the ACC and inferior/orbitofrontal prefrontal cortex (Kiefer et al., 1998; Bokura et al., 2001; Bekker et al., 2005). In our opinion, the smaller and later NoGo-N2 in the frontocentral electrode sites observed in anxious individuals here indicates that trait anxiety is associated with dysfunction in the frontal prefrontal cortex (including the ACC), which are crucial neural substrates known to be involved with the anxiety circuitry (Sehlmeyer et al., 2010). Taken together, the current finding of anxiety-modulated NoGo-N2 suggests that the premotor inhibitory process of trait anxious individuals is impaired, which might disrupt behavioral responses and inhibition.

The second ERP finding is that trait anxious participants were associated with an enhanced and latency-prolonged NoGo-P3 component. The NoGo-P3 is usually considered as the later monitoring and evaluation of the inhibition process outcome (Beste et al., 2010; Huster et al., 2013). The P3 enhancement to NoGo stimuli has been considered as more effort being devoted to monitoring of behavioral outcome (Sehlmeyer et al., 2010), while the prolonged NoGo-P3 latency is thought to be indicative of deliberative or “deeper” evaluation (Roche et al., 2005). Several studies related to response inhibition revealed that anxious individuals need to allocate more cognitive resources and make more control effort to withhold a response compared with healthy individuals, leading to a larger and/or longer NoGo-P3 (Karch et al., 2007; Ruchsow et al., 2007). Consistent with this idea, the current finding shows that the NoGo stimulus evoked larger and later NoGo-P3 in anxious individuals, suggesting that anxiety impairs inhibitory control system and makes us unable to evaluate and monitor the inhibition of incorrect responses in an efficient manner, leading to enhanced cognitive control effort or extra processing resources in the brain of anxious people.

Finally, one limitation should be pointed out for an appropriate interpretation of the current result. This study only measured the response inhibition in young, anxious adults (approximately 20 years old). Seeing that the cognitive characteristics of anxious people might differ between the young-adult group and other age groups (Krasucki et al., 1998; Goldberg et al., 2003), the generalizability of the current findings awaits to be tested in future work.

To sum up, this study has revealed that HTA participants have response inhibition deficits in the Go/NoGo paradigm, demonstrating a negative relationship between trait anxiety and response inhibition. The ERP results indicate that the psychological processes of premotor (reflected by NoGo-N2) and later evaluation of inhibition processes (reflected by NoGo-P3) both contribute to impaired response inhibition in anxiety. Specifically, HTA individuals’ response inhibition deficits are due to deficits of premotor inhibition control and inefficient evaluation and monitoring of NoGo stimuli. These findings would provide valuable knowledge about the underlying mechanism of the maladaptive response inhibition in trait anxious people.

Data Availability Statement

All datasets generated for this study are included in the article.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee of Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences (H14019). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

LX and DZ designed the study. LX conducted the experiment and analyzed the data. LX, LM, JW, WZ, and DZ contributed to the manuscript.

Funding

This study was funded by the PhD research startup foundation of Guizhou Normal University [GZNUD(2018)35], the National Natural Science Foundation of China (31970980), Guangdong Key Basic Research Grant (2018B030332001), and Shenzhen Basic Research Project (JCYJ20180305124305294).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Amodio, D. M., Bartholow, B. D., and Ito, T. A. (2014). Tracking the dynamics of the social brain: ERP approaches for social cognitive and affective neuroscience. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 385–393. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ansari, T. L., and Derakshan, N. (2010). Anxiety impairs inhibitory control but not volitional action control. Cogn. Emot. 24, 241–254. doi: 10.1080/02699930903381531

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ansari, T. L., and Derakshan, N. (2011). The neural correlates of impaired inhibitory control in anxiety. Neuropsychologia 49, 1146–1153. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.01.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

APA. (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th Edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

Google Scholar

Aron, A. R., and Poldrack, R. A. (2005). The cognitive neuroscience of response inhibition: relevance for genetic research in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 1285–1292. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.10.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aylward, J., Valton, V., Goer, F., Mkrtchian, A., Lally, N., Peters, S., et al. (2017). The impact of induced anxiety on affective response inhibition. R. Soc. Open Sci. 4:170084. doi: 10.1098/rsos.170084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bannon, S., Gonsalvez, C. J., Croft, R. J., and Boyce, P. M. (2002). Response inhibition deficits in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Res. 110, 165–174. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(02)00104-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bari, A., and Robbins, T. W. (2013). Inhibition and impulsivity: behavioral and neural basis of response control. Prog. Neurobiol. 108, 44–79. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2013.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Basten, U., Stelzel, C., and Fiebach, C. J. (2011). Trait anxiety modulates the neural efficiency of inhibitory control. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 3132–3145. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., Ball, R., and Ranieri, W. (1996). Comparison of the beck depression inventories -IA and -II in psychiatric outpatients. J. Pers. Assess. 67, 588–597. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa6703_13

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bekker, E. M., Kenemans, J. L., and Verbaten, M. N. (2005). Source analysis of the N2 in a cued go/nogo task. Cogn. Brain Res. 22, 221–231. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.08.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beste, C., Willemssen, R., Saft, C., and Falkenstein, M. (2010). Response inhibition subprocesses and dopaminergic pathways: basal ganglia disease effects. Neuropsychologia 48, 366–373. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.09.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Black, D. W. (2008). Anxiety and the spectrum of obsessive-compulsive disorder. CNS Spectr. 13, 4–5. doi: 10.1017/s1092852900026870

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bokura, H., Yamaguchi, S., and Kobayashi, S. (2001). Electrophysiological correlates for response inhibition in a go/nogo task. Clin. Neurophysiol. 112, 2224–2232. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(01)00691-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dar, K. A., and Iqbal, N. (2014). Worry and rumination in generalized anxiety disorder and obsessive compulsive disorder. J. Psychol. 149, 1–15. doi: 10.1080/00223980.2014.986430

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eimer, M. (1993). Effects of attention and stimulus probability on ERPs in a go/nogo task. Biol. Psychol. 35, 123–138. doi: 10.1016/0301-0511(93)90009-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., and Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: attentional control theory. Emotion 7, 336–353. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.336

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Falkenstein, M., Hoormann, J., and Hohnsbein, J. (1999). ERP components in Go/Nogo tasks and their relation to inhibition. Acta Psychol. 101, 267–291. doi: 10.1016/s0001-6918(99)00008-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldberg, J. H., Breckenridge, J. N., and Sheikh, J. I. (2003). Age differences in symptoms of depression and anxiety: examining behavioral medicine outpatients. J. Behav. Med. 26, 119–132. doi: 10.1023/a:1023030605390

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodman, W. K., Price, L. H., Rasmussen, S. A., Mazure, C., Delgado, P., Heninger, G. R., et al. (1989). The Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale: II. Validity. Arch. Gene. Psychiatry 46, 1012–1016.

Google Scholar

Goodwin, G. M. (2015). The overlap between anxiety, depression and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 17, 249–260.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Grillon, C., Robinson, O. J., O’Connell, K., Davis, A., Alvarez, G., Pine, D. S., et al. (2017). Clinical anxiety promotes excessive response inhibition. Psychol. Med. 47, 484–494. doi: 10.1017/s0033291716002555

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gu, R., Ge, Y., Jiang, Y., and Luo, Y. J. (2010). Anxiety and outcome evaluation: the good, the bad and the ambiguous. Biol. Psychol. 85, 200–206. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Helton, W. S. (2009). Impulsive responding and the sustained attention to response task. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 31, 39–47. doi: 10.1080/13803390801978856

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herrmann, M. J., Jacob, C., Unterecker, S., and Fallgatter, A. J. (2003). Reduced response-inhibition in obsessive-compulsive disorder measured with topographic evoked potential mapping. Psychiatry Res. 120, 265–271. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(03)00188-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hirsh, J. B., and Inzlicht, M. (2008). The devil you know: neuroticism predicts neural response to uncertainty. Psychol. Sci. 19, 962–967. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02183.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, Y. X., Bai, L., Ai, H., Li, W., Yua, C., Liu, J., et al. (2009). Influence of trait-anxiety on inhibition function: evidence from ERPs study. Neurosci. Lett. 456, 1–5. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2009.03.072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hughes, M. E., Alloy, L. B., and Cogswell, A. (2008). Repetitive thought in psychopathology: the relation of rumination and worry to depression and anxiety symptoms. J. Cogn. Psychother. 22, 271–288. doi: 10.1891/0889-8391.22.3.271

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huster, R. J., Enriquez-Geppert, S., Lavallee, C. F., Falkenstein, M., and Herrmann, C. S. (2013). Electroencephalography of response inhibition tasks: functional networks and cognitive contributions. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 87, 217–233. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jakuszkowiak-Wojten, K., Landowski, J., Wiglusz, M. S., and Cubała, W. J. (2015). Impulsivity in anxiety disorders: a critical review. Psychiatr. Danub. 27, S452–S455.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Kaiser, S., Unger, J., Kiefer, M., Markela, J., Mundt, C., and Weisbrod, M. (2003). Executive control deficit in depression: event-related potentials in a go/nogo task. Psychiatry Res. 122, 169–184. doi: 10.1016/s0925-4927(03)00004-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karch, S., Graz, C., Jager, L., Karamatskos, E., Flatz, A. S., Lutz, J., et al. (2007). Influence of anxiety on electrophysiological correlates of response inhibition capacities in alcoholism. Clin. EEG Neurosci. 38, 89–95. doi: 10.1177/155005940703800211

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Katz, R., De Sanctis, P., Mahoney, J. R., Sehatpour, P., Murphy, C. F., Gomez-Ramirez, M., et al. (2010). Cognitive control in late-life depression: response inhibition deficits and dysfunction of the anterior cingulate cortex. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 18, 1017–1025. doi: 10.1097/jgp.0b013e3181d695f2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kiefer, M., Marzinzik, F., Weisbrod, M., Scherg, M., and Spitzer, M. (1998). The time course of brain activations during response inhibition: evidence from event-related potentials in a go/nogo task. Neuroreport 9, 765–770. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199803090-00037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, M. S., Kim, Y. Y., Yoo, S. Y., and Kwon, J. S. (2007). Electrophysiological correlates of behavioral response inhibition in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Depress. Anxiety 24, 22–31. doi: 10.1002/da.20195

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krasucki, C., Howard, R., and Mann, A. (1998). The relationship between anxiety disorders and age. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 13, 79–99. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1099-1166(199802)13:2<79::aid-gps739>3.0.co;2-g

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lam, K. S. L., and Aman, M. G. (2007). The repetitive behavior scale-revised: independent validation in individuals with autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 37, 855–866. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0213-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luo, Y., Wu, T., Broster, L. S., Feng, C., Zhang, D., Gu, R., et al. (2014). The temporal course of the influence of anxiety on fairness considerations. Psychophysiology 51, 834–842. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12235

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martial, V. D. L., Ceschi, G., Zermatten, A., Dunker, D., and Perroud, A. (2005). Investigation of response inhibition in obsessive-compulsive, disorder using the hayling task. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 11, 776–783. doi: 10.1017/s1355617705050927

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McWilliams, L. A., and Cox, B. J. (2001). How distinct is anxiety sensitivity from trait anxiety? A re-examination from a multidimensional perspective. Pers. Individ. Differ. 31, 813–818. doi: 10.1016/s0191-8869(00)00182-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nelson, B. D., Kessel, E. M., Jackson, F., and Hajcak, G. (2016). The impact of an unpredictable context and intolerance of uncertainty on the electrocortical response to monetary gains and losses. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 16, 153–163. doi: 10.3758/s13415-015-0382-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pacheco-Unguetti, A. P., Acosta, A., Lupiáñez, J., Román, R., and Derakshan, N. (2012). Response inhibition and attentional control in anxiety. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. Hove. 65, 646–660. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2011.637114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ren, Z. N., Yang, J. M., and Yuan, J. J. (2019). Unconscious impulsivity control maintains the ability of behavioral inhibitory control in males: evidence of reaction time cost. Psych. J. 8, 330–341. doi: 10.1002/pchj.299

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Righi, S., Mecacci, L., and Viggiano, M. P. (2009). Anxiety, cognitive self-evaluation andperformance: ERP correlates. J. Anxiety Disord. 23, 1132–1138. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2009.07.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roche, R. A. P., Garavan, H., Foxe, J. J., and O’Mara, S. M. (2005). Individual differences discriminate event-related potentials but not performance during response inhibition. Exp. Brain Res. 160, 60–70. doi: 10.1007/s00221-004-1985-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodgers, J., Glod, M., Connolly, B., and Mcconachie, H. (2012). The relationship between anxiety and repetitive behaviours in autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 42, 2404–2409. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1531-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruchsow, M., Groen, G., Kiefer, M., Beschoner, P., Hermle, L., Ebert, D., et al. (2008). Electrophysiological evidence for reduced inhibitory control in depressed patients in partial remission: a go/nogo study. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 68, 209–218. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.01.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruchsow, M., Reuter, K., Hermle, L., Ebert, D., Kiefer, M., and Falkenstein, M. (2007). Executivecontrol in obsessive-compulsive disorder: event-related potentials in a go/nogo task. J. Neural Transm. 114, 1595–1601. doi: 10.1007/s00702-007-0779-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmajuk, M., Liotti, M., Busse, L., and Woldorff, M. G. (2006). Electrophysiological activity underlying inhibitory control processes in normal adults. Neuropsychologia 44, 384–395. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sehlmeyer, C., Konrad, C., Zwitserlood, P., Arolt, V., Falkenstein, M., and Beste, C. (2010). ERP indices for response inhibition are related to anxiety-related personality traits. Neuropsychologia 48, 2488–2495. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.04.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sehlmeyer, C., Sonja, S., Zwitserlood, P., Pfleiderer, B., Kircher, T., Arolt, V., et al. (2009). Human fear conditioning and extinction in neuroimaging: a systematic review. PLoS One 4:e5865. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0005865

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shek, D. T. (1993). The chinese version of the state-trait anxiety inventory: its relationship to different measures of psychological well-being. J. Clin. Psychol. 49, 349–358. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(199305)49:3<349::aid-jclp2270490308>3.0.co;2-j

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. R., and Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologist Press.

Google Scholar

Su-Hao, P., Qian, T., and Bin, X. (2014). Inhibitory control on induced negative emotion in individuals with trait anxiety. Chinese Mental Health J. 28, 215–220.

Google Scholar

Wang, Y., Yang, J., Yuan, J., Fu, A., Meng, X., and Li, H. (2011). The impact of emotion valence on brain processing of behavioral inhibitory control: spatiotemporal dynamics. Neurosci. Lett. 502, 112–116. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2011.07.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woltering, S., Liu, Z., Rokeach, A., and Tannock, R. (2013). Neurophysiological differences in inhibitory control between adults with ADHD and their peers. Neuropsychologia 51, 1888–1895. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.06.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xia, L., Gu, R., Zhang, D., and Luo, Y. (2017). Anxious individuals are impulsive decision-makers in the delay discounting task: an ERP study. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 11, 1–11. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, L., and Li, X. B. (2014). “Anxiety alters brain activity of response inhibition: evidence from event related potentials and source current density analysis,” in 4th IEEE International Conference on Information Science and Technology (Shenzhen, China: IEEE). doi: 10.1109/ICIST.2014.6920355

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, S., Huang, Y., Zhang, H., and Luo, Y. (2010). Neural mechanism of how emotion affects behavioral inhibition. Adv. Psy. Sci. 4, 605–615.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Yilmaz, A. E. (2015). The role of worry and rumination in the symptoms of anxiety and depression. Turk. J. Psychiatry 26, 107–115. doi: 10.5080/u8017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yuan, J. J., Xu, M. M., Yang, J. M., and Li, H. (2017). The application of the two-choice oddball paradigm to the research of behavioral inhibitory control. Sci. Sin. 47, 1065–1073. doi: 10.1360/n052017-00125

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yuan, J., Meng, X., Yang, J., Yao, G., Hu, L., and Yuan, H. (2012). The valence strength of unpleasant emotion modulates brain processing of behavioral inhibitory control: neural correlates. Biol. Psychol. 89, 240–251. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.10.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, D., Ding, H., Wang, X., Qi, C., and Luo, Y. (2015). Enhanced response inhibition in experienced fencers. Sci. Rep. 5:16282. doi: 10.1038/srep16282

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: response inhibition, trait anxiety, event-related potential, Go/NoGo, N2, P3

Citation: Xia L, Mo L, Wang J, Zhang W and Zhang D (2020) Trait Anxiety Attenuates Response Inhibition: Evidence From an ERP Study Using the Go/NoGo Task. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 14:28. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2020.00028

Received: 01 December 2019; Accepted: 07 February 2020;
Published: 10 March 2020.

Edited by:

Jiajin Yuan, Southwest University, China

Reviewed by:

XianXin Meng, Nanyang Normal College, China
Jingbo Gong, Hunan University of Chinese Medicine, China

Copyright © 2020 Xia, Mo, Wang, Zhang and Zhang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Dandan Zhang, zhangdd05@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.