- 1Cognitive Neuroscience Division, Gertrude H. Sergievsky Center, Taub Institute for Research on Alzheimer’s Disease and the Aging Brain, Columbia University Irving Medical Center, New York, NY, United States
- 2Center for the Study of Movement Cognition and Mobility, Neurological Institute, Tel Aviv Sourasky Medical Center, Tel Aviv, Israel
- 3Department of Neurology and Neurosurgery, Sackler Faculty of Medicine, and Sagol School of Neuroscience, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel
- 4Department of Physical Therapy, Sackler Faculty of Medicine, and Sagol School of Neuroscience, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel
- 5Rush Alzheimer’s Disease Center and Department of Orthopedic Surgery, Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, IL, United States
- 6Department of Neurology, Columbia University Irving Medical Center, New York, NY, United States
Objective: Cognition and mobility are interrelated. However, this association can be impacted by the specific facets of cognition and mobility that are measured, and further by the different task conditions, e.g., single- versus dual-task walking, under which these associations are evaluated. Systematically studying the multiple facets of cognitive-mobility associations under both the task conditions is critical because both cognition and mobility change with age and pose significant risks associated with falls, morbidity, and disability.
Methods: Using a cross-sectional, prospective study design, data from 124 healthy adults [mean age (SD) = 61.51 (11.90); mean education (SD) = 15.94 (2.18)] were collected. A comprehensive battery of cognitive tests was administered, and gait was assessed using a small, lightweight, three-axis accelerometer with a gyroscope.
Analytical Plan: Data were transformed, and only relatively strong relationships survived after strict statistical criteria adjusting for multiple comparisons were applied. Spearman rho correlation coefficients were used to examine the matrix of correlations between the cognitive-motor variables while adjusting for age and gender.
Results: Executive functions, processing speed, and language were associated with distinct facets of variability, pace, and asymmetry, especially under the dual-task walking condition. Both turns and transitions were also associated with cognition during the Timed Up and Go Task.
Conclusion: Our results extend converging evidence of the involvement of executive functions and processing speed in specific aspects of mobility, along with the role of language. The study has important implications for aging in terms of both assessment and rehabilitation of cognition and gait as well as for the emerging dual-tasking theories and the role of the neural pathways involved in mobility.
Introduction
Cognition and mobility are both multifaceted. Whereas cognitive functions can be classified into executive function, memory, attention, language, and processing speed (Lezak et al., 2004), gait – a predominant aspect of mobility – can be subdivided into pace, rhythm, asymmetry, and variability. Transitions and turns reflect additional, independent and putatively distinct mobility domains. There is compelling evidence that these cognitive functions and mobility domains are interrelated, especially among older adults, however, systematic study of their inter-relationships is lacking.
Key components of mobility such as gait, transitions (e.g., sit to stand, stand to sit), and turns have all been related to cognition. For example, older adults with better executive function walked with increased gait speed (Yogev-Seligmann et al., 2008; Morris et al., 2016), and those with slower processing speed had reduced rhythm (Martin et al., 2012). Both executive function and processing speed are susceptible to age-related changes (Brickman et al., 2007; Glisky, 2007; Salthouse, 2010), while decrements in gait integrity (such as stability) are associated with increasing age (Terrier and Reynard, 2015). Moreover, prospective studies have shown that among older adults, changes in certain aspects of gait predict cognitive decline and dementia, while the converse has also been observed with executive function and memory (i.e., mostly verbal memory) predicting a decline in gait (Yogev-Seligmann et al., 2008; Montero-Odasso et al., 2012; Morris et al., 2016). Nonetheless, several studies have suggested that the exact nature of this cognitive-mobility relationship still needs to be fully elucidated (Morris et al., 2016; Montero-Odasso et al., 2018). One such approach is to examine these associations in a relatively large cohort of healthy individuals consisting of diverse age-ranges. If the findings of cognitive-mobility associations across diverse age-ranges parallel those previously found in older adults, clinicians, and researchers can then better understand the mechanisms associated with gait, especially given that early changes in cognition and gait have the potential to identify those at risk for falls and cognitive decline (Montero-Odasso et al., 2012).
The cognitive-mobility association can be influenced by the task condition, e.g., simple walking without any interference (single task condition) or walking while performing another task such as verbal fluency (dual-task condition) (Yogev-Seligmann et al., 2008). Dual-tasking is important to study for several reasons. Mechanistically, it can be used to probe the automaticity of gait. Clinically, it can be used to unmask subclinical changes before an actual diagnosis and, predict critical outcomes such as falls and disability (Beauchet et al., 2009; Verghese et al., 2012; Li et al., 2018). A systematic study of the relationship between cognitive function and mobility while taking into account the multi-faceted nature of these functions, with and without dual-tasking, is lacking.
In contrast to the many studies that have reported on the decline in gait speed during dual-tasking (Beauchet et al., 2009; Verghese et al., 2012), there is limited evidence describing the specific one-to-one relationships that link cognition and mobility taking into account both single and dual-tasking. Pace and the stride-to-stride variability of gait, along with acceleration measures of transitions and turns have been related to attention and processing speed (Maki and McIlroy, 2007; Lord et al., 2013; Mirelman et al., 2014). Deficits in executive function and processing speed were associated with increased falls in community-dwelling older adults (Herman et al., 2010), and those with clinical impairments such as Parkinson’s disease (Mirelman et al., 2013). In contrast, others reported that rhythm was related to memory (i.e., verbal memory) and that pace was more restricted to executive function, memory, and language (Holtzer et al., 2012). A recent review indicated that asymmetry was not associated with any cognitive domain and that rhythm was linked to processing speed, but consistent associations were not found between gait variability and cognition (Morris et al., 2016), despite previous work which suggests otherwise. Such diverse findings are obfuscated by the nature of the gait variables included in a specific study (e.g., emphasis on the pace, i.e., gait speed, domain but not others), the types of cognitive domains studied (i.e., inconsistencies in the cognitive battery used across studies), and the nature of the dual-task selected for study. In addition, the cognitive-mobility relationships varied with respect to the specific cohort in question. For example, one study found that pace was compromised to a higher extent in patients with non-amnestic MCI having executive function, attention and language deficits as compared to amnestic MCI patients (Verghese et al., 2008), whereas other studies found contrasting associations between pace and executive function among healthy older adults (Morris et al., 2016).
There is, therefore, a pressing need to systematically and simultaneously examine multiple facets of both cognition and mobility under different task conditions, i.e., single and dual-task walking using a spectrum of tests to measure multiple facets of cognition. The current study sought to parse out the differential associations in a healthy cohort by generating a relatively large matrix of the cognitive and motor measurements. Such an approach can improve the sensitivity to detect relationships between mobility and cognition.
Materials and Methods
Study Design and Participants
Data were collected prospectively from community-dwelling participants who were recruited through random market mailings. Following established procedures, participants were required to be native English speakers, strongly right-handed, and have a minimum of fourth-grade reading level (Stern et al., 2014). They were also screened for MRI contraindications, and hearing and visual impairment that would interfere with testing. Older participants were assessed for dementia or MCI using the Dementia Rating Scale (DRS). The Internal Review Board of the College of Physicians and Surgeons of Columbia University approved this study. Prior to the testing session, written informed consent was obtained from all participants and compensation was provided at the end of the study.
Cognitive Measures
Four domains of cognition, i.e., executive function, memory, language, and processing speed, were measured using paper-and-pencil tests, and computerized tasks from the Reference Ability Neural Network Study (Stern et al., 2014). These domains were selected based on the results of latent constructs identified in large-scale studies of cognition across the adult lifespan (Salthouse, 2009). Table 1 displays the specific cognitive measures in each of the four domains. Further details about the tests can be found elsewhere (Kramer et al., 2014; Stern et al., 2014; Gazes et al., 2016; Habeck et al., 2016).
Mobility Measures
A small, lightweight, three-axis accelerometer with a gyroscope (DynaPort; McRoberts, The Hague, Netherlands) was worn on the lower back to quantify gait and the TUG test. The gait assessment included walking along a 20-m-long corridor for 1 min under two conditions: (1) preferred, usual-walking speed, and (2) dual-tasking, reciting words that start with the letter A while walking. The mean stride time, gait speed, stride length, stride and step time variability, stride regularity, step regularity, and step symmetry were quantified as previously described (Mirelman et al., 2013; Herman et al., 2014). The TUG test consisted of standing up from a chair, walking 3 m to a designated location at a normal pace, turning around, walking back, and sitting back down on the same chair. Acceleration signals were derived from three axes: vertical, mediolateral, and anterior–posterior. Angular velocities were derived from the gyroscope as yaw (rotation around the vertical axis), pitch (rotation around the mediolateral axis), and roll (rotation around the anterior–posterior axis) to derive quantitative measures for four subtasks: sit-to-stand and stand-to-sit transitions, walking, and turning (Weiss et al., 2011; Mirelman et al., 2014). Mobility measures were classified into six domains, transitions and turn domains from the TUG test, and variability, asymmetry, rhythm, and pace domains from the gait assessments (Lord et al., 2012).
Statistical Analyses
Non-linear transformations were applied using a semi-automated R shiny application (Shachar et al., 2018). For the list of the full transformations applied to the cognitive and mobility variables, see Supplementary Table 1. In cases where an order reversing transformation was used, the transformed values were multiplied by −1. Hereafter, outliers were detected while adjusting for age and gender. The outliers were determined according to Bonferroni p-values for Studentized residuals (based on a t-test) that are smaller than 4/n (Weisberg, 2014). For the list of outliers removed, see Supplementary Table 2. In addition, to circumvent the problem of a limited range of values, we excluded variables on which participants obtained similar values with 80% frequency.
Spearman rho correlation coefficients were used to examine the correlations between the cognitive-motor variables while adjusting for age and gender. Specifically, the correlation between each motor variable and each grouping of cognitive variables by the same domain (e.g., executive function, language, speed, and memory) were determined as a group of hypotheses. Therefore, the number of hypothesis groups was equal to the number of motor variables multiplied by the number of cognitive domains, and the number of correlations within each hypothesis group was equal to the number of cognitive variables belonging to the same domain. The statistical analysis was computed in two stages. First, motor variables and cognitive domains were selected if the Simes p-value (Simes, 1986) calculated on all Spearman correlations belonged to the same cognitive and motor domains (hypothesis group), was smaller than 0.05. Second, p-values belonging to a chosen motor variable-cognitive domain group of hypotheses were tested with adjustment to control the false discovery rate (FDR), using the Benjamini-Hochberg (BH) method, at a level of 0.05 (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995). This was performed for each of the chosen hypothesis groups separately. All statistical analyses were done using R software version 3.4.3.
Results
Participant Characteristics
One hundred and twenty-four, healthy adults were recruited from the community as a part of a large, ongoing study. Participants’ characterized are summarized in Table 2. Gait speed was an average of 0.93 (SD = 0.2) m/s. To illustrate the range of scores in our cohort, the scatterplots of the cognitive-mobility variables that were significantly correlated with each other are presented in Figures 1–3.
Figure 1. Scatterplots and the 95% confidence intervals (in gray) of Spearman’s rank correlation between executive functions and mobility. Relationship between (A) WAIS-3 BD (total correct) and dual task walk time stride variability, (B) CPT (total ratio) and Single task walk step symmetry, (C) Paper folding (proportion on time correct) and sit to stand range anterior–posterior, (D) Paper Folding (proportion on time correct) and turn yaw duration, (E) matrix reasoning (proportion on time correct) and turn yaw duration, and (F) letter set and turn yaw duration. Adjusted values reflect correction for multiple comparisons.
Figure 2. Scafterplots and the 95% confidence intervals (in gray) of Spearman’s rank correlation between language and mobility. Relationship between (A) WAIS-R vocabulary and dual task walk stride length, (B) category fluency (total correct) and dual task walk stride regularity, (C) category fluency (total correct) and dual task walk stride time variability, and (D) picture naming (proportion on time correct) and dual task walk stride time variability. Adjusted p-values reflect correction for multiple comparisons.
Figure 3. Scatterplots and the 95% confidence intervals (in gray) of Spearman’s rank correlation between processing speed and mobility. Relationship between (A) WAIS-R DS (total correct) and dual task walk stride regularity, (B) WAIS-R DS (total correct) and dual task walk step regularity, (C) Digit symbol (median correct RT) and dual task walk step regularity, and (D) TMT-A (time in seconds) and turn yaw duration. Adjusted p-values reflect correction for multiple comparisons.
Associations of Cognition With Mobility
Tables 3, 4 provide information on the mean (SD) and ranges for the mobility and cognitive measures, respectively. Table 5 summarizes the correlation results of the cognitive-motor relationships. Briefly, executive function was inversely correlated with turn duration (p = 0.048), range of sit-to-stand (p = 0.031), dual-task stride time variability (p = 0.042), and dual-task step asymmetry (p = 0.033). Language was correlated with dual-task stride time variability (p = 0.022), dual-task stride regularity (p = 0.030), and most strongly with dual-tasking pace (i.e., stride length) (p = 0.001). Processing speed was associated with dual-task step regularity and dual-task stride regularity (p < 0.05) and turn duration (p = 0.024). Memory was not significantly related to any of the mobility measures (p > 0.06).
Discussion
Using a relatively comprehensive set of cognitive-mobility measures and a rather conservative statistical approach, we observed two striking results. First, in line with previous studies, executive function and processing speed were predominantly related to gait variability and turns, while memory was not significantly related to any of the mobility domains (Hausdorff et al., 2005; Iersel et al., 2008; Martin et al., 2012). Secondly, language appears to play a relatively strong role across different gait variability measures, specifically during the dual task condition.
In contrast to the current findings, two previous studies from the same group of researchers found that memory was associated with mobility. Specifically, memory was related to decreased gait speed during both single- and dual-task conditions (Holtzer et al., 2006; Tripathi et al., 2019). Based on these previous studies, one may argue that the memory tests used in the current study were not adequately representative of the memory domain to detect associations (Stern et al., 2014). However, studies using other memory tests such as the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test, the Rey Complex Figure test, and the Paired Associates Learning test also did not find independent associations between gait and memory (Hausdorff et al., 2005; Iersel et al., 2008; Martin et al., 2012). Population characteristics may be a partial explanatory factor as both of the previous studies (Holtzer et al., 2006; Tripathi et al., 2019) included older adults with comorbid conditions (e.g., arthritis, hypertension, diabetes, and a few with neurological diagnosis such as MCI and stroke), while we focused on healthy subjects. Another possible explanation is that the cognitive demand associated with the task itself and the instructions given during the task may be intrinsically different and load differently on specific cognitive abilities. Specifically, explicit instructions to focus on both on walking and the task of reciting alternate letters of the alphabet used by Holtzer et al. (2006) and Tripathi et al.’s (2019) studies may require a higher level of episodic and working memory resources versus generating words that begin with a specific letter which may load more on executive function and working memory. However, this explanation is speculative and needs to be empirically evaluated. The common finding for the lack of association between memory and gait found across most studies is based on the idea that walking, by itself, is a complex activity requiring minimal aspects of memory functioning (Hausdorff et al., 2005). As such, walking relies on executive functioning and multitasking to a larger extent than other aspects of cognition. Indeed, the relationship of memory to gait is complex especially once a non-walking task (for the dual-task condition) is introduced.
For executive functions, differential relationships were found with mobility such that it was associated with dual-task condition stride time variability, single-task condition step symmetry, and turn duration and sit-to-stand transition during the TUG task. Dual-task walking and turns are recognized to be complex motor functions with relatively higher cognitive demands. Both walking during the dual-task condition and, transitioning and turning during the single-task condition can thus make walking less safe and increase the risk of falls (Yogev-Seligmann et al., 2008; Herman et al., 2010; Maidan et al., 2017). While turns were associated with several executive function tasks, only one executive function task was related to transition during a sit to stand task. Transitioning from a sitting to standing position requires a series of complex motor skills such as moving forward with the body while seated, accelerating in the vertical plane while pushing upward from the seat and moving the body upward, and then slowing the momentum to achieve stability to stand (Janssen et al., 2002; Mirelman et al., 2014). Comparatively, turning requires a series of other complex motor skills involving coordination of the limbs, aligning and stabilizing one’s posture with various aspects of gait, and manipulating specific subsets of movements are required. Additionally, turning also requires greater involvement of visual processing along with intact balance and spatial perception (Mirelman et al., 2014). Studies have found that while both transitions and turns are associated with executive functioning, turns are additionally associated with processing speed, and visuo-spatial perception (Herman et al., 2011; Mirelman et al., 2014; Mancini et al., 2016; Mellone et al., 2016). Overall, our findings on executive functions and mobility suggest that, in healthy individuals, relatively lowered cognitive performance may be linked to increased risk of gait alterations during the performance of these complex motor functions, or that lowered cognition may represent a higher vulnerability to gait disturbances. While cause and effect cannot be evaluated in this cross-sectional analyses, the present findings, consistent with previous studies (Brustio et al., 2017), highlight the relationship between executive functions and specific aspects of mobility.
Regarding language, category fluency and confrontational naming were associated with various aspects of dual-task stride time variability and dual-task stride regularity, whereas vocabulary showed the strongest associations with dual-tasking pace (see Table 5). Previous studies have consistently found language to be associated with gait characteristics that load on the pace domain (Holtzer et al., 2006, 2012; Duff et al., 2007; Morris et al., 2016). Language, specifically relating to vocabulary tests, represent crystallized knowledge and generally remain stable or improve with age (Habeck et al., 2016). Vocabulary is generally used as one of the proxy measures of cognitive reserve (Stern, 2012). The strong association between vocabulary and gait may be linked to cognitive reserve. However, this requires further empirical testing. It is important to note that the nature of the dual task used in the present study was language-based and may, therefore, be driving the associations between language and mobility. Perhaps, if a non-language based task was selected (such as a visual scanning, digit span, auditory scanning task or serial subtractions) the results may change. Nevertheless, the language-based task often chosen for the dual-task condition across studies is intended to mimic real-life, wherein individuals frequently talk while walking.
For processing speed, both paper-and-pencil and computerized versions of the digit symbol tasks were associated with stride time variability and gait asymmetry under dual-task conditions (see Table 5), whereas a processing speed task involving components of visual scanning and mental switching was associated with turn duration during the TUG test. Previous studies have also found processing speed to be associated with various aspects of gait including pace, postural control and rhythm (Morris et al., 2016). The association between processing speed and various gait characteristics is not surprising given the greater reliance of both these functions on shared motor pathways. In support of this finding, recent papers using functional neuroimaging techniques have found that walking under single- and dual-task conditions was similarly associated with sensorimotor, vestibular and visual networks, with greater connectivity in the left fronto-parietal network including the supplementary motor areas during the dual-task condition (Yuan et al., 2015).
Interestingly, the majority of the cognitive-mobility associations were related to dual-task conditions and to turning duration. This finding suggests that in healthy individuals the association between cognition and gait is dependent on the complexity of the task. Perhaps in the simple, single-task condition, gait is relatively more automatic and less cognitively demanding, at least among healthy adults. However, with a cognitive load (e.g., talking while walking, during turns), both cognitive and mobility processes become impaired (Odonkor et al., 2013). One relatively straightforward explanation of the cognitive-motor dual-tasking effect is the capacity sharing theory. This theory proposes that simultaneously performing two attention-demanding tasks will cause the performance of one or both of the tasks to suffer due to limited information processing (Li and Lindenberger, 2002; Li et al., 2005, 2018). A second explanation, as detailed in a recent review paper (Li et al., 2018), proposes an explanation related to the Principle of Neural Overlap – viz.-a-viz., during the dual-task condition, both the motor and cognitive components of the task may engage shared neural pathways, thus leading to overall reduced costs. A third related explanation comes from functional neuroimaging studies which have found that increased bilateral activation of the prefrontal regions was associated with lowered performance during the dual-task condition, while increased left prefrontal region activation was linked with better performance (Li et al., 2018). Complementing these findings, a recent structural neuroimaging study (Tripathi et al., 2019) found that single and dual-task conditions were differentially involved with brain networks (gait during the single task condition was associated with supplementary motor area, precuneus cortex, and the middle frontal gyrus, whereas dual-task gait with medial prefrontal, cingulate, and thalamic regions).
In summary, executive function, perceptual speed, and language were associated with specific facets of mobility, especially during dual-tasking. These associations suggest that different combinations of physiological/cognitive systems are operating to regulate specific mobility domains. For example, different connectivity for specific aspects of mobility such as gait speed and gait variability have been identified (Lo et al., 2017; Tripathi et al., 2019). The current findings lend itself to further exploration of different brain pathways. Specifically, the overlapping association of stride regularity during a dual task with language and processing speed suggests the involvement of a different cortical circuity as compared to that involving the association of turn duration with executive function and processing speed. Research with specific biomarkers or clinical conditions affecting different cortical systems could shed light on whether the motor versus cognitive pathways are parallel, overlapping/intersecting, or causal. For example, it is known that slow walking speed is one of the features of frailty (Fried et al., 2001). The combination of specific gait and cognitive measures may aid in the early identification of the multisystem dysregulation associated with frailty. Another approach could be to compare the brain regions along with the cognitive-mobility phenotypes in known patient groups (e.g., Parkinson’s disease versus progressive supranuclear palsy) to better understand the role of shared substrates in these clinical syndromes (Hong et al., 2015). Additionally, a better understanding of the possible mediators associated with cognitive reserve and brain reserve on these cognitive-motor correlations may shed light on possible interventions that can impact these relations. One can hypothesize, for example, that brain reserve may contribute to inter-individual variation in cognition, and these may differentially affect the cognitive-motor associations. Similarly, the cognitive-motor associations might be influenced by the differential ability to cope with brain changes, which would implicate cognitive reserve. Future work that extends the present findings to examine the roles of cognitive and brain reserve will be informative.
Conclusion
In the current study, we investigated a complex matrix of cognitive-mobility relationships in healthy adults in order to identify unique patterns. Our results, based on rather strict statistical criteria to avoid false positive findings, extend previous findings highlighting the role of executive functioning, language, and processing speed in dual-task walking conditions and for both turns and transitions that are proposed to be putatively independent mobility domains. Given the converging line of evidence, we have now increased the confidence in the findings of cognitive-mobility associations among healthy adults. The inclusion of other cognitive domains such as visuospatial functioning in future studies can help further elucidate these patterns. It will also be interesting to contrast the results of the present study with those using a global composite measure of cognition.
This present analyses was based on a cross-sectional study. Although gait and cognition are apparently interdependent, the study design makes it challenging to infer causality. More research across various clinical conditions and with longitudinal and experimental study designs will help to shed light on this issue. Another interesting line of work can also seek to examine the neuroimaging correlates and cortical control of these cognitive-mobility relationships. Additionally, the influence of possible genetic and environmental factors on cognition, mobility, and their relationships should be carefully investigated in follow-up investigations. Future studies may consider using a more stratified approach to investigating the cognition-mobility relationships by examining subdomains of cognition, such as contextual versus non-contextual verbal memory, verbal versus non-verbal memory, and immediate versus delayed memory. Nevertheless, the current study provides insight into specific features of motor-cognitive interactions and their impact per domain in aging and sets the stage for these future studies.
The implications of our finding are especially important for older adults. In aging and disease, as both cognitive functions and mobility decline, these two factors often interact with each other and increase the risk of adverse events and conditions (e.g., falls, frailty, disability, cognitive decline). Therefore, it becomes imperative to provide specific targeted interventions to improve cognitive, motor function and their inter-relationships that can subsequently result in improved functional mobility and cognition. Ultimately, the nature of such findings can improve the detection of early mobility and cognitive changes in older adults, and thereby enhance the development of novel therapies for those with clinical impairments.
Data Availability
The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.
Ethics Statement
The Internal Review Board of the College of Physicians and Surgeons of Columbia University approved this study. Prior to the testing session, written informed consent was obtained from all participants and compensation was provided at the end of the study.
Author Contributions
PS, IM, ZA, JH, AM, and YS conceived the concept and rationale for the study. ZA collected the data. PS, YS, IM, and TK analyzed the data. PS, IM, JH, AM, and YS interpreted the results and drafted the manuscript. All authors participated in the approval of the final version of the manuscript and take responsibility for the content and interpretation of this manuscript.
Funding
This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health (NIH) (Grant Nos. R01 AG026158, RF1AG038465, and F32AG053035).
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
We thank the research participants and assistants for their time and effort.
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnagi.2019.00166/full#supplementary-material
Abbreviations
DT, dual task; MCI, mild cognitive impairment; TUG, timed up and go.
References
Beauchet, O., Annweiler, C., Dubost, V., Allali, G., Kressig, R., Bridenbaugh, S., et al. (2009). Stops walking when talking: a predictor of falls in older adults? Eur. J. Neurol. 16, 786–795. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-1331.2009.02612.x
Benjamini, Y., and Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Statist. Soc. B 57, 289–300.
Brickman, A. M., Habeck, C., Zarahn, E., Flynn, J., and Stern, Y. (2007). Structural MRI covariance patterns associated with normal aging and neuropsychological functioning. Neurobiol. Aging 28, 284–295.
Brustio, P. R., Magistro, D., Zecca, M., Rabaglietti, E., and Liubicich, M. E. (2017). Age-related decrements in dual-task performance: comparison of different mobility and cognitive tasks. a cross sectional study. PLoS One 12:e0181698. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0181698
Duff, K., Mold, J. W., and Roberts, M. M. (2007). Walking speed and global cognition: results from the OKLAHOMA Study. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 15, 31–39.
Fried, L. P., Tangen, C. M., Walston, J., Newman, A. B., Hirsch, C., Gottdiener, J., et al. (2001). Frailty in older adults: evidence for a phenotype. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 56, M146–M157.
Gazes, Y., Bowman, F. D., Razlighi, Q. R., O’shea, D., Stern, Y., and Habeck, C. (2016). White matter tract covariance patterns predict age-declining cognitive abilities. NeuroImage 125, 53–60. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.016
Glisky, E. L. (2007). Changes in cognitive function in human aging in source in Brain Aging Models, Methods, and Mechanisms, ed D. R. Riddle , Boca Raton FL: CRC Press/Taylor & Francis, 3–20.
Habeck, C., Gazes, Y., Razlighi, Q., Steffener, J., Brickman, A., Barulli, D., et al. (2016). The reference ability neural network study: life-time stability of reference-ability neural networks derived from task maps of young adults. NeuroImage 125, 693–704. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.077
Hausdorff, J. M., Yogev, G., Springer, S., Simon, E. S., and Giladi, N. (2005). Walking is more like catching than tapping: gait in the elderly as a complex cognitive task. Exp. Brain Res. 164, 541–548.
Herman, T., Giladi, N., and Hausdorff, J. M. (2011). Properties of the ‘timed up and go’test: more than meets the eye. Gerontology 57, 203–210. doi: 10.1159/000314963
Herman, T., Mirelman, A., Giladi, N., Schweiger, A., and Hausdorff, J. M. (2010). Executive control deficits as a prodrome to falls in healthy older adults: a prospective study linking thinking, walking, and falling. J. Gerontol. A Biomed. Sci. Med. Sci. 65, 1086–1092. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glq077
Herman, T., Weiss, A., Brozgol, M., Giladi, N., and Hausdorff, J. M. (2014). Gait and balance in Parkinson’s disease subtypes: objective measures and classification considerations. J. Neurol. 261, 2401–2410.
Holtzer, R., Verghese, J., Xue, X., and Lipton, R. B. (2006). Cognitive processes related to gait velocity: results from the Einstein Aging Study. Neuropsychology 20, 215–223.
Holtzer, R., Wang, C., and Verghese, J. (2012). The relationship between attention and gait in aging: facts and fallacies. Motor Control 16, 64–80.
Hong, J. Y., Yun, H. J., Sunwoo, M. K., Ham, J. H., Lee, J.-M., Sohn, Y. H., et al. (2015). Comparison of regional brain atrophy and cognitive impairment between pure akinesia with gait freezing and Richardson’s syndrome. Front. Aging Neurosci. 7:180. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00180
Iersel, M. B. V., Kessels, R. P., Bloem, B. R., Verbeek, A. L., and Olde Rikkert, M. G. (2008). Executive functions are associated with gait and balance in community-living elderly people. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 63, 1344–1349.
Janssen, W. G., Bussmann, H. B., and Stam, H. J. (2002). Determinants of the sit-to-stand movement: a review. Phys. Ther. 82, 866–879.
Kramer, J. H., Mungas, D., Possin, K. L., Rankin, K. P., Boxer, A. L., Rosen, H. J., et al. (2014). NIH EXAMINER: conceptualization and development of an executive function battery. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 20, 11–19. doi: 10.1017/S1355617713001094
Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., Loring, D. W., and Fischer, J. S. (2004). Neuropsychological Assessment. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Li, K. Z., Bherer, L., Mirelman, A., Maidan, I., and Hausdorff, J. M. (2018). cognitive involvement in balance. gait and dual-tasking in aging: a focused review from a neuroscience of aging perspective. Front. Neurol. 9:913. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2018.00913
Li, K. Z., Krampe, R. T., and Bondar, A. (2005). “An ecological approach to studying aging and dual-task performance,” in Cognitive Limitations in Aging and Psychopathology, eds R. W. Engle, G. Sedek, U. von Hecker, and D. N. McIntosh (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 190–218.
Li, K. Z., and Lindenberger, U. (2002). Relations between aging sensory/sensorimotor and cognitive functions. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 26, 777–783.
Lo, O.-Y., Halko, M. A., Zhou, J., Harrison, R., Lipsitz, L. A., and Manor, B. (2017). Gait speed and gait variability are associated with different functional brain networks. Front. Aging Neurosci. 9:390. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00390
Lord, S., Galna, B., and Rochester, L. (2013). Moving forward on gait measurement: toward a more refined approach. Mov. Dis. 28, 1534–1543. doi: 10.1002/mds.25545
Lord, S., Galna, B., Verghese, J., Coleman, S., Burn, D., and Rochester, L. (2012). Independent domains of gait in older adults and associated motor and nonmotor attributes: validation of a factor analysis approach. J. Gerontol. A Biomed. Sci. Med. Sci. 68, 820–827. doi: 10.1093/gerona/gls255
Maidan, I., Bernad-Elazari, H., Giladi, N., Hausdorff, J. M., and Mirelman, A. (2017). When is higher level cognitive control needed for locomotor tasks among patients with Parkinson’s disease? Brain Topogr. 30, 531–538.
Maki, B. E., and McIlroy, W. E. (2007). Cognitive demands and cortical control of human balance-recovery reactions. J. Neural Trans. 114, 1279–1296.
Mancini, M., Schlueter, H., El-Gohary, M., Mattek, N., Duncan, C., Kaye, J., et al. (2016). Continuous monitoring of turning mobility and its association to falls and cognitive function: a pilot study. J. Gerontol. A Biomed. Sci. Med. Sci. 71, 1102–1108. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glw019
Martin, K. L., Blizzard, L., Wood, A. G., Srikanth, V., Thomson, R., Sanders, L. M., et al. (2012). Cognitive function, gait, and gait variability in older people: a population-based study. J. Gerontol. A Biomed. Sci. Med. Sci. 68, 726–732.
Mellone, S., Mancini, M., King, L. A., Horak, F. B., and Chiari, L. (2016). The quality of turning in Parkinson’s disease: a compensatory strategy to prevent postural instability? J. Neuroeng. Rehab. 13:39. doi: 10.1186/s12984-016-0147-4
Mirelman, A., Heman, T., Yasinovsky, K., Thaler, A., Gurevich, T., Marder, K., et al. (2013). Fall risk and gait in Parkinson’s disease: the role of the LRRK2 G2019S mutation. Mov. Dis. 28, 1683–1690. doi: 10.1002/mds.25587
Mirelman, A., Weiss, A., Buchman, A. S., Bennett, D. A., Giladi, N., and Hausdorff, J. M. (2014). Association between performance on timed up and go subtasks and mild cognitive impairment: further insights into the links between cognitive and motor function. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 62, 673–678. doi: 10.1111/jgs.12734
Montero-Odasso, M., Almeida, Q. J., Bherer, L., Burhan, A. M., Camicioli, R., Doyon, J., et al. (2018). Consensus on shared measures of mobility and cognition: from the canadian consortium on neurodegeneration in aging (CCNA). J. Gerontol. A 74, 897–909. doi: 10.1093/gerona/gly148
Montero-Odasso, M., Verghese, J., Beauchet, O., and Hausdorff, J. M. (2012). Gait and cognition: a complementary approach to understanding brain function and the risk of falling. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 60, 2127–2136. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2012.04209.x
Morris, R., Lord, S., Bunce, J., Burn, D., and Rochester, L. (2016). Gait and cognition: mapping the global and discrete relationships in ageing and neurodegenerative disease. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 64, 326–345. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.02.012
Odonkor, C. A., Thomas, J. C., Holt, N., Latham, N., VanSwearingen, J., Brach, J. S., et al. (2013). A comparison of straight-and curved-path walking tests among mobility-limited older adults. J. Gerontol. A Biomed. Sci. Med. Sci. 68, 1532–1539. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glt060
Salthouse, T. A. (2009). Decomposing age correlations on neuropsychological and cognitive variables. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 15, 650–661. doi: 10.1017/S1355617709990385
Salthouse, T. A. (2010). Selective review of cognitive aging. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 16, 754–760.
Shachar, N., Mitelpunkt, A., Kozlovski, T., Galili, T., Frostig, T., Brill, B., et al. (2018). The importance of nonlinear transformations use in medical data analysis. JMIR Med. Inform. 6:e27. doi: 10.2196/medinform.7992
Simes, R. J. (1986). An improved Bonferroni procedure for multiple tests of significance. Biometrika 73, 751–754.
Stern, Y. (2012). Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet Neurol. 11, 1006–1012. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70191-6
Stern, Y., Habeck, C., Steffener, J., Barulli, D., Gazes, Y., Razlighi, Q., et al. (2014). The Reference Ability Neural Network Study: motivation, design, and initial feasibility analyses. Neuroimage 103, 139–151. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.09.029
Terrier, P., and Reynard, F. (2015). Effect of age on the variability and stability of gait: a cross-sectional treadmill study in healthy individuals between 20 and 69 years of age. Gait Post. 41, 170–174. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2014.09.024
Tripathi, S., Verghese, J., and Blumen, H. M. (2019). Gray matter volume covariance networks associated with dual-task cost during walking-while-talking. Hum. Brain Mapp. 40, 2229–2240. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24520
Verghese, J., Holtzer, R., Lipton, R. B., and Wang, C. (2012). Mobility stress test approach to predicting frailty, disability, and mortality in high-functioning older adults. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 60, 1901–1905. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2012.04145.x
Verghese, J., Robbins, M., Holtzer, R., Zimmerman, M., Wang, C., Xue, X., et al. (2008). Gait dysfunction in mild cognitive impairment syndromes. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 56, 1244–1251. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2008.01758.x
Weiss, A., Herman, T., Plotnik, M., Brozgol, M., Giladi, N., and Hausdorff, J. (2011). An instrumented timed up and go: the added value of an accelerometer for identifying fall risk in idiopathic fallers. Physiol. Measure. 32, 2003–2018. doi: 10.1088/0967-3334/32/12/009
Yogev-Seligmann, G., Hausdorff, J. M., and Giladi, N. (2008). The role of executive function and attention in gait. Mov. Dis. 23, 329–342.
Keywords: mobility, gait, cognition, aging, elderly, executive function, processing speed, vocabulary
Citation: Sunderaraman P, Maidan I, Kozlovski T, Apa Z, Mirelman A, Hausdorff JM and Stern Y (2019) Differential Associations Between Distinct Components of Cognitive Function and Mobility: Implications for Understanding Aging, Turning and Dual-Task Walking. Front. Aging Neurosci. 11:166. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2019.00166
Received: 18 February 2019; Accepted: 14 June 2019;
Published: 02 July 2019.
Edited by:
Ashok Kumar, University of Florida, United StatesReviewed by:
Bhaskar Roy, University of Alabama at Birmingham, United StatesAida Karachi, University of Florida, United States
Copyright © 2019 Sunderaraman, Maidan, Kozlovski, Apa, Mirelman, Hausdorff and Stern. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Yaakov Stern, eXMxMUBjb2x1bWJpYS5lZHU=