Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 25 June 2019
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior

Everyday Memory in Healthy Aging: Porous but Not Distorted

  • Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy II, Division of Mental Health & Old Age Psychiatry, University of Ulm, Ulm, Germany

Most studies targeting age-association of memory functions report a decline in recognition hits and an increase in false alarms. The goal of the present study was to assess these findings in tasks with day-to-day relevance. We investigated healthy young (YA; age 26.90 ± 3.55 years) and old (OA; age 69.80 ± 5.85 years) adults. Participants were asked to watch six news and six commercials and complete a recognition task relating to the information presented in the videos. OA had a lower hit rate in both news and commercials compared to YA. However, the number of false alarms (FA) was the same in both age groups. Applying signal detection theory, we found age differences in discriminability for both news and commercials paradigm. The groups showed no differences in bias and both chose a liberal answering tendency. We interpret our finding as a result of complex recognition items in an ecologically valid task. Multi-feature items offer an advantage in correct rejection—it is enough to know that at least one feature of an item is false. This benefit does not extend to hits, where all features of an item need to be recognized. This indicates that recognition memory of naturalistic stimuli in OA is porous, but not distorted.

Background

Aging goes along with a decline in various cognitive functions such as visual-constructive abilities, processing speed, and memory (Di Carlo et al., 2000; Park et al., 2002; MacDonald et al., 2004; Royall et al., 2005; Ardila, 2007). Changes in memory performance are not homogeneous and different aspects of memory change differently. Among memory deficits, episodic memory performance undergoes the most pronounced changes with increasing age (Souchay et al., 2000; Daselaar et al., 2003; Kramer et al., 2003; Nilsson, 2003; Salthouse, 2016). Concerning memory processes, literature suggests that the most drastic deterioration occurs in recall (Hultsch, 1969; Petersen et al., 1992; Hertzog et al., 2010), whereas recognition is only moderately affected (Jacoby, 1999; Ratcliff et al., 2004; Spaniol et al., 2006; Van Ocker et al., 2017).

Recognition errors comprise two kinds of errors, not recognizing studied items (misses), and falsely recognizing new unstudied items (false alarms, FA; Stanislaw and Todorov, 1999; Shapiro, 2014). A recent meta-analysis of 232 studies by Fraundorf et al. (2019) has reported that younger adults generally outperformed older adults (OA) in recognition tasks and that older adults showed a more liberal answering tendency resulting in an increase in false alarms. Interestingly, it was reported that age differences in recognition accuracy were largest for easy tasks. Furthermore, older adults memory showed an even greater detriment, when recognition task involved new combinations of already familiar items. The main effect of age on recognition memory persisted across different studies in spite of variability in learning material, emotional valance of the stimuli or retention interval, suggesting that recognition memory decline involves a general process operating across different modalities. An increase of erroneous memory content was also previously observed in different paradigms such as a recognition version of the Deese-Roediger-McDermott (DRM) procedure (Roediger and McDermott, 1995; Seamon et al., 2002), conceptual similarity (Paige et al., 2016), self-related information (Rosa et al., 2015), misinformation (Meade et al., 2012), and misattribution (Mitchell and Johnson, 2009).

While such paradigms are useful to test theoretical models and advance understanding of memory construct, it has been questioned if they can fully account for cognitive processes faced by healthy older and younger persons on a daily basis (Chaytor and Schmitter-Edgecombe, 2003). A recent study reported that DRM and other above mentioned tasks do not sufficiently predict memory outcomes and questioned the implications to use these paradigms to predict memory distortions in real-life situations (Patihis et al., 2018).

One way to bridge the gap between laboratory and ecologically valid paradigms would be to create assessments using verisimilitude approach, which refers to development of tests that are comprised of everyday cognitive tasks. Such instruments were developed for everyday skills such as attention (Robertson et al., 1994), executive function (Wilson et al., 1996) and some aspects of memory (Wilson et al., 2004, 2008). A study of memory for commercials reported that despite having less correct rejections than young adults (YA), older adults demonstrated better memory for emotionally meaningful content compared to neutral one (Fung and Carstensen, 2003). Another study by Mazurek et al. (2015) required healthy younger and older participants to hide objects in a room, which was later followed by a surprise memory task, where participants were asked to freely recall what, where, and when they had hidden. Results showed that older adults recalled significantly less full combinations of object, location and time, but memory for incomplete combinations (i.e., what-where, what-when, and where-when) did not differ between the two groups. Furthermore, the ability to bind the three features together varied significantly in the older adults group, with some individuals performing worse and some better than younger adults. The authors partly explained this finding through differences in memory strategies used by the participants—an ecologically valid task might make it easier for older adults to apply more efficient memory strategies, such as “mental travel,” which they rely upon in everyday life. The performance on the new memory task was predicted by the existing measures of episodic memory. Additionally and in contrast to existing memory measures, the performance on the new memory task was also predicted by the self-evaluation memory questionnaires. The authors suggested that such ecologically valid task could offer an advantage over conventional scales by picking up on very early signs of cognitive decline in older population.

Exposure to material frequently seen on TV, which often serves as a most common information acquisition source, has been studied previously. Daily exposure to material such as news or commercials influences our attitudes, preferences and decision making (Martin, 2003; Prior, 2003; Weitzer and Kubrin, 2004). For that reason, it is important to assess memory for this kind of information presentation across different age groups. Thus far, studies using real life paradigms of news or commercials in healthy populations focused primarily on brain activity (Frings et al., 2010), emotional salience (Fung and Carstensen, 2003) or pre-existing attitudes (Frenda et al., 2013) rather than false alarms. Although recognition memory using such everyday life material has already been investigated in the context of disorders directly affecting memory performance (Sejunaite et al., 2017, 2018), the pattern and causes of false alarms with healthy aging in everyday life remains vague.

Generally, the age-related decline in hits and the increase in false alarms are linked to changes in brain activity. Functional imaging studies reported an increased task-related activity in the left temporal, frontal, and posterior parietal cortex among older adults during memory tasks (Frings et al., 2010; Craik and Rose, 2012). This pattern was thought to reflect increased effort and decreased reliance on automatic effortless processing in older subjects compared to young adults. Two brain regions that seem to be affected the most by aging were medial temporal and prefrontal cortex (Buckner, 2004; Butler et al., 2004; Hedden and Gabrieli, 2004; Raz et al., 2005; Meade et al., 2012). They were associated with increased susceptibility to lure items in recognition (Plancher et al., 2009; Fandakova et al., 2013a) as well as increased misattribution of memory sources (Craik et al., 1990; Chan and McDermott, 2007; Fandakova et al., 2013b) respectively. Aging-related hippocampal volume reduction puts a strain on effective memory process requiring other cortical areas, such as prefrontal cortex, to compensate for the deficits (Cabeza, 2002; Cabeza et al., 2002; Persson et al., 2006). In contrast, older adults that were able to maintain memory processing patterns akin to those in young adults were reported to have fewer false recognitions (Fandakova et al., 2015).

Further support for the involvement of decreased frontal and medial temporal functioning in false recognitions comes from studies that compared memory performance with neuropsychological correlates. Better performance in memory measures such as free and cued recall was shown to contribute to binding separate features into complex memories and increase the number of hits (Henkel et al., 1998; McCabe et al., 2009). Higher scores on frontal function measures such as verbal fluency, working memory, abstract reasoning and problem solving were associated with less false alarms (Roediger and Geraci, 2007; McCabe et al., 2009) and better evaluation of contextual features of memory traces (Henkel et al., 1998).

To account for this discrepancy in memory performance with age, multiple explanations were proposed (Johnson and Raye, 1998; Roediger et al., 2001; Devitt and Schacter, 2016). Although theories explaining age-related increase in false alarms are numerous, three of them are viewed as the most prominent to explain age-associated factors (Healey and Kahana, 2016). Associative deficit hypothesis (Naveh-Benjamin, 2000; Old and Naveh-Benjamin, 2008) postulates that older adults are selectively impaired in the ability to bind separate features together into meaningful memory traces. This association deficit persists even when memory for features themselves is intact (Chalfonte and Johnson, 1996). This hypothesis helps to explain the source monitoring deficits frequently reported in older adults and often viewed as a distinct false memory theory. Inhibitory deficit hypothesis (Hasher and Zacks, 1988; Healey and Kahana, 2016) argues that age-related increase in false alarms arises due to decreased ability to inhibit irrelevant information. This hypothesis helps to explain the effects of interference (Hamm and Hasher, 1992; May and Hasher, 1998; Manard et al., 2014) as well as effects of priming (Madden, 1986; Ikier et al., 2008) on memory. Last, according to cognitive slowing hypothesis (Salthouse, 1996a, b), aging is associated with a general reduction in most cognitive abilities (Ardila et al., 2000; Park et al., 2002; Darowski et al., 2008; Zahodne et al., 2011). According to this theory, a diminished performance results from an age-associated decrease of the speed of cognitive processing—controlling for speed explains up to 70% of variance in memory tasks (Aminoff et al., 2012).

Taken together the literature suggests that older persons are more prone to false alarms. The studies, however, mostly used experimental paradigms loosely related to day-to-day situations. The goal of the present study was to investigate recognition memory with ecologically valid material (news and commercials) in healthy aging. We hypothesized that older adults would have more false alarms in an ecologically valid memory paradigm than young adults. Furthermore, we hypothesized that age differences would be explained by age-associated decline of performance on other cognitive tasks such as measures of frontal function.

Participants

A statistical power analysis was performed for sample size estimation, based on data from a similar recognition memory study for advertisements (Fung and Carstensen, 2003). Using the means and standard deviations for neutral material the calculated effect size d was 1.07. With an alpha = 0.05, and power = 0.90, the sample size estimated with this effect size is 16 per group for between-group comparisons. The present study is a follow-up study to two prior studies assessing false alarms in healthy older persons and older persons with depressive disorder or Alzheimer’s dementia (Sejunaite et al., 2017, 2018). The group of older persons in the present study was the same as in the prior studies. We, therefore, chose to use a similar sample size for the group with young healthy adults. The sample in the present study comprised a total of 21 young adults [YA; age 21–35 years; 26.90 ± 3.55 (Mean ± Standard Deviation); 13 females] and 20 older adults (OA; age 61–83 years; 69.80 ± 5.85; 10 females). Both, YA and OA consisted of volunteers recruited by local advertising to partake in a study on memory and aging. Central nervous system disorders such as neurodegenerative disorders and affective disorders as well as mental retardation and addictive behavior were ruled out by taking medical history. The exclusion criteria for Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) and Montgomery-Asberg-Depression-Rating-Scale (MADRS) were <28 and >9, respectively.

Materials and Methods

The present study was designed as a non-randomized, cross-sectional study. All participants were briefed about the study design. The neuropsychological assessment as well as experimental sessions took place at the Division of Mental Health and Old Age Psychiatry of Ulm University. The study received approval of the local ethics committee and was done in accordance with the local ethical standards of the Ulm University and the guidelines outlined in the Declaration of Helsinki (World Medical Association, 2013).

Neuropsychological Assessments

Clinical Scales

Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE; Folstein et al., 1975).

The MMSE is a widely used instrument to give an overview over global cognitive functioning. It comprises questions on orientation, registration, short-term memory, language use, comprehension, and basic motor skills. The score ranges from 0 to 30, with a score below 24 indicating a cognitive impairment.

Montgomery-Asberg-Depression-Rating-Scale (MADRS; Montgomery and Asberg, 1989)

The score in the MADRS reflects the affective state of the examinee as assessed by a health care professional and consists of 10 aspects to be evaluated: apparent sadness, communicated sadness, inner tension, sleep, appetite, concentration, impetus, callousness, pessimistic thoughts and suicidal ideation. Each of the aspects is given a score from 0 to 6 according to its severity. The total score ranges from 0 to 60. Scores 0–8 indicate no depression, 8–16 a mild, 16–24 a moderate, and 24 and higher a severe depression.

Neuropsychological Tests

Vocabulary Test (Wortschatztest, WST; Schmidt and Metzler, 1992).

In the Wortschatztest (WST), the examinee needs to find an actual word among five non-word distractors. The word list among which the actual word needs to be chosen increases in difficulty as the test progresses. The number of correct answers (maximum 40) is counted and the raw values are converted into IQ scores.

California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT; Niemann et al., 2008).

The California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT) is a verbal memory test, assessing variables such as immediate recall, free and cued recall after short delay, free and cued recall after long delay as well as recognition. A list of 16 words (four words of each category: fruit, clothing, drinks, tools) is read to the participant a total of five times (CVLT 1 … CVLT 5). After each round, the participant is encouraged to recall as many words as possible. Immediate recall is followed by a free and cued delayed recall after 5 (CLT short delay) and 20 min (CVLT long delay) intervals respectively, and a Yes/No recognition task (CVLT recognition).

Digit and Visual Span (Wechsler Memory Scale Revised, WMS-R; Härting et al., 2000).

The Digit Span test comprises digit span forward and digit span backward. In the digit span forward the participants are asked to repeat a sequence of digits until either the maximum number of eight digits per sequence is reached or until two consecutive incorrectly repeated sequences of same length. In the digit span backwards condition, the same procedure is applied with the task to repeat the digits backward. The same principle was implemented for the Visual Span using Corsi-block forward and backward. One point is given for each correct answer with scores ranging from 0 to 12 except for the forward visual span with scores ranging from 0 to 14.

Symbol Span (Wechsler Memory Scale—Fourth Edition, WMS-IV; Wechsler et al., 2012).

The Symbol Span subtest of the Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS)-IV assesses sequential working memory using abstract symbols as stimuli. The test requires recognizing previously presented symbols in their correct order. The number of symbols gradually increases from one to seven symbols, with each string of symbols being presented one after the other. After four consecutive errors, the test is terminated. Correctly recalled symbols in their right sequence are given 2 points, correct symbols in the wrong order are given 1 point, incorrectly recalled symbols are given 0 points.

Trail Making Tests A and B (TMT-A and TMT-B; Reitan and Wolfston, 1985).

The TMT are tests to assess visual attention and mental flexibility and requires an examinee to draw pencil lines in ascending order from 1 to 25 Trail Making Test A (TMT-A) and 25 encircled numbers and corresponding letters in an alternating order (TMT-B) that are randomly dispersed on a DIN-A-4 sheet. The discrepancy between the TMT-A and TMT-B (i.e., TMT-B minus TMT-A) is an indicator of deficits in mental flexibility. The instructions require working as fast as possible while maintaining maximum accuracy.

Fluency Tasks (Regensburg Verbal Fluency Test, RWT; Aschenbrenner et al., 2000).

RWT assesses semantic and phonetic verbal fluency. An examinee is instructed to generate as many words as possible in 1 min that belong to the category “animals” (semantic verbal fluency) as well as words starting with the letters “P” and “S” (phonemic fluency).

Experimental Paradigms

News and commercials were used as a memory task representing the daily memory processes. To assess the relevance of this task all participants were asked whether or not they regularly watch news and commercials. Fourteen and 20 of YA and OA respectively, regularly watch news programmes. Likewise, seven YA and six OA reported to watch commercials.

News

Six news videos were shown to the participants with each video being between 27 s and 39 s. All videos were selected from the same popular daily TV news show Tagesschau from the ARD-channel and were originally broadcasted between the 1980s and early 1990s. We selected old news to avoid familiarity bias. The news topics pertained to domestic affairs (skateboard safety, merging of state-run train companies, changes in TV licence fee, river pollution, shortage in vocational training places, ferryboat incident). All six videos had the same format with a speaker (three female and three male speakers) and extra information such as a photo or a map being shown in the background.

Commercials

Six commercials were shown to the participants with each video being between 25 s and 34 s. All clips were selected from the internet and were originally broadcasted between the 1990s and early 2000s. The content of the commercials pertained to groceries of the brands that are still on the market today (beer, flour, rice, chocolate, detergent and grocery retailer).

Recognition Task

A recognition task with 12 statements was designed for each video to assess the number of correctly retrieved memory content from the respective video. The task for each video was presented immediately after watching the respective video. Six out of 12 statements contained information actually presented in the video (signal) whereas the remaining six statements contained information that was made up by the investigators to assess the number of erroneous memory items (noise). Out of the six signal statements asking about actually present information, three of them contained original information, and the other three were negated. There were three possible answer choices: “Yes” (the statement is true and directly corresponds to the video), “No” (the statement is true, but negated), and “Unknown” (the information has not been addressed or shown in the video). Three examples of such statements and the scoring system are presented in Table 1. In contrast to conventional recognition memory paradigms that rely on two answer choices (usually “old item” vs. “new item”), we have introduced negated statements to diminish older adult’s reliance on gist (Schacter et al., 1997; Tun et al., 1998; Dennis et al., 2007) by encouraging a more conscious decision making process, which has been reported to reduce the number of false alarms in older adults (Multhaup, 1995; Grady and Craik, 2000). The original German questionnaires administered to the patients together with the links to the videos used in the study are available from the corresponding author upon request.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Scoring matrix for hits and false alarms used in the primary analysis.

At the end of the experimental part, participants were asked to evaluate subjective feeling of difficulty of the questions on a 5-point Likert scale with “1” being very easy and “5” being very difficult as well as to give a subjective estimate of how many questions they answered correctly and how many questions were non-answerable.

Procedure

Each participant completed a neuropsychological assessment prior to the experimental task. Before proceeding to the videos, participants were asked whether they watch news and commercials on a daily basis. Participants were then told that they will be shown short video clips of six news and six commercials, which will be followed a recognition task with three answer choices: (1) yes, the statement is true; (2) no, the statement is false; and (3) unknown, the information from the statement was not presented in the video. To illustrate the answer choices participants were given the following example: “Imagine that recognition statement says ‘The apple in video was red.’ If you remember seeing a red apple in the video, answer ‘yes.’ If you remember seeing a green apple in the video, answer ‘no.’ If you do not remember seeing an apple in the video at all, answer ‘unknown’.” After the instruction, participants proceeded to watch video clips. Recognition task for the respective video clip was presented immediately after the respective video. Answer choices were repeated prior to each recognition task. After watching all videos participants were asked to evaluate the perceived difficulty of the tasks and give an estimate of the number of correctly recognized items (hits) and correctly rejected false statements (correct rejections).

Data Analysis

All statistical analyses were carried out using the SPSS (SPSS 21.0 for Windows, Chicago, IL, USA, 2012). The normality of distribution of hits and false alarms as well as the neuropsychological measures was tested with the Kolmogorov-Smirnov Test for each group separately. There was a homogeneity of variance between OA and YA for all the variables except MADRS, TMT-A, CVLT long delay, CVLT long-delay, CVLT hits, CVLT false alarms and subjective estimate of hits in commercials as assessed by Levene’s Test for Equality of Variances. In case of unequal variances, degrees of freedom were adjusted. Group comparisons were calculated using t-test, correlations using Pearson’s correlation coefficient. Other statistical procedures were used as indicated. Apart from comparing raw scores, the number of hits and false alarms was used to calculate the discriminability (d’) and bias (C) according to Signal Detection theory (Stanislaw and Todorov, 1999). Effect sizes were calculated using Cohen’s d.

Results

There was a significant difference in the years of formal education among participants (YA 14.19 ± 2.14; OA 11.35 ± 2.60; t(39) = 3.830, p < 0.001). However, education in older persons in Germany is just a weak indicator of general intellectual abilities due to the broken biographies in the sequels of WWII in Germany. Hence, the difference is representative of the official statistics within the German population (Statistisches Bundesamt, 2016). However, IQ scores for both groups were within a normal range (IQ YA 114.67 ± 9.04; IQ OA 107.90 ± 10.71). Demographic variables are shown in Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Demographic and neuropsychological variables of young adults (YA) and older adults (OA).

Older persons had significantly lower scores for the vocabulary test, the MMSE, and significantly higher scores for the MADRS. Despite the statistical significance, all scores for both groups were within a clinically normal range.

Older persons performed worse in measures of visual span and working memory, executive function (TMT), and most aspects of verbal memory than younger persons (Table 2). No group differences were observed in digit span as well as semantic and phonemic verbal fluency.

An overview of the number of times YA and OA answered yes, no or unknown for each item type (i.e., original signal, reversed signal, noise) is presented in Table 3.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Means and Standard Deviations (M ± SD) for the number of responses given to each type of item.

Before calculating the group differences in recognition, we compared the subjective difficulty, subjective performance estimate and the news and commercials watching habits between the two groups (Table 4). While potential group differences of material relevance between YA and OA were not directly measured, we measured frequency of watching news and commercials and subjective difficulty. Our results show that OA watch news significantly more often (χ(1)2 = 8.039, p = 0.005) and subjectively perceived questions to the news material as significantly easier than YA (YA 3.48 ± 0.81, OA 2.84 ± 0.90; t(38) = 2.343, p = 0.024, effect size 0.84). Although there was no significant difference in the frequency of watching advertisements between the two groups (χ(1)2 = 0.053, p = 0.819), OA perceived the questions accompanying the advertisement videos as being significantly harder (YA 2.71 ± 0.72, OA 3.26 ± 0.87; t(38) = −2.183, p = 0.035, effect size 0.69).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Hits and false alarms on watching news and commercials in young adults (YA) and older adults (OA).

To control for the effect of commercials watching habits on the recognition outcome, we conducted a 2 (YA vs. OA) × 2 (watching commercials vs. not watching commercials) ANOVA with hits in commercials recognition task as a dependent variable. There was no significant interaction between the effects of age and commercials watching habits, F(1,37) = 0.345, p = 0.561. There was a significant main effect of age group F(1,37) = 8.619, p = 0.006; however not of the commercials watching habits F(1,37) = 0.526, p = 0.473. The same procedure was repeated with false alarms for commercials as a dependent variable. There was no significant interaction between age and commercials watching habits F(1,37) = 0.325, p = 0.572 and no significant main effect of either age (F(1,37) = 0.192, p = 0.664) or watching habits (F(1,37) = 0.377, p = 0.543) on false alarms. As all of the participants claimed to watch news regularly, our data did not allow us to look into the same effects for news; however, we assume that the influence of news and commercials watching habits on recognition memory works similarly in news as in commercials.

An overview of subjective and objective hits, false alarms is presented in Table 4. Subjective estimation of number of hits and correct rejections was comparable in YA and OA. YA had significantly more hits in the videos than OA in both, news and commercials. In contrast, the number of false alarms was similar in OA and YA.

Analysis of discriminability has revealed a significant difference in d′ between YA (1.26 ± 0.46) and OA (0.89 ± 0.55), t(39) = 2.339, p = 0.025 (effect size 0.73) in news and commercials recognition task, t(39) = 2.821, p = 0.007 (YA 1.49 ± 0.46; OA 1.08 ± 0.48; effect size 0.87).

Both OA and YA displayed a liberal answering strategy with mean C values being below zero for both groups in news (YA −0.19 ± 0.21; OA −0.20 ± 0.30, effect size 0.04) and in commercials (YA −0.19 ± 0.26; OA −0.07 ± 0.21, effect size 0.50). The groups showed no differences in bias, t(39) = 0.086, p = 0.932 and t(39) = −1.505, p = 0.140 for news and commercials, respectively.

Contrary to our hypothesis, OA and YA had a similar amount of false alarms in both paradigms. However, in line with the literature (e.g., Fraundorf et al., 2019), OA had significantly fewer hits and inferior discriminability in both news and commercials paradigm.

As our paradigm represents a modified use of a signal detection theory by incorporating reversed signal items, we wanted to rule out gist influence on older adults and ran an alternative analysis by dividing the signal items into original statements and reversed statements. Koutstaal and Schacter (1997) and Schacter et al. (1999) described a procedure in which in addition to classical signal detection theory (comparison of signal vs. noise) noise items are compared to items strongly related to original signal items. This procedure was developed to assess false recognition of lure items conceptually, perceptually, or semantically related to studied items and aims to draw a distinction between baseline false alarms and memory mistakes as a result of similarity to signal items or gist memory.

According to this modification of signal detection theory we first compared discriminability and bias measures between the YA and OA using yes answers to original signal items (hits) to the sum of yes and no answers to noise items (false alarms) to evaluate a baseline false alarm rate. This analysis omitted hits to reversed items. The alternative scoring system is displayed in Table 5. There were no significant differences for the number of hits to original signal items between the groups (news: YA 14.52 ± 1.57, OA 13.40 ± 2.46, effect size 0.54, t(39) = 1.754, p = 0.087; commercials: YA 15.52 ± 1.78, OA 14.35 ± 2.43, effect size 0.55, t(39) = 1.770, p = 0.085). There was a significant difference in discriminability for news (YA 1.56 ± 0.57, OA 1.13 ± 0.56, effect size 0.65, t(39) = 2.465, p = 0.018) but not for commercials (YA 1.75 ± 0.59, OA 1.41 ± 0.74, effect size 0.73, t(39) = 1.624, p = 0.112). There were no differences in bias (news: YA −0.35 ± 0.29, OA −0.32 ± 0.42, effect size 0.08, t(39) = 2.465, p = 0.018; commercials: YA −0.31 ± 0.30, OA −0.24 ± 0.28, effect size 0.24, t(39) = −0.834, p = 0.409).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Scoring matrix for hits and false alarms used in the secondary analysis without the reversed signal items.

Similarly, we next compared discriminability and bias using yes answers to reversed signal items (hits for gist signal) and false alarms to assess gist memory. This analysis omits the original signal items. The scoring system designed to measure the strength of the gist signal is displayed in Table 6. There were no significant differences in the hits to gist signal between OA and YA (news: YA 3.52 ± 1.54, OA 4.35 ± 1.81, effect size 0.49, t(39) = −1.576, p = 0.123, commercials: YA 2.48 ± 1.44, OA 3.35 ± 1.95, effect size 0.50, t(39) = −1.637, p = 0.110). There were also no group differences in either discriminability (news: YA −0.49 ± 0.38, OA −0.47 ± 0.48, effect size 0.05, t(39) = −0.181, p = 0.858; commercials: YA −0.59 ± 0.65, OA −0.51 ± 0.58, effect size 0.13, t(39) = −0.427, p = 0.671) or bias (news: YA 0.68 ± 0.31, OA 0.48 ± 0.36, effect size 0.60, t(39) = 1.896, p = 0.065; commercials: YA 0.86 ± 0.23, OA 0.72 ± 0.30, effect size 0.52, t(39) = 1.624, p = 0.112) for gist memory.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Scoring matrix for hits and false alarms used to assess sensitivity to gist signal.

Discussion

It is generally acknowledged that memory performance decreases with age. Research on age-associated memory deficits explored different aspects of both recall and recognition. The majority of the paradigms in such studies employed stimuli that are only indirectly related to everyday life. The present study employed a type of stimuli that younger and older adults encounter on a daily basis. Participants were shown news and commercials and subsequently performed a recognition task to assess hits and false alarms for video content.

News and commercials watching habits did not have an effect on the recognition task outcome. Older adults subjectively found news recognition task easier, whereas young adults found commercials recognition task easier. Despite the differences in the subjective difficulty, there was no difference between the two groups in the subjective estimate of correct responses. This result helps to exclude possible adverse effects due to low performance expectations among older adults (Chasteen et al., 2005; Hess and Hinson, 2006; Hess et al., 2009). Contrary to our hypothesis, the older adults did not have more false alarms compared to young adults, despite younger adults scoring significantly better in most of the neuropsychological variables. There was, however, a difference in the number of hits, with younger adults performing significantly better in both experimental paradigms. After calculating the effect size, we observed that older adults scored slightly over one half of a standard deviation worse on news hits and one standard deviation worse on commercials hits. Signal detection theory supports these results. Discriminability scores for news and commercials in young adults were significantly higher. Discriminability is a measure calculated using both hits and false alarms. Group variance of hits and group variance of false alarms was comparable. Thus, the observed discriminability difference results from the difference in the number of hits. Older and younger adults showed no differences regarding answering tendencies and both have shown moderately liberal bias. There have been some reports, that response bias might be a stable cognitive trait (Kantner and Lindsay, 2012, 2014) and that the decision criterion shifts as a function of memory strength, strategy, personality and affect (Aminoff et al., 2012).

As our hypothesis on increased false alarms in older adults was rejected, we could not further pursue the original plan of looking into what neuropsychological variables explain differences in false alarms between young and older adults. Nevertheless, the outcomes of neuropsychological assessment are in harmony with age-related cognitive deficits reported in the literature (Ardila et al., 2000; Park et al., 2002; Zahodne et al., 2011; Lipnicki et al., 2013). The present study observed a generally diminished episodic memory performance in older persons. Differences were observed only in the tasks of free recall, and not recognition memory, which most likely represent a ceiling effect in the test’s sensitivity. Moreover, attention, working memory as well as some aspects of executive function (as measured by the Trail Making Test) were diminished. Other measures of executive function (semantic and phonemic verbal fluency) did not differ in younger and older persons. None of the deficits in older adults were clinically indicative of pathology. This significantly reduces the possibility that the failure to demonstrate age-related increase in false alarms was due to unusually high cognitive performance in this sample of older adults.

The present study supports earlier findings of aging-related decrease in hits, however, it contradicts reports on age-associated increase in false alarms (Seamon et al., 2002; Dennis et al., 2008; Rosa and Gutchess, 2013; Devitt and Schacter, 2016; Paige et al., 2016). Out of the three recognition memory theories described in the introduction, the associative deficit hypothesis (Naveh-Benjamin, 2000; Old and Naveh-Benjamin, 2008) offers a possible explanation for the absence of an increase in false alarms among older adults in our study. Chalfonte and Johnson (1996) argued that association (binding) of features belonging to the same stimulus enriches memory. Hence, what we remember is not blue and pen but rather blue pen as a single entity.

The results of the current study can be interpreted in a similar way. In order to make a decision about the truthfulness of a statement in our paradigm, participants were required not only to memorize but also to bind the features together into a single entity. In order to correctly accept a correct statement [The older woman1 offers rice2 to a girl3], one needs to have encoded and bound all three features correctly (square brackets indicate that the features form a single signal). The recognition of some item features is not enough for a hit, as our paradigm introduces ambiguity, where half of the original items are presented in the negated form. The recognition process for the negated form of the original signal is similar: a composite item [Skateboards1 are popular2 in Germany3] needs to be bound and encoded as a single entity in order to recognize that the statement presented in the recognition task, [Skateboards are unpopular in Germany], is the negated form of the original statement. Any information unit missing or being different negates the original message signal in a Boolean sense and should induce a “no” in recognition of this composite item. Since the item is treated as one homogeneous signal it needs to be recognized or negated in its entirety. If it is not recognized completely, it is missed: remembering only skateboards or only Germany is not enough for a successful recognition. In contrast, items that present noise without any elements of a signal, e.g., [The woman1 wears a blue dress2 while baking3] should only be answered with “unknown,” as in the whole videos sequence there is no [Woman wearing a blue dress while baking]. Anything but the response “unknown” represents a false alarm.

In their meta-analysis, Fraundorf et al. (2019) reported that young adults mostly outperform older adults in easy recognition tasks but in some instances of more complex recognition tasks, older adults performed at least as good as or better than young adults. The authors were not able to identify any variables that would explain preserved memory performance in older adults in their meta-analysis. However, this special circumstance seems to appear in studies using ecologically valid tasks with complex recognition statements (LaVoie and Malmstrom, 1998; Matzen and Benjamin, 2013). The present study offers additional support to this possibility and is in-line with a repeatedly documented decrease in binding ability among older adults (Chalfonte and Johnson, 1996; Naveh-Benjamin, 2000; Old and Naveh-Benjamin, 2008; Fraundorf et al., 2019). While making a correct recognition harder due to the binding deficit, complex items might paradoxically offer an advantage to older adults, when it comes to correct rejection, because several false features within a noise item make this item more salient and easier to identify as noise.

After separating original and reversed statements in the subsequent analysis we have found that statistically significant difference in hits between the groups has disappeared, however, the trend has remained. It could be that statistical significance was affected by the decrease in items after separating original and reversed signal items. Alternatively it could be speculated that inferior performance of older adults in recognition task for reversed statements might result from their reduced cognitive processing abilities, as processing of sentential negation requires additional neural resources of response inhibition to the original affirmative form of the particular item (Tettamanti et al., 2008; Bartoli et al., 2013; Beltrán et al., 2018). Furthermore, using the same analysis the difference in the discriminability for commercials disappears. This might indicate slight differences in “news” stimuli compared to “commercials” stimuli, which needs to be addressed in future studies. Further studies are needed to address these issues.

Calculating susceptibility to gist memory by examining discriminability and bias estimates calculated from yes responses to reversed signal items showed no group differences. Although previous studies have reported a greater reliance on gist among older adults (e.g., Koutstaal et al., 1999; LaVoie and Faulkner, 2000), the failure to observe it in our study could be attributed to several factors. The participants in our study were given detailed information about the items they will be confronted with in the recognition task and what type of answer is appropriate for each item. Such a priori instructions and warnings have been shown to decrease memory errors in older adults (Carmichael and Gutchess, 2016). It is also possible that the timing of the recognition task has contributed to a decreased reliance on gist. The recognition task in our study was performed right after seeing the actual material, which means that the verbatim traces were still robustly represented in the participant’s memory and the reliance on gist, which increases with the temporal deterioration of the verbatim traces, was not yet necessary (Abadie and Camos, 2018). It is important to note that despite susceptibility to gist signal not providing a statistically significant group difference, it showed a moderate effect size. This suggests that this result might reflect a lack of power rather than a genuine absence of performance differences between young and older adults.

There are several limitations to the study. First, demographic variables for general intellectual abilities, overall cognitive score, and assessment of mood were different between young and older persons. The differences were small and all scores in both groups were within normal range and representative of the general population, yet it cannot be excluded that it had an influence on the results. Moreover, the relevance of the information reported in the news clips and the familiarity with the products mentioned in the commercials was not assessed. It was previously reported that factors such as selectivity in task engagement and perceived emotional goals affect memory performance in the older adults (Hess, 2005), thus we cannot rule out that these differences exist between or within the age groups. Future paradigms addressing ecologically valid memory tasks should further explore the effects of familiarity and the perceived meaningfulness of the task.

Conclusion

Decrease in correct recognitions and increase in false alarms among older adults has been well documented and explained by deficits in feature binding, inhibition and cognitive slowing. The present study demonstrated that age-related increase in false alarms is not universal but subject to the complexity of the stimuli. An ecologically valid task requiring binding of several features increases the likelihood of a correct rejection. This demonstrates that while memory is patchy in older persons it is not distorted for real-life situations.

Ethics Statement

The study received approval of the local ethics committee of the Ulm University (Application No. 233/15). All persons gave their informed consent prior to their participation in the study.

Author Contributions

KS and CL were involved in acquisition of the data, data analysis, and drafting and revising the manuscript. KS, CL and MR were involved in designing the study, interpretation of the data, and drafting and revising the manuscript. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Abbreviations

CVLT, California Verbal Learning Test; DRM, Deese-Roediger-McDermott procedure; FA, False alarms; MADRS, Montgomery-Asberg-Depression-Rating-Scale; MMSE, Mini Mental State Examination; OA, Older adults; RWT, Regensburg Verbal Fluency Test; TMT, Trail Making Test; WMS, Wechsler Memory Scale; WST, Vocabulary test; YA, Young adults.

References

Abadie, M., and Camos, V. (2018). False memory at short and long term. J. Exp. Psychol. doi: 10.1037/xge0000526 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aminoff, E. M., Clewett, D., Freeman, S., Frithsen, A., Tipper, C., Johnson, A., et al. (2012). Individual differences in shifting decision criterion: a recognition memory study. Mem. Cogn. 40, 1016–1030. doi: 10.3758/s13421-012-0204-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ardila, A. (2007). Normal aging increases cognitive heterogeneity: analysis of dispersion in WAIS-III scores across age. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 22, 1003–1011. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2007.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ardila, A., Ostrosky-Solis, F., Rosselli, M., and Gómez, C. (2000). Age-related cognitive decline during normal aging: the complex effect of education. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 15, 495–513. doi: 10.1016/s0887-6177(99)00040-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aschenbrenner, S., Tucha, O., and Lange, K. W. (2000). RWT: Regensburger Wortflüssigkeits-Test. Göttingen: Hogrefe Verlag.

Google Scholar

Bartoli, E., Tettamanti, A., Farronato, P., Caporizzo, A., Moro, A., Gatti, R., et al. (2013). The disembodiment effect of negation: negating action-related sentences attenuates their interference on congruent upper limb movements. J. Neurophysiol. 109, 1782–1792. doi: 10.1152/jn.00894.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beltrán, D., Muñetón-Ayala, M., and de Vega, M. (2018). Sentential negation modulates inhibition in a stop-signal task. Evidence from behavioral and ERP data. Neuropsychologia 112, 10–18. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buckner, R. L. (2004). Memory and executive function in aging and AD: multiple factors that cause decline and reserve factors that compensate. Neuron 44, 195–208. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Butler, K. M., McDaniel, M. A., Dornburg, C. C., Price, A. L., and Roediger, H. L. III. (2004). Age differences in veridical and false recall are not inevitable: the role of frontal lobe function. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 11, 921–925. doi: 10.3758/bf03196722

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabeza, R. (2002). Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults: the HAROLD model. Psychol. Aging 17, 85–100. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.17.1.85

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabeza, R., Anderson, N. D., Locantore, J. K., and McIntosh, A. R. (2002). Aging gracefully. Compensatory brain activity in high-performing older adults. Neuroimage 17, 1394–1402. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1280

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carmichael, A. M., and Gutchess, A. H. (2016). Using warnings to reduce categorical false memories in younger and older adults. Memory 24, 853–863. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2015.1059454

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chalfonte, B. L., and Johnson, M. K. (1996). Feature memory and binding in young and older adults. Mem. Cogn. 24, 403–416. doi: 10.3758/bf03200930

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, J. C. K., and McDermott, K. B. (2007). The effects of frontal lobe functioning and age on veridical and false recall. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 14, 606–611. doi: 10.3758/bf03196809

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chasteen, A. L., Bhattacharyya, S., Horhota, M., Tam, R., and Hasher, L. (2005). How feelings of stereotype threat influence older adults’ memory performance. Exp. Aging Res. 31, 235–260. doi: 10.1080/03610730590948177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chaytor, N., and Schmitter-Edgecombe, M. (2003). The ecological validity of neuropsychological tests: a review of the literature on everyday cognitive skills. Neuropsychol. Rev. 13, 181–197. doi: 10.1023/b:nerv.0000009483.91468.fb

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craik, F. I., Morris, L. W., Morris, R. G., and Loewen, E. R. (1990). Relations between source amnesia and frontal lobe functioning in older adults. Psychol. Aging 5, 148–151. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.5.1.148

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craik, F. I. M., and Rose, N. S. (2012). Memory encoding and aging: a neurocognitive perspective. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 1729–1739. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.11.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Darowski, E. S., Helder, E., Zacks, R. T., Hasher, L., and Hambrick, D. Z. (2008). Age-related differences in cognition: the role of distraction control. Neuropsychology 22, 638–644. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.22.5.638

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daselaar, S. M., Veltman, D. J., Rombouts, S. A., Raaijmakers, J. G., and Jonker, C. (2003). Neuroanatomical correlates of episodic encoding and retrieval in young and elderly subjects. Brain 126, 43–56. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dennis, N. A., Kim, H., and Cabeza, R. (2007). Effects of aging on true and false memory formation: an fMRI study. Neuropsychologia 45, 3157–3166. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dennis, N. A., Kim, H., and Cabeza, R. (2008). Age-related differences in brain activity during true and false memory retrieval. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 20, 1390–1402. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20096

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Devitt, A. L., and Schacter, D. L. (2016). False memories with age: neural and cognitive underpinnings. Neuropsychologia 91, 346–359. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.08.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Carlo, A., Baldereschi, M., Amaducci, L., Maggi, S., Grigoletto, F., Scarlato, G., et al. (2000). Cognitive impairment without dementia in older people: prevalence, vascular risk factors, impact on disability. The Italian Longitudinal Study on aging. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 48, 775–782. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2000.tb04752.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fandakova, Y., Lindenberger, U., and Shing, Y. L. (2015). Maintenance of youth-like processing protects against false memory in later adulthood. Neurobiol. Aging 36, 933–941. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.10.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fandakova, Y., Shing, Y. L., and Lindenberger, U. (2013a). Differences in binding and monitoring mechanisms contribute to lifespan age differences in false memory. Dev. Psychol. 49, 1822–1832. doi: 10.1037/a0031361

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fandakova, Y., Shing, Y. L., and Lindenberger, U. (2013b). High-confidence memory errors in old age: the roles of monitoring and binding processes. Memory 21, 732–750. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2012.756038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., and McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fraundorf, S. H., Hourihan, K. L., Peters, R. A., and Benjamin, A. S. (2019). Aging and recognition memory: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 145, 339–371. doi: 10.1037/bul0000185

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frenda, S. J., Knowles, E. D., Saletan, W., and Loftus, E. F. (2013). False memories of fabricated political events. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 49, 280–286. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2012.10.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frings, L., Mader, I., and Hüll, M. (2010). Watching TV news as a memory task—brain activation and age effects. BMC Neurosci. 11:106. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-11-106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fung, H. H., and Carstensen, L. L. (2003). Sending memorable messages to the old: age differences in preferences and memory for advertisements. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85, 163–178. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.1.163

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grady, C. L., and Craik, F. I. (2000). Changes in memory processing with age. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 10, 224–231. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(00)00073-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamm, V. P., and Hasher, L. (1992). Age and the availability of inferences. Psychol. Aging 7, 56–64. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.7.1.56

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Härting, C., Markowitsch, H. J., Neufeld, H., Calabrese, P., Deisinger, K., and Kessler, J. (2000). WMS-R: Wechsler Gedächtnistest—Revidierte Fassung. Bern: Huber.

Google Scholar

Hasher, L., and Zacks, R. T. (1988). “Working memory, comprehension, and aging: a review and a new view,” in The Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Advances in Research and Theory, ed. G. H. Bower (San Diego, CA: Academic Press), 193–225.

Google Scholar

Healey, M. K., and Kahana, M. J. (2016). A four-component model of age-related memory change. Psychol. Rev. 123, 23–69. doi: 10.1037/rev0000015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hedden, T., and Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2004). Insights into the ageing mind: a view from cognitive neuroscience. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5, 87–96. doi: 10.1038/nrn1323

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henkel, L. A., Johnson, M. K., and De Leonardis, D. M. (1998). Aging and source monitoring: cognitive processes and neuropsychological correlates. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 127, 251–268. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.127.3.251

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hertzog, C., McGuire, C. L., and Lineweaver, T. T. (2010). Aging, attributions, perceived control, and strategy use in a free recall task. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 5, 85–106. doi: 10.1076/anec.5.2.85.601

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hess, T. M. (2005). Memory and aging in context. Psychol. Bull. 131, 383–406. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.3.383

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hess, T. M., and Hinson, J. T. (2006). Age-related variation in the influences of aging stereotypes on memory in adulthood. Psychol. Aging 21, 621–625. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.21.3.621

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hess, T. M., Hinson, J. T., and Hodges, E. A. (2009). Moderators of and mechanisms underlying stereotype threat effects on older adults’ memory performance. Exp. Aging Res. 35, 153–177. doi: 10.1080/03610730802716413

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hultsch, D. F. (1969). Adult age differences in the organization of free recall. Dev. Psychol. 1, 673–678. doi: 10.1037/h0028271

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ikier, S., Yang, L., and Hasher, L. (2008). Implicit proactive interference, age, and automatic versus controlled retrieval strategies. Psychol. Sci. 19, 456–461. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02109.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacoby, L. L. (1999). Ironic effects of repetition: measuring age-related differences in memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 25, 3–22. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.25.1.3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, M. K., and Raye, C. L. (1998). False memories and confabulation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2, 137–145. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(98)01152-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kantner, J., and Lindsay, D. S. (2012). Response bias in recognition memory as a cognitive trait. Mem. Cogn. 40, 1163–1177. doi: 10.3758/s13421-012-0226-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kantner, J., and Lindsay, D. S. (2014). Cross-situational consistency in recognition memory response bias. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 21, 1272–1280. doi: 10.3758/s13423-014-0608-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koutstaal, W., and Schacter, D. L. (1997). Gist-based false recognition of pictures in older and younger adults. J. Mem. Lang. 37, 555–583. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1997.2529

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koutstaal, W., Schacter, D. L., Galluccio, L., and Stofer, K. A. (1999). Reducing gist-based false recognition in older adults: encoding and retrieval manipulations. Psychol. Aging 14:220. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.14.2.220

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kramer, J. H., Yaffe, K., Lengenfelder, J., Delis, D. C., and Joel, H. (2003). Age and gender interactions on verbal memory performance. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 9, 97–102. doi: 10.1017/s1355617703910113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LaVoie, D. J., and Faulkner, K. (2000). Age differences in false recognition using a forced choice paradigm. Exp. Aging Res. 26, 367–381. doi: 10.1080/036107300750015750

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LaVoie, D. J., and Malmstrom, T. (1998). False recognition effects in young and older adults’ memory for text passages. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 53, P255–P262. doi: 10.1093/geronb/53b.4.p255

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lipnicki, D. M., Sachdev, P. S., Crawford, J., Reppermund, S., Kochan, N. A., Trollor, J. N., et al. (2013). Risk factors for late-life cognitive decline and variation with age and sex in the Sydney Memory and Ageing Study. PLoS One 8:e65841. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0065841

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacDonald, S. W. S., Dixon, R. A., Cohen, A.-L., and Hazlitt, J. E. (2004). Biological age and 12-year cognitive change in older adults: findings from the Victoria Longitudinal Study. Gerontology 50, 64–81. doi: 10.1159/000075557

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Madden, D. J. (1986). Adult age differences in visual word recognition: semantic encoding and episodic retention. Exp. Aging Res. 12, 71–78. doi: 10.1080/03610738608259439

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manard, M., Carabin, D., Jaspar, M., and Collette, F. (2014). Age-related decline in cognitive control: the role of fluid intelligence and processing speed. BMC Neurosci. 15:7. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-15-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, B. A. S. (2003). The influence of gender on mood effects in advertising. Psychol. Mark. 20, 249–273. doi: 10.1002/mar.10070

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matzen, L. E., and Benjamin, A. S. (2013). Older and wiser: older adults’ episodic word memory benefits from sentence study contexts. Psychol. Aging 28, 754–767. doi: 10.1037/a0032945

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

May, C. P., and Hasher, L. (1998). Synchrony effects in inhibitory control over thought and action. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 24, 363–379. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.24.2.363

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mazurek, A., Bhoopathy, R. M., Read, J. C. A., Gallagher, P., and Smulders, T. V. (2015). Effects of age on a real-world What-Where-When memory task. Front. Aging Neurosci. 7:74. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCabe, D. P., Roediger, H. L. III., McDaniel, M. A., and Balota, D. A. (2009). Aging reduces veridical remembering but increases false remembering: neuropsychological test correlates of remember-know judgments. Neuropsychologia 47, 2164–2173. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.11.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meade, M. L., Geraci, L. D., and Roediger, H. L. III. (2012). Neuropsychological status in older adults influences susceptibility to false memories. Am. J. Psychol. 125, 449–467. doi: 10.5406/amerjpsyc.125.4.0449

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitchell, K. J., and Johnson, M. K. (2009). Source monitoring 15 years later: what have we learned from fMRI about the neural mechanisms of source memory? Psychol. Bull. 135, 638–677. doi: 10.1037/a0015849

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montgomery, S. A., and Asberg, M. (1989). MADR-Skala: Zur Psychometrischen Beurteilung Depressiver Symptome. Erlangen: Perimed Fachbuch-Verlagsgesellschaft.

Google Scholar

Multhaup, K. S. (1995). Aging, source, and decision criteria: when false fame errors do and do not occur. Psychol. Aging 10, 492–497. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.10.3.492

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Naveh-Benjamin, M. (2000). Adult age differences in memory performance: tests of an associative deficit hypothesis. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 26, 1170–1187. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.26.5.1170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niemann, H., Sturm, W., Thöne-Otto, A. I. T., and Willmes, K. (2008). CVLT California Verbal Learning Test: German Adaptation. Frankfurt: Pearson Assessment.

Google Scholar

Nilsson, L. G. (2003). Memory function in normal aging. Acta Neurol. Scand. Suppl. 179, 7–13. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0404.107.s179.5.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Old, S. R., and Naveh-Benjamin, M. (2008). Differential effects of age on item and associative measures of memory: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Aging 23, 104–118. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.23.1.104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paige, L. E., Cassidy, B. S., Schacter, D. L., and Gutchess, A. H. (2016). Age differences in hippocampal activation during gist-based false recognition. Neurobiol. Aging 46, 76–83. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.06.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, D. C., Lautenschlager, G., Hedden, T., Davidson, N. S., Smith, A. D., and Smith, P. K. (2002). Models of visuospatial and verbal memory across the adult life span. Psychol. Aging 17, 299–320. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.17.2.299

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patihis, L., Frenda, S. J., and Loftus, E. F. (2018). False memory tasks do not reliably predict other false memories. Psychol. Conscious. Theor. Res. Pract. 5, 140–160. doi: 10.1037/cns0000147

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Persson, J., Nyberg, L., Lind, J., Larsson, A., Nilsson, L.-G., Ingvar, M., et al. (2006). Structure-function correlates of cognitive decline in aging. Cereb. Cortex 16, 907–915. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, R. C., Smith, G., Kokmen, E., Ivnik, R. J., and Tangalos, E. G. (1992). Memory function in normal aging. Neurology 42, 396–401. doi: 10.1212/WNL.42.2.396

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Plancher, G., Guyard, A., Nicolas, S., and Piolino, P. (2009). Mechanisms underlying the production of false memories for famous people’s names in aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychologia 47, 2527–2536. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.04.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prior, M. J. (2003). Any good news in soft news? The impact of soft news preference on political knowledge. Polit. Commun. 20, 149–171. doi: 10.1080/10584600390211172

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ratcliff, R., Thapar, A., and McKoon, G. (2004). A diffusion model analysis of the effects of aging on recognition memory. J. Mem. Lang. 50, 408–424. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2003.11.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raz, N., Lindenberger, U., Rodrigue, K. M., Kennedy, K. M., Head, D., Williamson, A., et al. (2005). Regional brain changes in aging healthy adults: general trends, individual differences and modifiers. Cereb. Cortex 15, 1676–1689. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reitan, R. M., and Wolfston, D. (1985). The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery: Theory and Clinical Interpretation. Tucson, AZ: Neuropsychology Press.

Google Scholar

Robertson, I. H., Ward, T., Ridgeway, V., and Nimmo-Smith, I. (1994). “The test of everyday attention (TEA),” in Test Reviews, ed. A. W. Mc Anespie (Bury St. Edmunds: Thames Valley Test Company), 197–221.

Google Scholar

Roediger, H. L., Balota, D. A., and Watson, J. M. (2001). “Spreading activation and arousal of false memories,” in The Nature of Remembering: Essays in Honor of Robert G. Crowder/edited by Henry L. Roediger III … [et al.], eds R. G. Crowder and H. L. Roediger (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 95–115.

Google Scholar

Roediger, H. L. III., and Geraci, L. (2007). Aging and the misinformation effect: a neuropsychological analysis. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 33, 321–334. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.33.2.321

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roediger, H. L., and McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories: remembering words not presented in lists. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 21, 803–814. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.21.4.803

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosa, N. M., Deason, R. G., Budson, A. E., and Gutchess, A. H. (2015). Self-referencing and false memory in mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology 29, 799–805. doi: 10.1037/neu0000186

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosa, N. M., and Gutchess, A. H. (2013). False memory in aging resulting from self-referential processing. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 68, 882–892. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbt018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Royall, D. R., Palmer, R., Chiodo, L. K., and Polk, M. J. (2005). Normal rates of cognitive change in successful aging: the freedom house study. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 11, 899–909. doi: 10.1017/s135561770505109x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salthouse, T. A. (1996a). General and specific speed mediation of adult age differences in memory. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 51, 30–42. doi: 10.1093/geronb/51b.1.p30

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salthouse, T. A. (1996b). The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychol. Rev. 103, 403–428. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.103.3.403

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salthouse, T. A. (2016). Theoretical Perspectives on Cognitive Aging. New York, NY: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Schacter, D. L., Israel, L., and Racine, C. (1999). Suppressing false recognition in younger and older adults: the distinctiveness heuristic. J. Mem. Lang. 40, 1–24. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1998.2611

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schacter, D. L., Koutstaal, W., and Norman, K. A. (1997). False memories and aging. Trends Cogn. Sci. 1, 229–236. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(97)01068-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, K. H., and Metzler, P. (1992). WST—Wortschatztest. Weinheim: Beltz.

Google Scholar

Seamon, J. G., Luo, C. R., Schwartz, M. A., Jones, K. J., Lee, D. M., and Jones, S. J. (2002). Repetition can have similar or different effects on accurate and false recognition. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 46, 323–340. doi: 10.1006/jmla.2001.2811

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sejunaite, K., Lanza, C., and Riepe, M. W. (2017). Everyday memory in patients with Alzheimer’s disease: fragmentary and distorted. J. Alzheimers Dis. 60, 1489–1498. doi: 10.3233/jad-170493

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sejunaite, K., Lanza, C., and Riepe, M. W. (2018). Everyday false memories in older persons with depressive disorder. Psychiatry Res. 261, 456–463. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.01.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shapiro, M. A. (2014). “Signal detection measures of recognition memory,” in Measuring Psychological Responses to Media Messages ed. A. Lang (New York, NY: Routledge), 145–160.

Google Scholar

Souchay, C., Isingrini, M., Espagnet, L., and Souchay, C. (2000). Aging, episodic memory feeling-of-knowing and frontal functioning. Neuropsychology 14, 299–309. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.14.2.299

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spaniol, J., Madden, D. J., and Voss, A. (2006). A diffusion model analysis of adult age differences in episodic and semantic long-term memory retrieval. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 32, 101–117. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.1.101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stanislaw, H., and Todorov, N. (1999). Calculation of signal detection theory measures. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 31, 137–149. doi: 10.3758/bf03207704

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Statistisches Bundesamt. (2016). Mikrokonzensus: Bevoelkerung und Erwerbstaetigkeit. Beruf, Ausbildung und Arbeitsbedingungen der Erwerbstaetigen in Deutschland. Wiesbaden: Statistisches Bundesamt.

Google Scholar

Tettamanti, M., Manenti, R., Della Rosa, P. A., Falini, A., Perani, D., Cappa, S. F., et al. (2008). Negation in the brain: modulating action representations. Neuroimage 43, 358–367. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tun, P. A., Wingfield, A., Rosen, M. J., and Blanchard, L. (1998). Response latencies for false memories: gist-based processes in normal aging. Psychol. Aging 13, 230–241. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.13.2.230

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Ocker, J. C., Light, L. L., Olfman, D., and Rivera, J. (2017). Effects of repetition on age differences in associative recognition. Memory 25, 350–359. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2016.1177089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wechsler, D., Petermann, F., and Lepach, A. C. (2012). WMS-IV: Wechsler Memory Scale—Fourth Edition. Frankfurt: Pearson Assessment.

Google Scholar

Weitzer, R., and Kubrin, C. E. (2004). Breaking news: how local TV news and real-world conditions affect fear of crime. Justice Q. 21, 497–520. doi: 10.1080/07418820400095881

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, B. A., Alderman, N., Burgess, P. W., Emslie, H., and Evans, J. J. (1996). Behavioral Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndom. Bury St. Edmunds: Thames Valley Test Company.

Google Scholar

Wilson, B. A., Greenfield, E., Clare, L., Baddeley, A., Cockburn, J., Watson, P., et al. (2008). Rivermead Behavioural Memory Test—Third Edition (RBMT-3). London: Pearson Assessment.

Google Scholar

Wilson, B. A., Shiel, A., Foley, J., Emslie, H., Groot, Y., and Hawkins, K. (2004). Cambridge Test of Prospective Memory. Bury St. Edmunds: Thames Valley Test Company.

Google Scholar

World Medical Association. (2013). World medical association declaration of helsinki: ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. JAMA 310, 2191–2194. doi: 10.1001/jama.2013.281053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zahodne, L. B., Glymour, M. M., Sparks, C., Bontempo, D., Dixon, R. A., MacDonald, S. W. S., et al. (2011). Education does not slow cognitive decline with aging. 12-year evidence from the victoria longitudinal study. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 17, 1039–1046. doi: 10.1017/s1355617711001044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: memory, recognition, false alarms, daily life, neuropsychology, healthy aging

Citation: Sejunaite K, Lanza C and Riepe MW (2019) Everyday Memory in Healthy Aging: Porous but Not Distorted. Front. Aging Neurosci. 11:153. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2019.00153

Received: 28 November 2018; Accepted: 11 June 2019;
Published: 25 June 2019.

Edited by:

Rommy Von Bernhardi, Pontifical Catholic University of Chile, Chile

Reviewed by:

Jesse Q. Sargent, Francis Marion University, United States
Kristin Flegal, University of Glasgow, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2019 Sejunaite, Lanza and Riepe. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Matthias W. Riepe, matthias.riepe@uni-ulm.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.