Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Endocrinol., 26 July 2019
Sec. Neuroendocrine Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Neuromodulation of Exercise: Impact on Different Kinds of Behavior View all 11 articles

Effects of Different Types of Exercise Training on the Cortisol Awakening Response in Children

  • 1Department of Sport Psychology, Institute of Sports Science, Humboldt University Berlin, Berlin, Germany
  • 2Faculty of Human Sciences, Medical School Hamburg, Hamburg, Germany
  • 3Department of Sport Psychology, Institute for Sport Psychology and Sport Pedagogy, Leipzig University, Leipzig, Germany

Context: Due to great variability of the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal (HPA)-axis, research has to produce better-controlled findings to make a more meaningful statement regarding the effect of exercise training (ET) on the cortisol awakening response (CAR), especially in children.

Objective: The aim of the study was to investigate the effects of different ET interventions on the CAR in children.

Design and setting: We conducted a short-term training study for 10 weeks in primary schools in Westphalia, Germany.

Participants: 71 children (9–10 years old) were randomly assigned to a cardiovascular exercise group (n = 27), a motor exercise group (n = 23), or a control group (n = 21).

Intervention: An experienced instructor trained the children in an after-school setting in 45 min sessions, three times a week over the course of 10 weeks.

Main outcome measure: CAR (0, +30 min) was assessed on 2 schooldays one week apart before and after the 10-week intervention. A Shuttle Run Test was performed to determine the cardiovascular fitness. Motor fitness was assessed using the Heidelberg Gross Motor Test.

Results: Children who enhanced their cardiovascular fitness over the course of the intervention showed an increased CAR after the intervention time (B = 0.213), whereas children who underwent a motor exercise intervention and at the same time gained in motor fitness exhibited a decreased CAR after intervention (B = −0.188).

Conclusions: It has been speculated that other neurobiological pathways are activated by different exercise interventions. The extent to which these ET effects on CAR can be applied in clinical settings needs further investigation.

Précis: The 10-weeks longitudinal effects of cardiovascular vs. motor exercise interventions (three times a week) on CAR in children show that these interventions exert different effects on hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis activity.

Introduction

Children and adolescents in industrialized countries today increasingly suffer from inactivity and associated health issues such as obesity and psychological disorders, e.g., as an effect of stress exposure (1). Exercise training (ET), however, is an easy-to-implement intervention, which can be administered in group settings including schools. It has been proven that ET has long-term beneficial effects and is a cost-efficient and sustainable strategy to improve health in various mental and physical disorders (2, 3). ET is defined as a structured exercise program that involves the use of large muscle groups for extended periods of time. ET differs from physical activity (PA) in its planned and structured nature (4). Even though it appears that experts believe prevention of diseases should start in childhood and adolescence, there are rarely studies that focus on the effects of ET on health in children under the age of 12 (5). The present study focuses on an age group that lacks extensive research on stress related hormonal indices (e.g., HPA axis activity).

The HPA axis is a highly stress-responsive system and shows a strong diurnal pattern with the glucocorticoid cortisol as an end product. One suitable marker for determining HPA axis activity is the CAR. The CAR is a reliable measure for the acute responsiveness of the HPA axis and can serve as a useful index of adrenocortical activity (6). Cortisol levels, which are measured during the first 30 min after awakening show an increase of 50–70% in the vast majority of adults but generally seem to be less pronounced in children and adolescence (7). In adults, CAR is generally positively associated with job and general life stress (8). Even though it is still unclear what contributes to a “healthy” CAR, ET has been argued to alter the HPA axis activity in adults depending on the intensity of the intervention (e.g., moderate vs. vigorous) as well as on the intervention type (e.g., aerobic vs. yoga) (6). Thus, exercise intensity is an issue like in other interventions to promote mental health (9, 10).

To our knowledge, there are no randomized and controlled intervention studies focusing on the effects of ET on the CAR in children. Only a few studies investigated the relationship between ET and CAR in children and adolescents in cross-sectional designs presenting insconsistent results. For example, a positive correlation between CAR and vigorous PA was found in a study focusing on 8-year-old girls suffering from metabolic syndrome (11). In contrast, a lower CAR was linked to the duration of acute daytime sport among healthy older adolescents (aged 10–18 years) (12). Finally, a study among healthy 8-year-old children showed no differences in the diurnal salivary cortisol pattern based on the level of the overall daytime PA (13). However, children's general physical fitness level and regular physical activity were not assessed, which could affect cortisol activity. Further, these were all cross-sectional study designs, which limit causal relationships between ET/PA and CAR.

As the exercise intervention type (6) might affect chronic stress levels (e.g., CAR), it could be argued that exercise interventions that focus on improvement of motor abilities might be beneficial for preserving cognitive resources and thus, freeing resources to deal with complex situations in daily life and resulting in a reduced stress response (14). Cardiovascular exercise, by contrast, may lead to stronger neurogenesis, which might result in stronger cortisol responses after a chronic exercise intervention (15). It was previously argued that hippocampal neurogenesis is mainly promoted by cardiovascular exercise interventions (16). Niemann et al. (17) could not reveal changes in hippocampal volume after 6 months of motor demanding training, but found a significant increase in volume after cardiovascular training in elderlies. Thus, specific stressors influence neurogenesis and the HPA axis activity in different ways (18).

Overall, the existing literature does not allow for causal inference because it is unknown if the parameter itself (being more physically fit) caused the changes in HPA axis activity or if other factors might have accounted for these differences. Therefore, controlled intervention studies are required to focus on the causal relationship between exercise and HPA axis activity (e.g., CAR) in a young, pre-adolescent age group. Martikainen et al. (13) stipulated that the exercise intervention type needs to be manipulated. Taken together, the current study aims to fill this gap by investigating the effect of a 10-week ET intervention (cardiovascular vs. motor fitness group) on the HPA axis activity in 8- to 10-year-old children. We hypothesized that cardiovascular exercise leads to an increased CAR response and motor exercise training results in a decreased, or no change in, CAR response among children.

Methods

Participants

Data1 of 71 prepubescent primary school children (39 female) between 9 and 10 years (Mage = 9.4; SDage = 0.6) with no psychological or physical impairments (e.g., obesity) were randomly assigned to a cardiovascular exercise group (CV, n = 27), a motor exercise group (MO, n = 23), or a control group (CON, n = 21).

Inclusion/Exclusion Criteria

All participants were recruited from local schools and inclusion criteria were 9–10 years of age, right-handedness, corrected-to or normal vision and prepubescent status according to parent and self-report on the Tanner staging system (below a score of 2 on the five-point scale) (20). In case of the presence of mental and physical impairments and/or previous or actual intake of psychoactive substances, participants were deemed ineligible. Before the study commenced, the ethics committee of the German Psychological Society approved the protocol (HB 02201 6_amd_092011). All participants and their legal guardians provided informed written consent after study procedures were explained in detail. The study was conducted following the guidelines set forth in the declaration of Helsinki and registered in the German Clinical Trials Register (DRKS00016590).

Measurements

Cortisol Awakening Response (CAR)

The CAR can be defined as the change in cortisol concentration immediately post-awakening: it represents a discrete aspect of the cortisol circadian cycle and has good intra-individual stability across time (21). Morning salivary cortisol levels were assessed at home at two time points. Children obtained saliva samples 0 and 30 min after awakening with the help of their parents, who were instructed personally and with the help of written manuals. Wakening time was 7 o'clock for all children. In order to increase reliability of the measure two cortisol samples (0, +30 min) were taken one week later both on the same day of the week pre- and post-intervention. Children were asked to refrain from physical activity one day before the assessment. Overall salivary cortisol was sampled eight times (see Figure 1 and Table 1). Saliva samples were analyzed from whole saliva collected via the SaliCap®system (IBL, Hamburg, Germany). For each assessment, participants were asked to accumulate saliva in their mouth for 2 min and refrain from swallowing while doing so. The accumulated saliva was then transferred into a pre-labeled vial via a straw. After arriving at school, research staff collected and stored the samples at −20°C until analysis. Cortisol levels (nmol/l) were analyzed using a commercially available enzyme-linked immunoassay (IBL, Hamburg, Germany) at the Biochemical Laboratory of the Department of Clinical Biopsychology, University of Marburg. Intra- and inter-assay coefficients of variation were 6.7 and 7.6%, respectively.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Salivary cortisol raw scores (nmol/l) for each measurement point pre (t1, t2) and post (t3, t4) intervention.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Salivary cortisol raw scores (nmol/l) for each time point.

CAR was calculated as the area under the curve (AUCg), in a first step using the mean of both saliva samples (immediately and 30 min after awakening) for pre and post-intervention separately, as it has been shown that awakening cortisol levels might be sensitive to differences in daily activities. In a final step, AUCg was calculated in accordance to Fekedulegn et al. (22). The raw scores for salivary cortisol are illustrated in Figure 1.

Cardiovascular Fitness (Card Fit)

Card Fit was tested with the Shuttle Run Test, a standard method for determining cardiorespiratory fitness in school children. Children were asked to run between two lines set 20 m apart. In accordance with the standardization used elsewhere (23), the children ran back and forth continuously with an initial speed of 8.0 km.h-1, increasing the level by 0.5 km h−1 each minute. Acoustic signals in a given frequency were used to control the pace. In all stages the students were motivated by cheering and by a pacemaker. We determined the HRmax as well as the maximum scores reached, which are the level and number of shuttles reached before fatigue (i.e., unable to maintain pace).

Motor Fitness (Mot Fit)

Mot Fit was assessed using the Heidelberg Gross Motor Test for children. We included the performance of six motor tasks [i.e., balance, rhythm, spatiotemporal orientation, and motor adaptation to moving objects; for further details see Koutsandréou et al. (19)] that were quantitatively measurable and calculated a sum score. For example, in the motor adaptation to moving objects task, points were earned by first throwing a ball backword through straddled legs against a 3 m distant wall and then, catching the rebounding ball (two points) or just touching or dropping it (one point).

Intervention

For 10 weeks, three times a week, for 45 min, an experienced exercise instructor trained the participants after school in groups of 7–14 children. The CV group trained their cardiovascular fitness via running and running-based games, however, varied to avoid boredom. The MO group focused on improving fine and gross motor body coordination through playful coordination exercises with low intensity: for the cardiovascular system. The control group received assisted homework sessions to prevent attention bias and control for retest effects (19). As previously reported (19) the three experimental groups (MO: 125.4 bpm, CV: 138.8 bpm, CON: 79.4 bpm) differed significantly regarding their mean heart rate levels: (recorded by F1 Polar HR monitors; Polar, Kempele, Finland) during exericse with MO and CV scoring significantly higher than CON but also CV scoring slightly higher than MO.

Procedure

For the pre-post-design of this study, cardiovascular and motor fitness were assessed in the week prior to the start of the intervention, and 1 week before the last intervention appointment. Following the recommendation by Hellhammer et al. (24), CAR was assessed twice prior and after the intervention: 1 week prior (1st time pre) and on the day of the start of the intervention (2nd time pre), as well as 1 week before the last intervention appointment and at the end of the 10-week intervention.

Statistical Analyses

For statistical analysis cortisol values were log-transformed to achieve normal distribution. A series of hierarchical regression analyses were performed in three blocks to predict post-intervention average CAR value for the two assessments (t3, t4), and to control for lower order effects before testing for higher order effects (see Table 1). In the first block we controlled for sex, the average value of CAR for the two pre-intervention assessments (t1, t2), pre-intervention cardiovascular fitness level, and age. In the second block, the experimental groups (MO vs. CON, CV vs. CON) were included as categorical variables in the model, as well as the change scores for cardiovascular and motor fitness. In the third block, the interaction terms between cardiovascular/motor fitness and intervention condition were added (cardiovascular fitness × CV; cardiovascular fitness × MO; motor fitness × CV; motor fitness × MO). For statistical analysis SPSS 24 software (IBM, Armonk, USA) was used.

Results

In order to test the effects of a 10-week physical exercise intervention on motor vs. cardiovascular fitness on CAR in children, we computed a hierarchical regression analysis using three blocks (see Table 2). In the first block, post-intervention CAR was residualized for age, gender, pre-cardiovascular performance, and pre-intervention CAR. Including pre-intervention CAR (B = 0.284, p = 0.001), age (B = 0.094, ns), gender (B = −0.170, ns), and pre-cardiovascular performance (B = −0.012, ns) rendered the regression analysis on post-intervention CAR significant, R2 = 0.187, F(4, 65) = 3.730, p = 0.009. In Model 2, we included changes in participants' intervention-induced cardiovascular (ΔCard Fit) and motor fitness (ΔMot Fit), as well as participants' training intervention [cardiovascular exercise training (CV) or motor exercise training group (MO)] compared to the control group in the model. In Model 3, the interaction terms (multiplicative term of z-standardized variables) of training intervention (CV vs. MO) by changes in participants' fitness levels (ΔMot Fit, ΔCard Fit) were added to the regression. Adding the cardiovascular and motor exercise group and the cardiovascular and motor pre-to-post-fitness changes did not result in a significant improvement in Model 2. However, including the interaction terms significantly improved Model 3, ΔR2 = 0.130, ΔF(4, 57) = 2.914, p = 0.029, and rendered the whole model significant, R2 = 0.366, F(12, 57) = 2.353, p = 0.005 (see Table 2). Participants in the CV who enhanced their cardiovascular fitness over the course of the intervention showed an increased CAR after the intervention time (B = 0.213), whereas children who underwent a motor exercise intervention and at the same time gained in motor fitness exhibited a decreased CAR after 10 weeks of intervention (B = −0.188; see Figure 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Results of the hierarchical regression analysis of post-intervention CAR (cortisol awakening response, log-transformed AUC) with gender, age, pre-intervention CAR, and performance in the shuttle run test (Pre CP) were entered in Model 1.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Post-intervention CARs (log-transformed AUC) in the three experimental groups (CV, MO, Control) as function of change in motor fitness (left graph) and change in cardiovascular fitness (right graph).

Discussion

The aim of this study was to investigate the effects of cardiovascular vs. motor exercise interventions on CAR in 9 to 10-year-old children. Our results show that cardiovascular and motor exercise exert different effects on HPA axis activity. Thus, specific exercises influence the HPA axis activity in different ways. Whereas, an increase in cardiovascular fitness was accompanied by an increase in HPA axis activity, particularly in children who underwent a cardiovascular exercise program, an increase in motor fitness in children who underwent a motor exercise program was accompanied by a decrease in CAR.

First, it should be kept in mind that it is still challenging to identify whether a large or small CAR is considered “healthy.” Second, to our knowledge, there is not enough systematic research on children, who represent an under-studied population. Research in this field is scarce, which makes it challenging to put results into context and highlights the importance of addressing the effects of different exercise interventions on the cortisol activity in children in future research. We base our argumentation on findings in adults showing that a higher increase in CAR is generally positively associated with job and general life stress (8).

Children who did increase their cardiovascular fitness, regardless of their experimental group assignment, showed an increase in cortisol activity. This is in line with previous findings in children indicating vigorous physical activity was positively related with 30 min post-waking cortisol values (11). With respect to children that did increase their cardiovascular fitness and presented an increase in cortisol activity, it could be argued that this is a result of an HPA axis hyper-responsiveness as a biological consequence of the frequent activation of the axis triggered by exercise stress: this may be part of the physiological adaptation of the neuroendocrine system to chronic demands (25).

Utilizing a set of salivary cortisol data in a small sample population of healthy older adults (mean age 65), a robust cortisol awakening response, and increased CAR after exercise training (a 6-month supervised intervention designed to reach 60–70% of their maximum heart rate reserve, 3 days a week, without any mentioned motor demands) were observed (26). However, one has to keep in mind that the cortisol response like many mental health related responses to exercise is depending on age (27).

Children in the motor exercise group that did increase their motor fitness showed a decrease in cortisol activity. This result is in line with research linking lower CAR to the duration of daytime sport (12). In a study with healthy young adults, the impact of long duration and high intensity of exam stress on the CAR supports that the HPA axis is down-regulated by chronic major stress, with this downregulation reflected by a reduction of the CAR (28). Although the mechanisms resulting in hypocortisolism are not yet fully clarified, possible explanations include changes in the biosynthesis of HPA-axis hormones and/or availability and functioning of their receptors at all levels of the HPA-axis [see Heim et al. (29) for discussion].

One could argue that due to improved motor abilities cognitive resources are freed to deal with complex situations in daily life resulting in a reduced chronic stress response (14). It has been further suggested that having control or no control over stress can have opposite effects on neural plasticity (15). Regarding the training of the cognitive system in this group one could speculate that it leads to an improved self-regulation and thus influencing neural input/traffic to the HPA axis. Results by Blair et al. (30) indicate that moderately higher levels of cortisol are associated with better performance on self-regulation, however it remains to be evaluated if this elevation in cortisol would lead to a change in CAR. One could also argue that the cognitive challenge of this exercise should induce neurogenesis (31). However, these argumentations would impede an explanation of the observed differences. Only the following hypothesis backed up by human data can somehow explain our findings. A recent study about the hippocampal volume in older humans after a 6-month intervention period, indicates that only, cardiovascular but not motor demanding training led to increases in hippocampal volume (17) which is positively related to the magnitude of the CAR (32).

One limitation of the study is that we only used two time points to assess the AUCg for the CAR. However, this procedure has been previously presented by different authors (5). Another limitation is that in the motor exercise group it is not possible to standardize the intensity for the neural nor for the cardiovascular system to compensate for motor demands of the different exercises, which can be challenging for one participant and more difficult for another participant. It cannot be ruled out that training programs matched for cardiovascular load, but with different coordinative demands would have resulted in a different pattern of results. As we previously reported both experimental exercise groups differed in intensity (19, 33).

Also, even though the children in the MO group were provided with playful exercises, and children in the CV group completed a variation of running exercises, it remains unclear how they were perceived by the children themselves. Finally, we did not control for subjectively perceived (chronic) stress prior or post-intervention. Future research, especially when focusing on children, should do so in order to control for potential confounding variables, as general life stress in adults has been associated with differences in CAR (8).

Another limitation of our study is that we did not assess abdominal fat in these children. It is known that abdominal fat may affect neuroendocrine responses mainly to psychological stress (34). However, significant changes in abdominal fat usually take place during puberty and children in our study were on average 9.4 years old which might diminish the effect of abdominal fat on HPA axis reactivity. Also, there is not enough systematic research on this specific understudied population and research in this field has not been well-discussed in the literature yet. This should be addressed in future studies and encourage researchers to add abdominal fat as a covariate.

One advantage of the present study though is that the AUCg used presents a more stable measure of the CAR because we used two AUCg measurements 1 week apart as previously recommended in the CAR guidelines by Stalder et al. (35). However, as training intensity and volume play a crucial role for the effects on CAR (6), future research should implement follow-up measurement points.

Overall, our results show that cardiovascular and motor exercise training in school exerts different effects on HPA axis activity. Whereas, an increase in cardiovascular fitness was accompanied by an increase in HPA axis activity, an increase in motor fitness in children was lead to a decrease in CAR.

Research has yet to produce more detailed and consistent findings to make a more meaningful statement regarding ET and its role on CAR, especially in children. The current study raises questions that future research needs to address in order to increase prevention of potential pathological diseases in childhood and adolescence.

Unfortunately, we simply do not know under what circumstances, for whom, and at what developmental periods under- vs. over-activation of the HPA-axis are most likely and how this is expressed by changes in CAR in this age group. While we suspect that under-activation of the HPA-axis may in fact be a reflection of more severe stress exposure and have more serious consequences than hyperactivation (36), however it needs to be established what the consequences are and under which cortisol concentration they occur.

Ethics Statement

Before the study commenced, the ethics committee of the German Psychological Society approved the protocol (HB 02201 6_amd_092011). All participants and their legal guardians provided informed written consent after study procedures were explained in detail. The study was conducted following the guidelines set forth in the declaration of Helsinki and registered in the German Clinical Trials Register (DRKS00016590).

Author Contributions

MW, HB, and FK contributed conception and design of the study. FK organized the database. FK and MW performed the statistical analysis. FK, MW, HB, and FL wrote the first draft of the manuscript. MW, HB, FK, FL, and AM-A wrote sections of the manuscript. All authors contributed to manuscript revision, read and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The study was conducted as part of the DFG (Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft) project BU1837/5-1.

Footnotes

1. ^Data from this sample (including working memory performance) have been previously reported (19) but neither of the analyses on HPA activity included in the current manuscript.

References

1. World Health Organization. Global Strategy on Diet, Physical Activity and Health - Childhood Overweight and Obesity (2017).

2. Yamada M, Arai H, Sonoda T, Aoyama T. Community-based exercise program is cost-effective by preventing care and disability in Japanese frail older adults. J Am Med Direct Assoc. (2012) 13:507–11. doi: 10.1016/j.jamda.2012.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Budde H, Akko DP, Ainamani HE, Murillo-Rodríguez E, Weierstall R. The impact of an exercise training intervention on cortisol levels and Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder in juveniles from an Ugandan refugee settlement: Study protocol for a randomized control trial. Trials. (2018) 19:364. doi: 10.1186/s13063-018-2753-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Budde H, Schwarz R, Velasques B, Ribeiro P, Holzweg M, Machado S, et al. The need for differentiating between exercise, physical activity, and training. Autoimmun Rev. (2015) 15:110–1. doi: 10.1016/j.autrev.2015.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Budde H, Machado S, Ribeiro P, Wegner M. The cortisol response to exercise in young adults. Front Behav Neurosci. (2015) 9:13. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Anderson T, Wideman L. Exercise and the cortisol awakening response: a systematic review. Sports Med Open. (2017) 3:37. doi: 10.1186/s40798-017-0102-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Rosmalen JGM, Oldehinkel AJ, Ormel J, de Winter AF, Buitelaar JK, Verhulst FC. Determinants of salivary cortisol levels in 10–12 year old children; a population-based study of individual differences. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2005) 30:483–95. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2004.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Chida Y, Steptoe A. Cortisol awakening response and psychosocial factors: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Biol Psychol. (2009) 80:265–78. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Gronwald T, Velasques B, Ribeiro P, Machado S, Murillo-Rodríguez E, Ludyga S, et al. Increasing exercise’s effect on mental health: exercise intensity does matter Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2018) 115:E11890–1. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1818161115

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Gronwald T, de Bem Alves A, Murillo-Rodríguez E, Latini A, Schuette J, Budde H. Standardization of exercise intensity and consideration of a doseresponse is essential. Commentary on “Exercise-linked FNDC5/irisin rescues synaptic plasticity and memory defects in Alzheimer’s models”, by Lourenco et al., published 2019 in Nature Medicine. J Sport Health Sci. (2019) 8:353–54. doi: 10.1016/j.jshs.2019.03.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. DuBose KD, McKune AJ. The relationship between objectively measured physical activity, salivary cortisol, and the metabolic syndrome score in girls. Pediatr. Exerc Sci. (2014) 26:221–30. doi: 10.1123/pes.2013-0109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. McHale S, Blocklin MK, Walter KN, Davis KD, Almeida DM, Cousino Klein L. The role of daily activities in youths' stress physiology. J Adolesc Health. (2012) 51:623–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2012.03.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Martikainen S, Pesonen AK, Lahti J, Heinonen K, Feldt K, Pyhälä T, et al. Higher levels of physical activity are associated with lower hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical axis reactivity to psychosocial stress in children. J Clin Endocrinol Metabol. (2013) 98:E619–27. doi: 10.1210/jc.2012-3745

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Godde B, Voelcker-Rehage C. Cognitive resources necessary for motor control in older adults are reduced by walking and coordination training. Front Hum Neurosci. (2017) 11:156. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00156

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Saaltink DJ, Vreugdenhil E. Stress, glucocorticoid receptors, and adult neurogenesis: a balance between excitation and inhibition? Cell Mol Life Sci. (2014) 71:2499–515. doi: 10.1007/s00018-014-1568-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Nokia MS, Lensu S, Ahtiainen JP, Johansson PP, Koch LG, Britton SL, et al. Physical exercise increases adult hippocampal neurogenesis in male rats provided it is aerobic and sustained. J Physiol. (2016) 594:1855–73. doi: 10.1113/JP271552

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Niemann C, Godde B, Staudinger UM, Voelcker-Rehage C. Exercise-induced changes in basal ganglia volume and cognition in older adults. Neuroscience. (2014) 281:147–63. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.09.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Ortega-Martínez S. Influences of prenatal and postnatal stress on adult hippocampal neurogenesis: the double neurogenic niche hypothesis. Behaviou Brain Res. (2015) 281:309–17. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.12.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

19. Koutsandréou F, Wegner M, Niemann C, Budde H. Effects of motor versus cardiovascular exercise training on children's working memory. Med Sci Sports Exerc. (2016) 48:1144–52. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000000869

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Tanner JM. Normal growth and techniques of growth assessment. J Clin Endocrinol Metabol. (1986) 15:411–51. doi: 10.1016/S0300-595X(86)80005-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Wüst S, Wolf J, Hellhammer DH, Federenko I, Schommer N, Kirschbaum C. The cortisol awakening response - normal values and confounds. Noise Health. (2000) 2:79–88.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

22. Fekedulegn DB, Andrew ME, Burchfiel CM, Violanti JM, Hartley TA, Charles LE, et al. Area under the curve and other summary indicators of repeated waking cortisol measurements. Psychosom Med. (2007) 69:651–9. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31814c405c

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. van Mechelen W, Hlobil H, Kemper HC. Validation of two running tests as estimates of maximal aerobic power in children. Eur J Appl Physiol Occupat Physiol. (1986) 55:503–6. doi: 10.1007/BF00421645

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Hellhammer J, Fries E, Schweisthal OW, Schlotz W, Stone AA, Hagemann D. Several daily measurements are necessary to reliably assess the cortisol rise after awakening: state- and trait components. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2007) 32:80–6. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2006.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Minetto MA, Lanfranco F, Tibaudi A, Baldi M, Termine A, Ghigo E. Changes in awakening cortisol response and midnight salivary cortisol are sensitive markers of strenuous training-induced fatigue. J Endocrinol Investig. (2008) 31:16–24. doi: 10.1007/BF03345561

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Drogos LL, Wynne-Edwards K, Zhou R, Hall SE, Tyndall AV, Longman RS, et al. Aerobic exercise increases cortisol awakening response in older adults. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2019) 103:241–8. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2019.01.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Budde H, Velasques B, Ribeiro P, Machado S, Emeljanovas A, Kamandulis S, et al. Does intensity or youth affect the neurobiological effect of exercise on major depressive disorder? Neurosci. Biobehav Rev. (2018) 84:492–4. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.09.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Duan H, Yuan Y, Zhang L, Qin S, Zhang K, Buchanan TW, et al. Chronic stress exposure decreases the cortisol awakening response in healthy young men. Stress. (2013) 16:630–7. doi: 10.3109/10253890.2013.840579

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Heim C, Ehlert U, Hellhammer DH. The potential role of hypocortisolism in the pathophysiology of stress-related bodily disorders. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2000) 25:1–35. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4530(99)00035-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Blair C, Granger D, Razza RP. Cortisol reactivity is positively related to executive function in preschool children attending head start. Child Dev. (2005) 76:554–67. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00863.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Fabel K, Kempermann G. Physical activity and the regulation of neurogenesis in the adult and aging brain. Neuromolecular Med. (2008) 10:59–66. doi: 10.1007/s12017-008-8031-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Pruessner M, Pruessner JC, Hellhammer DH, Bruce Pike G, Lupien SJ. The associations among hippocampal volume, cortisol reactivity, and memory performance in healthy young men. Psychiatry Res. (2007) 155:1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2006.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Garber CE, Blissmer B, Deschenes MR, Franklin B. Quantity and quality of exercise for developing and maintaining cardiorespiratory, musculoskeletal, and neuromotor fitness in apparently healthy adults: guidance for prescribing exercise. Med Sci Sports Exerc. (2011) 43:1334–59. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0b013e318213fefb

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Donoho CJ, Weigensberg MJ, Emken BA, Hsu JW, Spruijt-Metz D. Stress and abdominal fat: preliminary evidence of moderation by the cortisol awakening response in Hispanic peripubertal girls. Obesity. (2011) 19:946–52. doi: 10.1038/oby.2010.287

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Stalder T, Kirschbaum C, Kudielka BM, Adam EK, Pruessner JC, Wüst S, et al. Assessment of the cortisol awakening response: expert consensus guidelines. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2016) 63:414–32. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.10.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Gunnar MR, Vasquez DM. Low cortisol and a flattening of expected daytime rhythm: potential indices of risk in human development. Dev Psychopathol. (2001) 13:515–38. doi: 10.1017/S0954579401003066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cortisol, exercise training, physical stress, children, adolescents, motor exercise, cardiovascular exercise

Citation: Wegner M, Koutsandréou F, Müller-Alcazar A, Lautenbach F and Budde H (2019) Effects of Different Types of Exercise Training on the Cortisol Awakening Response in Children. Front. Endocrinol. 10:463. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2019.00463

Received: 21 January 2019; Accepted: 25 June 2019;
Published: 26 July 2019.

Edited by:

Denis Richard, Laval University, Canada

Reviewed by:

Gábor B. Makara, Hungarian Academy of Sciences (MTA), Hungary
Laurie Wideman, University of North Carolina at Greensboro, United States
Travis Anderson, University of North Carolina at Greensboro, United States, in Collaboration with reviewer LW
David H. St-Pierre, Université du Québec à Montréal, Canada

Copyright © 2019 Wegner, Koutsandréou, Müller-Alcazar, Lautenbach and Budde. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Mirko Wegner, bWlya28ud2VnbmVyJiN4MDAwNDA7aHUtYmVybGluLmRl

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.