Event Abstract

The Effect of Oxytocin on Autonomic Flexibility Following A Traumatic Experience: A Pilot Study

  • 1 Monash University, School of Psychological Sciences and Monash Institute for Cognitive and Clinical Neurosciences, Australia
  • 2 Caulfield Hospital, Pain Management and Research Centre, Australia
  • 3 Monash University, School of Public Health and Preventative Medicine, Australia
  • 4 Monash University, Institute for Safety, Compensation & Recovery Research, Australia

Aims: A traumatic experience can lead to poor emotion-regulation and hyperarousal. The hormone oxytocin appears to normalise emotional reactivity in persons with symptoms of psychopathology, and modulates the salience of social and emotional cues, leading to context-dependent changes in adaptive responding. The mechanistic effect of oxytocin following trauma remains unknown. Method: This small pilot study employed a double-blinded randomised crossover design to investigate the effect of oxytocin on autonomic-flexibility (high-frequency heart rate variability; HF-HRV) following a traumatic experience. This study also sought to determine whether effects were attributable to PTSD hyperarousal symptoms (Posttraumatic Checklist; PCL-5). Eleven participants (6 females, mean age 28) who experienced trauma (6-24 months prior) were asked to self-administer oxytocin (24IU) or placebo via nasal-spray, across two sessions at least one week apart. Each session included two tasks: a paced-breathing task (rest), which instructs participants to inhale and exhale at regular intervals, and the serial-sevens task (stress), which induces mild cognitive and physiological stress. HF-HRV was recorded from a 5-lead ECG system. Results: Following oxytocin administration, HF-HRV decreased during the paced-breathing task (p = 0.03) and serial-sevens task (p = 0.056) compared to placebo. This effect ceased when covarying for PTSD hyperarousal symptoms during the serial-sevens task (p = 0.08). Conclusions: These findings suggest that oxytocin increased the salience of cognitive stress, which was specifically attributable to hyperarousal symptoms. As an index of parasympathetic activity, lower HF-HRV suggests that oxytocin reduced the engagement of the parasympathetic nervous system (PNS). While caution is advised, given the small sample size, it appears that oxytocin may increase the perceived salience of, and attention to, stressors thereby exacerbating physiological stress states (i.e., decreasing the capacity of the PNS to dampen arousal).

References

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th ed.). United States of America: American Psychiatric Association.

Bartz, J. A., Zaki, J., Bolger, N., & Ochsner, K. N. (2011). Social effects of oxytocin in humans: Context and person matter. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(7), 301–309. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2011.05.002

Cardoso, C., Linnen, A.-M., Joober, R., & Ellenbogen, M. A. (2012). Coping style moderates the effect of intranasal oxytocin on the mood response to interpersonal stress. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 20(2), 84–91. doi:10.1037/a0025763

DePierro, J., D’Andrea, W., & Pole, N. (2013). Attention biases in female survivors of chronic interpersonal violence: Relationship to trauma-related symptoms and physiology. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 4, 1–10. doi:10.3402/ejpt.v4i0.19135

Fischer-Shofty, M., Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Harari, H., & Levkovitz, Y. (2010). The effect of intranasal administration of oxytocin on fear recognition. Neuropsychologia, 48(1), 179–184. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.09.003

Lanius, R. A., Vermetter, E., Loewenstein, R. J., Brand, B., Schmahl, C., Bremner, D., & Spiegel, D. (2010). Emotion Modulation in PTSD: Clinical and Neurobiological Evidence for a Dissociative Subtype. Am J Psychiatry, 167(6), 640–647. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09081168.Emotion

Lischke, A., Gamer, M., Berger, C., Grossmann, A., Hauenstein, K., Heinrichs, M., … Domes, G. (2012). Oxytocin increases amygdala reactivity to threatening scenes in females. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 37(9), 1431–1438. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.01.011

Olff, M., Frijling, J. L., Kubzansky, L. D., Bradley, B., Ellenbogen, M. A, Cardoso, C., … van Zuiden, M. (2013). The role of oxytocin in social bonding, stress regulation and mental health: an update on the moderating effects of context and interindividual differences. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 38(9), 1883–94. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.06.019

Quirin, M., Kuhl, J., & Dusing, R. (2011). Oxytocin buffers cortisol responses to stress in individuals with impaired emotion regulation abilities. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 36(6), 898-904. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.12.005

Thayer, J. F., Åhs, F., Fredrikson, M., Sollers, J. J., & Wager, T. D. (2012). A meta-analysis of heart rate variability and neuroimaging studies: Implications for heart rate variability as a marker of stress and health. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(2), 747–56. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.11.009

Keywords: Heart rate variability, Oxytocin, PTSD, Trauma, hyperarousal

Conference: ASP2017: 27th Annual Meeting for the Australasian Society for Psychophysiology, Parramatta, Australia, 29 Nov - 1 Dec, 2017.

Presentation Type: Poster Presentation

Topic: Abstract (General)

Citation: Perkins M, Giummarra MJ and Tracy LM (2019). The Effect of Oxytocin on Autonomic Flexibility Following A Traumatic Experience: A Pilot Study. Conference Abstract: ASP2017: 27th Annual Meeting for the Australasian Society for Psychophysiology. doi: 10.3389/conf.fnhum.2017.224.00030

Copyright: The abstracts in this collection have not been subject to any Frontiers peer review or checks, and are not endorsed by Frontiers. They are made available through the Frontiers publishing platform as a service to conference organizers and presenters.

The copyright in the individual abstracts is owned by the author of each abstract or his/her employer unless otherwise stated.

Each abstract, as well as the collection of abstracts, are published under a Creative Commons CC-BY 4.0 (attribution) licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) and may thus be reproduced, translated, adapted and be the subject of derivative works provided the authors and Frontiers are attributed.

For Frontiers’ terms and conditions please see https://www.frontiersin.org/legal/terms-and-conditions.

Received: 14 Oct 2017; Published Online: 25 Jan 2019.

* Correspondence: Ms. Monica Perkins, Monash University, School of Psychological Sciences and Monash Institute for Cognitive and Clinical Neurosciences, Melbourne, Australia, monicaperkins122@gmail.com