- 1Department of Sports Medicine and Medical Rehabilitation, I.M. Sechenov First Moscow State Medical University, Ministry of Health of the Russia Federation, Moscow, Russia
- 2Federal Scientific and Clinical Center for Sports Medicine of the Federal Biomedical Agency, Moscow, Russia
- 3Department of Rehabilitation and Sports Medicine, Kazan State Medical University, Kazan, Russia
Objectives: Enhancing physical performance and improving load tolerance through safe methods is a priority for most athletes. One potentially beneficial approach is listening to music, which exerts positive effects through various mechanisms. This study aims to investigate the influence of music on athletic performance and endurance, focusing specifically on its potential as an ergogenic aid in team sports—an area that has received less attention compared to individual sports.
Methods: To examine the effects of music on physical performance and load tolerance in team sports athletes, we conducted a systematic search for original English-language articles in PubMed, Mendeley, and the Cochrane Library from inception to June 2024, following PRISMA guidelines.
Results: The search identified eight studies that met the inclusion criteria, involving 140 participants from football, volleyball, and basketball. All studies demonstrated a low risk of bias. None of the studies included elite adult international-level athletes. The analysed parameters included peak power, sprint and jump performance, maximal oxygen consumption, repeated sprint ability, change of direction, and load tolerance indicators such as heart rate, rating of perceived exertion (RPE), and fatigue index. Most studies demonstrated a significant positive effect of music on these parameters; however, the protocols for music accompaniment were not standardised.
Conclusion: The findings suggest that music can positively impact both physiological and psychological factors, though its application in team settings requires further investigation. Given its safety and accessibility, music may serve as a valuable tool for enhancing performance in various sporting contexts. Future studies should include more detailed information on music usage protocols and involve larger sample sizes, particularly including adult elite athletes.
Introduction
Enhancing performance is a key priority for professional athletes. It can be improved by directly enhancing various physical parameters and by improving load tolerance and accelerating post-load recovery (1, 2). Effective ergogenic aids must be well-tolerated, safe, and compliant with anti-doping regulations (3, 4). At the highest competitive level, success is often determined by fractions of a second, and most competitors regularly use permitted dietary supplements and substances to improve performance (5–7). However, the list of permitted substances with proven efficacy is limited (8).
In this context, non-pharmacological aids that boost performance without risking anti-doping violations are gaining momentum. These include mouth rinses (9), music listening (10), body cooling during competitions in high temperatures (11), transcranial brain cortex stimulation (12), and other methods.
It is not surprising that the number of studies in this area is increasing. One of the most studied methods for enhancing performance is music listening, whose influence on various physical indicators began to be investigated last century (13, 14), but has become more widespread in the last decade (10). Regarding sports performance, music listening has been shown to increase anaerobic performance, improve perceptual-cognitive skills, and reduce rate of perceived exertion (RPE) (15, 16).
Possible mechanisms for enhancing performance through music are thoroughly described in reviews by Ballman and Terry et al. These mechanisms involve a combination of psychological, physiological, and psychophysiological factors (17, 18). Listening to music can enhance mood, reduce perceived fatigue, and increase positive emotions (19). For example, Hutchinson et al. found that participants who listened to music while running could maintain high intensity for longer while perceiving less exertion (20). Additionally, music during warm-up can increase alertness and peak power in Wingate tests among elite sprinters (21).
Music primarily affects physiological responses in the nervous and metabolic systems. Bigliassi et al. found that performing motor tasks while listening to music increased activity in the left inferior frontal gyrus (22). Activation of this brain region is associated with reduced RPE and improved movement organization. According to Jia et al., music listening during cycling can enhance parasympathetic reactivation and reduce heart rate variability after exercise (23). Music listening during physical exercise can also increase oxygen consumption and cardiac output, improving muscle oxygenation and reducing blood lactate levels (24–26). During physical activity, psychophysiological changes like increased arousal, reduced RPE, and improved autonomic control are observed. These effects are believed to stem from the combined influence of music on both psychological and physiological processes (18).
The results of the most recent systematic review and meta-analysis examining the effects of pre-task music on exercise performance and associated psychophysiological responses show that pre-task music enhances psychological responses and alleviates fatigue-related symptoms associated with exercise performance improvement. This effect is particularly pronounced when self-selected music is used and is more evident in active individuals than in trained athletes (10). However, the included studies mainly focused on healthy populations not involved in professional sport and were limited to listening to music exclusively before physical activity. In particular, despite evidence of the beneficial effects of music on various performance parameters, such as peak and mean power (27), repetitive sprint ability (28), speed (29), endurance (30), and mitigation of heat-related declines in exercise performance (31). These studies involve non-elite athletes and amateurs from individual sports, lack high-intensity physical testing to failure, and do not include sport-specific skill testing. Most existing studies have several limitations that hinder the extrapolation of their findings to team sports, particularly at the professional athlete level.
Another critical aspect to consider is the inapplicability of protocols used in individual sports to team-based settings. One of the key factors determining the effectiveness of music as an ergogenic aid is individual preference (18). It is almost impossible to select a single type of music that would be universally preferred by all team members. Consequently, in team sports, listening to music as an ergogenic strategy is likely to be most effective during training sessions where individual listening devices (e.g., portable headphones) can be used. However, this approach also presents challenges due to the need to maintain communication between team members. Thus, in team sports, music can primarily be used as an ergogenic aid during training or pre-competition through individual listening devices such as portable headphones.
Given the limitations of previous work, it may be of practical relevance to investigate the effects of different music application protocols as a potential ergogenic strategy in team sports. This systematic review aims to assess the ergogenic effects of music on physical performance and load tolerance in team sports athletes, providing a foundation for understanding its broader potential, including possible influences on sport-specific skills. This systematic review addresses the following question using the PICOS framework: In healthy professional team sports athletes (Population), does listening to music (Intervention) compared to no music (Comparison) enhance physical performance and load tolerance (Outcomes), as evaluated in randomized controlled trials (Study Design)?
Materials and methods
Search strategy
To identify studies on the effects of music listening on performance and load tolerance in healthy professional team sports athletes, a search was conducted in PubMed, Mendeley, and the Cochrane Library databases from their inception to June 16, 2024, according to PRISMA guidelines (32). The work within this study, including selection, analysis, and evaluation, was conducted from June to August 2024, with continuous updates based on newly released research findings. The search query was: (music OR melody OR “listening to music” OR “music playback” OR “music playing” OR “music listening” OR “musical intervention” OR “musical therapy” OR “background music” OR “music exposure” OR beats) AND (performance OR “technical performance” OR “physical performance” OR “mental performance” OR “physical indicators” OR “physical fitness” OR “loads” OR “workload” OR “recovery” OR “endurance” OR “stamina” OR “maximal aerobic power” OR “cardiorespiratory fitness” OR “strength” OR “muscular strength” OR “speed” OR “coordination” OR “ergogenic aids” OR “recovery state” OR “psychological recovery” OR “well-being” OR “mental state” OR “mood state”) AND (“football players” OR “soccer players” OR “footballers” OR “soccer athletes” OR “athletes” OR “sports players” OR “team sports”). No search filters or limits were applied; only the specified search query was used in each of the databases.
All relevant articles found were reviewed. The PICOS criteria were used to formulate the eligibility criteria (33):
- P (Population): Healthy athletes participating in team sports at various levels of sporting proficiency.
- I (Intervention): Listening to various types of music (different frequencies, through different devices).
- C (Comparison): Comparison was made between participants who listened to music during the experiment and those who did not.
- O (Outcome): Various changes in performance, load tolerance, and fatigue levels were analysed.
- S (Study Design): The review included original randomized controlled trials with parallel groups or crossover design conducted on humans.
Study selection
The inclusion criteria for this review were as follows: studies published in English, original randomized controlled trials with parallel groups or crossover design conducted on humans, and studies examining the effects of listening to various types of music (different frequencies, through different devices). No specific exclusion criteria were applied beyond these parameters.
An initial literature search created a selection of studies covering various aspects of music listening. The studies were analysed based on design, participant characteristics, performance assessment methods, musical conditions, experiment duration, and other relevant factors. The studies were classified by type of sport and experimental results. After analysing the publications, the results were compared, and conclusions were drawn about the effects of music listening on physical performance and load tolerance in healthy professional team sports athletes. These studies were analysed in detail by two independent researchers, V.T.M. and K.E.S. In cases of disagreement on data interpretation, a senior expert, B.E.N., was consulted. The search results were downloaded and filtered in the Mendeley Reference Manager v2.64.0 (Mendeley Ltd. UK) systematic review software. A manual search helped identify other suitable articles in eligible full-text articles to be incorporated in the systematic review. A consensus was formed on the final studies included. All identified studies were assessed for risk of bias employing the revised Cochrane Risk of Bias Tool for Randomised Trials (RoB 2) (34). The RoB2 was covering the following evaluation domains: bias arising from the randomization process, bias due to deviations from intended interventions, bias due to missing outcome data, bias in measurement of the outcome and bias in selection of the reported result. Two independent reviewers V.T.M. and K.E.S assessed the risk of bias, and in cases of disagreement, a third reviewer B.E.N. was consulted to reach a consensus. This approach ensures the reliability and reproducibility of our findings.
This systematic review was submitted for registration in PROSPERO; however, the registration was declined as PROSPERO does not accept systematic reviews related to performance in sports. To ensure transparency, we adhered to rigorous methodological guidelines and provided a detailed description of our review protocol within this manuscript.
Synthesis of result
Due to significant variability in music protocols (e.g., selection criteria, tempo ranging from 60 to >140 bpm, and listening methods such as individual headphones or shared stereo systems), a narrative synthesis was chosen over a meta-analysis to qualitatively summarize the findings.
Results
Study selection
Based on keywords and their combinations, 1,116 records were found in the analysed databases. Subsequently, 140 records were excluded (139 duplicates, and 1 article was removed due to lack of data). After analysing titles and abstracts, 932 records were excluded because they did not meet the main criteria and topic of this systematic review. Of the remaining 44 studies, 38 were excluded after thorough screening: 4 had no research results (incomplete study), 4 were abstracts only, 3 lacked a control group, and 27 involved amateur athletes. Additionally, reference lists of each study were reviewed to minimize the risk of missing suitable studies for this systematic review. Out of 277 documents in reference lists, 2 studies met all PICOS criteria and were included in the review. Finally, 8 publications in English describing studies with healthy professional team sports athletes investigating the effects of music listening on performance and load tolerance were identified (Figure 1, Table 1).

Table 1. Studies on the effects of music listening on various indicators in healthy professional team sports athletes.
Study participants
The total number of research participants was 140, comprising 114 males and 26 females—athletes from team sports—with an average age ranging from 14.85 to 25.5 years. In three studies, the participants were football players whose level of sports training ranged from semi-professional (35, 36) to elite athletes (37). A further three studies involved elite basketball players (38–40). Two studies focused on volleyball players from the 1st to 3rd divisions (41) and the national team (42).
Music protocol
In three studies, the choice of music played was made by the researchers (37, 39, 42). In two studies, the music was chosen by the participants (38, 40), whereby in one of these studies the participants had to choose music from a list provided by the researchers (38). In the remaining studies, the music was selected by both the participants and the researchers, with the groups split accordingly (35, 36, 41).
The tempo of the selected music was up to 120 bpm (35, 41) and higher in all studies, including (35, 41). In the study by Blasco-Lafarga et al., the tempo ranged from 'slow' to “fast” music (113.91 ± 30.68 bpm and 116.02 ± 17.30 bpm, respectively), which participants classified as low and high motivating music (38).
Only one study reported an exact value for volume (70 decibels) (39)]. Another study mentioned that the sound intensity was set at 70% of the maximum possible level (42). Two other studies indicated that the intensity was moderate and comfortable (36, 40), while the remaining studies did not provide descriptions of the sound intensity (35, 37, 38, 41). Five studies used individual headphones (35–37, 40, 41), two used a common stereo system (39, 42), and one did not specify (38). The duration of music playback did not exceed 2 min (41), 10 min (35, 36) or 15 min (37). The remaining studies did not report the exact duration of listening. However, Eliakim et al., 2012, Eliakim et al., 2007 and Hammami et al., 2021 reported the total duration of the music playlist (39, 40, 42).
Outcomes
The results are presented in Table 1 and included jump test height and power, sprint test distance and time (e.g., RSA, R-COD, V-cut test, 30-15 IFT) and power measured during the Wingate anaerobic test. In addition, psychometric indicators, RPE, heart rate during different phases of physical activity, lactate level, VO2max, SaO2 and body temperature were analysed.
Risk of bias/quality appraisal
Most studies included in this review can be considered high methodological quality: all were randomized with sufficient sample size and control groups (Table 2). The studies included in this review conducted their own sample size calculations using various established methodologies. Each study justified its sample size based on statistical considerations, such as power analysis, effect size estimation, or adherence to previously validated study designs. As a result, the included studies reported sufficient sample sizes to support the validity of their findings, which collectively strengthen the reliability of our review.
Discussion
This review highlights the growing interest in music as a performance enhancer, with seven of the eight included studies published within the last five years. This may be due to existing data on the positive effects of music listening on performance and load tolerance in individual sports athletes (43, 44).
In accordance with the predefined criteria, the review included only studies involving athletes from team sports. Most frequently, the participants were young athletes, which is reasonable given that the use of various substances as ergogenic aids may be challenging in this cohort. The participants' level ranged from semi-professional to elite athletes; however, no elite adult athletes were included.
Music was most frequently listened during warm-up before testing in four studies included in this review (35–37, 42), which is likely the most replicable protocol under team sports conditions. In three studies, music was listened to during physical testing (38–40), and in one, music started one minute before testing and continued during testing (41).
Notably, no studies compared music listening during different periods of training (e.g., warm-up vs. sport-specific load or rest), although there is evidence that the effect of different tempo music may vary depending on the time of day (35, 36).
Additionally, the studies used varied music listening protocols. Music was presented both through personal headphones and through a stereo system. Individual headphones are likely more optimal as they allow athletes to choose their preferred music and change it at any time. According to Belkhir et al. and Bentouati et al., participant-chosen music had a greater impact compared to researcher-chosen music (35, 36, 45). These findings support Ballmann et al.'s review, noting that preferred music has the most significant effect on various performance aspects (18). It is worth noting that future studies should consider the convenience of using headphones during physical activity, as well as the impact of headphone use on physical performance. This is especially important if participants in the control group perform the same tasks while wearing headphones without sound, as the presence of a noise-canceling mode may trigger various physiological responses (46, 47).
Only one study provided precise digital values for sound intensity (70 decibels) (39). Another mentioned that intensity was 70% of the maximum possible (42), two described intensity as moderate and comfortable (36, 40), and the rest did not describe sound intensity (35, 37, 38, 41). The analysed studies did not compare the effects of music at different sound intensities; however, previous research involving healthy individuals, amateur athletes, and highly trained taekwondo athletes demonstrated significant benefits from using music with higher intensity/volume (48–50). Five studies limited the tempo to >120 bpm (35, 37, 39, 40, 42), while others varied the tempo from 60 to 140 bpm (36, 38, 41). Music with a tempo >120 bpm showed more positive effects on performance indicators compared to slower music (36, 38). A more detailed description of the music playback protocol would enhance comparability, reproducibility, and practical recommendations for athletes and their coaches.
All studies except one (41) provided evidence of the positive effects of music listening on various physical performance indicators (power, speed, jump height, VO2max) and load tolerance (heart rate, RPE, fatigue index).
One study found that low-motivation music significantly reduced RPE compared to high-motivation music and control group (38). In the high-motivation music group, there was a significant performance improvement in the 30-15 IFT test, which could explain the greater load in this group and, consequently, the absence of a significant decrease in RPE. Another study showed that RPE was significantly higher after testing (35) and warm-up (37, 42) compared to control groups, potentially due to increased performance and activity levels during these phases. Other studies did not include psychological factors such as RPE in their protocols; however, listening to music was shown to improve athletes' well-being after exertion (35, 36).
A significant reduction in the fatigue index was found in only one of three studies analysing this parameter (36). However, the absence of significant changes in the fatigue index in studies by Eliakim et al. and Hammami et al., with simultaneous significant increases in performance in anaerobic tests, likely indicates better load tolerance among participants listening to music (40, 42).
Regarding the physiological effects of music, two out of three studies found that listening to music significantly increased average heart rate during physical activity (39, 42), while post-exercise heart rate and recovery heart rate remained unchanged (42). One study also demonstrated a significant increase in VO2max, along with improved 30-15IFT test performance in the music group compared to the control group and low motivation music group (38). Other physiological parameters, including lactate levels, body temperature, and blood oxygen saturation, were assessed, but no significant changes were observed.
Regarding strength performance, data on the positive effects of music are contradictory. Belkhir et al. found significant increases in maximal and average jump height (36), whereas Gavanda et al. found no significant changes (41). Increased peak power during the Wingate test was observed, independent of gender, in both male and female participants (42). Tounsi et al. noted that listening to music during warm-up before the RSA test can improved performance in women, it only affects internal load in men (37). Most studies showed positive effects of music on sprint tests, including significant increases in sprint speed during the 30-15 IFT test, reduced sprint time during repeat sprint ability and repeat change of direction tests, and increased total and maximal distance in shuttle sprint tests (35, 37–40).
Limitations
While this review demonstrates music's positive effects on general physical performance indicators, it does not extensively address sport-specific skills such as technical execution under pressure or decision-making in game-like scenarios. These skills are integral to team sports performance, and future studies should investigate how music influences them, potentially through protocols simulating competitive conditions.
A notable limitation of the included studies is the absence of elite international-level athletes. While the participants were professional athletes from football, volleyball, and basketball, their competitive level may not reflect the demands of top-tier international competition. Future studies should prioritize including elite athletes in realistic training or competitive settings to determine whether music's ergogenic effects are consistent or amplified at higher performance levels, thereby enhancing the external validity of findings.
In addition, there is considerable variability in music intervention protocols, which could influence the observed ergogenic effects. Factors such as music preference, tempo, genre and individual motivation levels vary between studies, making direct comparisons difficult. In team sports, this variability is particularly relevant, as it is difficult to select a single type of music that will suit all players.
Directions for future research
To further investigate the effects of music on athletic performance and psychophysiological indicators, it is essential to develop studies that address existing limitations and extend current knowledge. Firstly, it is crucial to include international elite athletes in the research to increase the external validity of the results. This will allow the investigation of how music affects performance during high-intensity training and competition, as well as the identification of individual athlete preferences that may be associated with their psychophysiological responses.
Secondly, to ensure reproducibility of results, it is necessary to standardise the parameters of the musical accompaniment. This includes defining optimal tempo ranges (e.g., 120–140 BPM for aerobic activities), controlling volume levels and listening duration, and taking into account the cultural and individual preferences of athletes. Such measures will help to minimise variability and increase data reliability.
Third, to objectively assess the psychological effects of music, it is recommended to use validated tools such as the Profile of Mood States (POMS) to measure mood changes, the Rating of Perceived Exertion (RPE) scale to assess subjective perception of exertion, and motivation questionnaires (e.g., BRUMS). This will provide more accurate data on the effects of music on emotional state and athlete engagement.
In addition, it is important to study the long-term effects of music on performance and psychophysiological indicators. Longitudinal studies will help to understand how the regular use of music in training affects athletes' adaptation, motivation and fatigue levels over months or even years.
Finally, it is necessary to investigate whether the positive effects of music used during warm-up are maintained during competition. This includes analysing the effects of music on cognitive functions such as concentration and decision-making under competitive stress, and comparing the performance of athletes with and without music during warm-up.
Conclusion
The findings suggest that music can positively impact both physiological and psychological factors, though its application in team settings requires further investigation. Given its safety and accessibility, music may serve as a valuable tool for enhancing performance in various sporting contexts. Future studies should include more detailed information on music usage protocols and involve larger sample sizes, particularly including adult elite athletes.
Data availability statement
The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.
Author contributions
EB: Conceptualization, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – review & editing. TV: Writing – original draft, Resources, Data curation, Project administration. EK: Validation, Data curation, Writing – original draft, Formal analysis, Visualization. GM: Writing – original draft, Software, Investigation, Resources, Validation. MV: Writing – review & editing, Formal analysis, Software, Data curation, Visualization. SC: Methodology, Data curation, Writing – review & editing, Project administration, Funding acquisition. MB: Writing – review & editing, Funding acquisition, Software, Methodology, Data curation, Supervision.
Funding
The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Generative AI statement
The author(s) declare that no Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.
Publisher's note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
1. Hostrup M, Bangsbo J. Performance adaptations to intensified training in top-level football. Sports Med. (2023) 53(3):577–94. doi: 10.1007/s40279-022-01791-z
2. Kellmann M, Bertollo M, Bosquet L, Brink M, Coutts AJ, Duffield R, et al. Recovery and performance in sport: consensus statement. Int J Sports Physiol Perform. (2018) 13(2):240–5. doi: 10.1123/ijspp.2017-0759
3. Kerksick CM, Wilborn CD, Roberts MD, Smith-Ryan A, Kleiner SM, Jäger R, et al. ISSN exercise & sports nutrition review update: research & recommendations. J Int Soc Sports Nutr. (2018) 15:1–57. doi: 10.1186/s12970-018-0242-y
4. WADA. World Anti-Doping Code International Standard Prohibited List (2023). Available at: https://www.wada-ama.org/sites/default/files/2022-09/2023list_en_final_9_september_2022.pdf (Accessed September 10, 2024).
5. Frączek B, Warzecha M, Tyrała F, Pięta A. Prevalence of the use of effective ergogenic aids among professional athletes. Rocz Panstw Zakl Hig. (2016) 67(3):271–8.
6. McDuff D, Stull T, Castaldelli-Maia JM, Hitchcock ME, Hainline B, Reardon CL. Recreational and ergogenic substance use and substance use disorders in elite athletes: a narrative review. Br J Sports Med. (2019) 53(12):754–60. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2019-100669
7. Королева Е, Бутовский М, Малякин Г, Лазарев А, Телышев Д, Вахидов Т. Распространенность Употребления Алкоголя И Предтренировочного Кофеина И Их Влияние На Травматизм И Нарушения Сна Среди Элитных Молодых Футболистов. Спортивная медицина: наука и практика. (2023) 13(2):5–12. doi: 10.47529/2223-2524.2023.2.4
8. Peeling P, Binnie MJ, Goods PS, Sim M, Burke LM. Evidence-based supplements for the enhancement of athletic performance. Int J Sport Nutr Exerc Metab. (2018) 28(2):178–87. doi: 10.1123/ijsnem.2017-0343
9. Burke LM, Maughan RJ. The governor has a sweet tooth–mouth sensing of nutrients to enhance sports performance. Eur J Sport Sci. (2015) 15(1):29–40. doi: 10.1080/17461391.2014.971880
10. Delleli S, Ouergui I, Ballmann CG, Messaoudi H, Trabelsi K, Ardigò LP, et al. The effects of pre-task music on exercise performance and associated psycho-physiological responses: a systematic review with multilevel meta-analysis of controlled studies. Front Psychol. (2023) 14:1293783. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1293783
11. Adams WM, Hosokawa Y, Casa DJ. Body-cooling paradigm in sport: maximizing safety and performance during competition. J Sport Rehabil. (2016) 25(4):382–94. doi: 10.1123/jsr.2015-0008
12. Maudrich T, Ragert P, Perrey S, Kenville R. Single-session anodal transcranial direct current stimulation to enhance sport-specific performance in athletes: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Brain Stimul. (2022) 15(6):1517–29. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2022.11.007
13. Anshel MH, Marisi DQ. Effect of music and rhythm on physical performance. Res Q. (1978) 49(2):109–13. doi: 10.1080/10671315.1978.10615514
14. Karageorghis CI, Terry PC. The psychophysical effects of music in sport and exercise: a review. J Sport Behav. (1997) 20(1):54. doi: 10.5555/19971804084
15. Castañeda-Babarro A, Marqués-Jiménez D, Calleja-González J, Viribay A, León-Guereño P, Mielgo-Ayuso J. Effect of listening to music on wingate anaerobic test performance. A systematic review and meta-analysis. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2020) 17(12):4564. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17124564
16. Ouergui I, Jebabli E, Delleli S, Messaoudi H, Bridge CA, Chtourou H, et al. Listening to preferred and loud music enhances taekwondo physical performances in adolescent athletes. Percept Mot Skills. (2023) 130(4):1644–62. doi: 10.1177/00315125231178067
17. Terry PC, Karageorghis CI, Curran ML, Martin OV, Parsons-Smith RL. Effects of music in exercise and sport: a meta-analytic review. Psychol Bull. (2020) 146(2):91. doi: 10.1037/bul0000216
18. Ballmann CG. The influence of music preference on exercise responses and performance: a review. J Funct Morphol Kinesiol. (2021) 6(2):33. doi: 10.3390/jfmk6020033
19. Thompson WF, Schellenberg EG, Husain G. Arousal, mood, and the mozart effect. Psychol Sci. (2001) 12(3):248–51. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.003
20. Hutchinson JC, Jones L, Vitti SN, Moore A, Dalton PC, O'Neil BJ. The influence of self-selected music on affect-regulated exercise intensity and remembered pleasure during treadmill running. Sport Exerc Perform Psychol. (2018) 7(1):80. doi: 10.1037/spy0000115
21. Chtourou H, Jarraya M, Aloui A, Hammouda O, Souissi N. The effects of music during warm-up on anaerobic performances of young sprinters. Sci Sports. (2012) 27(6):e85-e8. doi: 10.1016/j.scispo.2012.02.006
22. Bigliassi M, Karageorghis CI, Bishop DT, Nowicky AV, Wright MJ. Cerebral effects of music during isometric exercise: an fmri study. Int J Psychophysiol. (2018) 133:131–9. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2018.07.475
23. Jia T, Ogawa Y, Miura M, Ito O, Kohzuki M. Music attenuated a decrease in parasympathetic nervous system activity after exercise. PLoS One. (2016) 11(2):e0148648. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0148648
24. Birnbaum L, Boone T, Huschle B. Cardiovascular responses to music tempo during steady-state exercise. J Exerc Physiol Online. (2009) 12(1):50–7.
25. Sonmez GT, Vatansever S, Olcucu B, Cinar V. Impact of music on exercise performance. Int J Rev Life Sci. (2015) 5:1307–12.
26. Eliakim M, Bodner E, Eliakim A, Nemet D, Meckel Y. Effect of motivational music on lactate levels during recovery from intense exercise. J Strength Cond Res. (2012) 26(1):80–6. doi: 10.1519/JSC.0b013e31821d5f31
27. Chtourou H, Chaouachi A, Hammouda O, Chamari K, Souissi N. Listening to music affects diurnal variation in muscle power output. Int J Sports Med. (2012) 33(01):43–7. doi: 10.1055/s-0031-1284398
28. Jebabli N, Ben Aabderrahman A, Boullosa D, Chtourou H, Ouerghi N, Rhibi F, et al. Listening to music during a repeated sprint test improves performance and psychophysiological responses in healthy and physically active male adults. BMC Sports Sci Med Rehabil. (2023) 15(1):21. doi: 10.1186/s13102-023-00619-1
29. Chtourou H, Hmida C, Souissi N. Effect of music on short-term maximal performance: sprinters vs. long distance runners. Sport Sci Health. (2017) 13:213–6. doi: 10.1007/s11332-017-0357-6
30. Thakare AE, Mehrotra R, Singh A. Effect of music tempo on exercise performance and heart rate among young adults. Int J Physiol Pathophysiol Pharmacol. (2017) 9(2):35.28533890
31. English T, Mavros Y, Jay O. Listening to motivational music mitigates heat-related reductions in exercise performance. Physiol Behav. (2019) 208:112567. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2019.112567
32. Ardern CL, Büttner F, Andrade R, Weir A, Ashe MC, Holden S, et al. Implementing the 27 prisma 2020 statement items for systematic reviews in the sport and exercise medicine, musculoskeletal rehabilitation and sports science fields: the persist (implementing prisma in exercise, rehabilitation, sport medicine and sports science) guidance. Br J Sports Med. (2022) 56(4):175–95. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2021-103987
33. Methley AM, Campbell S, Chew-Graham C, McNally R, Cheraghi-Sohi S. Pico, picos and spider: a comparison study of specificity and sensitivity in three search tools for qualitative systematic reviews. BMC Health Serv Res. (2014) 14(1):1–10. doi: 10.1186/s12913-014-0579-0
34. Sterne JAC, Savović J, Page MJ, Elbers RG, Blencowe NS, Boutron I, et al. Rob 2: a revised tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. Br Med J. (2019) 366:l4898. doi: 10.1136/bmj.l4898
35. Belkhir Y, Rekik G, Chtourou H, Souissi N. Listening to neutral or self-selected motivational music during warm-up to improve short-term maximal performance in soccer players: effect of time of day. Physiol Behav. (2019) 204:168–73. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2019.02.033
36. Belkhir Y, Rekik G, Chtourou H, Souissi N. Does warming up with different music tempos affect physical and psychological responses? The evidence from a chronobiological study. J Sports Med Phys Fitness. (2021) 62(1):149–56. doi: 10.23736/s0022-4707.21.12093-6
37. Tounsi M, Jaafar H, Aloui A, Tabka Z, Trabelsi Y. Effect of listening to music on repeated-sprint performance and affective load in young male and female soccer players. Sport Sci Health. (2019) 15:337–42. doi: 10.1007/s11332-018-0518-2
38. Blasco-Lafarga C, Ricart B, Cordellat A, Roldán A, Navarro-Roncal C, Monteagudo P. High versus low motivating music on intermittent fitness and agility in young well-trained basketball players. Int J Sport Exerc Psychol. (2022) 20(3):777–93. doi: 10.1080/1612197X.2021.1907762
39. Eliakim M, Meckel Y, Gotlieb R, Nemet D, Eliakim A. Motivational music and repeated sprint ability in junior basketball players. Acta Kinesiol Univ Tartu. (2012) 18:29–38. doi: 10.12697/akut.2012.18.04
40. Hammami R, Nebigh A, Selmi MA, Rebai H, Versic S, Drid P, et al. Acute effects of verbal encouragement and listening to preferred music on maximal repeated change-of-direction performance in adolescent elite basketball players—preliminary report. Appl Sci. (2021) 11(18):8625. doi: 10.3390/app11188625
41. Gavanda S, Hosang T, Grasser J, Wagener S, Sönmez N, Kayser I, et al. The influence of relaxing and self-selected stimulating music on vertical jump performance in male volleyball players. Int J Exerc Sci. (2022) 15(6):15. doi: 10.70252/pfnc1124
42. Eliakim M, Meckel Y, Nemet D, Eliakim A. The effect of music during warm-up on consecutive anaerobic performance in elite adolescent volleyball players. Int J Sports Med. (2007) 28(04):321–5. doi: 10.1055/s-2006-924360
43. Lee S, Kimmerly D. Influence of music on maximal self-paced running performance and passive post-exercise recovery rate. J Sports Med Phys Fitness. (2014) 56(1-2):39–48.
44. Smirmaul B, Dos Santos R, Da Silva Neto L. Pre-task music improves swimming performance. J Sports Med Phys Fitness. (2014) 55(12):1445–51.25303170
45. Bentouati E, Romdhani M, Khemila S, Chtourou H, Souissi N. The effects of listening to non-preferred or self-selected music during short-term maximal exercise at varied times of day. Percept Mot Skills. (2023) 130(1):539–54. doi: 10.1177/00315125221142662
46. Kostrna JR, Bigliassi M. Effects of noise-cancelling headphones on music experience during aerobic exercise. J Sports Sci. (2023) 41(14):1393–9. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2023.2273087
47. Pieper K, Spang RP, Prietz P, Möller S, Paajanen E, Vaalgamaa M, et al. Working with environmental noise and noise-cancelation: a workload assessment with eeg and subjective measures. Front Neurosci. (2021) 15:771533. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2021.771533
48. Karageorghis CI, Cheek P, Simpson SD, Bigliassi M. Interactive effects of music tempi and intensities on grip strength and subjective affect. Scand J Med Sci Sports. (2018) 28(3):1166–75. doi: 10.1111/sms.12979
49. Edworthy J, Waring H. The effects of music tempo and loudness level on treadmill exercise. Ergonomics. (2006) 49(15):1597–610. doi: 10.1080/00140130600899104
50. Ouergui I, Jebabli A, Messaoudi H, Delleli S, Chtourou H, Bouassida A, et al. The effects of tempo and loudness variations during warm-up with music on perceived exertion, physical enjoyment and specific performances in male and female taekwondo athletes. PLoS One. (2023) 18(4):e0284720. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0284720
Keywords: ergogenic aids, loads, performance, load tolerance monitoring, music
Citation: Bezuglov E, Vakhidov T, Kapralova E, Malyakin G, Vinogradov M, Chyogin S and Butovskiy M (2025) Music as an ergogenic aid in team sports: a systematic review. Front. Sports Act. Living 7:1514756. doi: 10.3389/fspor.2025.1514756
Received: 21 October 2024; Accepted: 16 April 2025;
Published: 13 May 2025.
Edited by:
Rubén Maneiro, University of Vigo, SpainReviewed by:
Miguel-Angel Gomez-Ruano, Universidad Politécnica de Madrid, SpainAnderson Meireles, Juiz de Fora Federal University, Brazil
Copyright: © 2025 Bezuglov, Vakhidov, Kapralova, Malyakin, Vinogradov, Chyogin and Butovskiy. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Elizaveta Kapralova, a2FwcmFsb3ZhZWxpekBnbWFpbC5jb20=