- 1The Faculty of Data and Decision Sciences, Technion - Israel Institute of Technology, Haifa, Israel
- 2The Centre for Human-Inspired Artificial Intelligence (CHIA), University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom
- 3Baruch Ivcher School of Psychology, Reichman University, Herzliya, Israel
- 4Cognitive Science Department, Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Jerusalem, Israel
- 5Rappaport Faculty of Medicine, Technion - Israel Institute of Technology, Haifa, Israel
Background: Recent advancements in Artificial Intelligence (AI) contributed significantly to suicide assessment, however, our theoretical understanding of this complex behavior is still limited.
Objective: This study aimed to harness AI methodologies to uncover hidden risk factors that trigger or aggravate suicide behaviors.
Methods: The primary dataset included 228,052 Facebook postings by 1,006 users who completed the gold-standard Columbia Suicide Severity Rating Scale. This dataset was analyzed using a bottom-up research pipeline without a-priory hypotheses and its findings were validated using a top-down analysis of a new dataset. This secondary dataset included responses by 1,062 participants to the same suicide scale as well as to well-validated scales measuring depression and boredom.
Results: An almost fully automated, AI-guided research pipeline resulted in four Facebook topics that predicted the risk of suicide, of which the strongest predictor was boredom. A comprehensive literature review using APA PsycInfo revealed that boredom is rarely perceived as a unique risk factor of suicide. A complementing top-down path analysis of the secondary dataset uncovered an indirect relationship between boredom and suicide, which was mediated by depression. An equivalent mediated relationship was observed in the primary Facebook dataset as well. However, here, a direct relationship between boredom and suicide risk was also observed.
Conclusion: Integrating AI methods allowed the discovery of an under-researched risk factor of suicide. The study signals boredom as a maladaptive ‘ingredient’ that might trigger suicide behaviors, regardless of depression. Further studies are recommended to direct clinicians’ attention to this burdening, and sometimes existential experience.
Introduction
Suicide, one of the major public-health concerns today following the COVID-19 crisis (1–3), is a highly complex human tragedy (4). Unfortunately, despite decades of research, our understanding of this somewhat enigmatic phenomenon is unsatisfying, as implied in the well-cited meta-analysis by Franklin et al. (5) According to this wide-scope meta-analysis, “the [suicide] field has primarily focused on the same risk factors for the past 50 years, with risk factors becoming increasingly homogenous over past five decades”. Nevertheless, a significant change may occur today, with the sweeping revolution in Artificial Intelligence (AI) and the ubiquitous spread of social media (6).
The recent introduction of strong Large Language Models (LLMs), such as GPT-3 (7), allowed researchers to analyze large amounts of authentic and valuable personal texts, which became highly accessible with the growing popularity of the various social networking sites (e.g., Facebook, Twitter). LLMs are capable of capturing subtle and meaningful patterns in raw data (e.g., posts or tweets) and previous research had already demonstrated their promising potential for suicide predictions (8). To date, dozens of AI studies reported of high quality predictions of suicide risk from social media, using purely bottom-up methods, which did not involve the more traditional, top-down examination of pre-defined hypotheses. The few that do include top-down examination require pre-defined hypotheses (9), and in that manner, reduce our ability to discover completely novel risk factors. Notably, these AI studies often achieved superior predictions than the top-down, theory-driven studies (10–12), however, to our knowledge, these AI-based predictions were never translated into actual theoretical advancements in suicidology.
In the absence of pre-defined hypotheses about specific risk factors and the opaque nature of the LLMs, which are often referred to as ‘black box’ models (13), it has been difficult to pinpoint the exact patterns or themes (i.e., risk factors) that drove these complex models to make their high quality predictions. In a way, the improved predictions generated by the LLMs came at a cost, as we now struggle to understand the internal mechanism of these models. As opposed to the top-down studies that typically examined a limited number of well-defined risk factors, the bottom-up AI studies analyzed numerous and implicit, data-driven features, thus restricting our ability to isolate the concrete psychosocial risks that might have been involved in the creation or maintenance of the suicide behaviors.
The overall goal of this research was to address this gap and harness the power of the LLMs for scientific discovery of risk factors. To exhaust the full potential of the LLMs, the first steps of the research pipeline were designed in a purely bottom-up manner, so that the results will not reflect upon predefined risk factors, but on authentic, data-driven, and perhaps less researched factors. Notably, the results from these almost fully automated steps indicated that the topic (i.e., theme) that contributed the most to the prediction of suicide addressed boredom experiences.
Boredom, or the “unfulfilled desire to be engaged in satisfying activity” as defined by Fahlman and collegues (14), is typically accompanied by mild negative emotions, low arousal and attentiveness, and decreased sense of meaning in life (15). This negative experience of boredom is even considered as a significant risk factor, or an inherent component, of depression (16, 17). However, to our knowledge, the role of boredom in the emergence and maintenance of suicide ideation and behaviors has not been characterized in the literature. In fact, following the ‘bottom-up’ discovery of boredom, we conducted a literature review using APA PsycInfo, the abstracting and indexing database of the American Psychological Association, and found that boredom is rarely perceived as a unique risk factor of suicide (for more information, see the Discussion section) (18–21).
We therefore completed the research pipeline with a final step that consisted of the collection of a new dataset. Using this new dataset, we could now examine a pre-defined hypothesis (that emerged from the previous bottom-up steps) that boredom experiences will predict suicide risk, either directly or indirectly, through the mediating variable of depression. In this way, we could further characterize the role of boredom in suicide behavior and illustrate how the new LLMs can be leveraged, not only for practical prediction of suicide risk, but also for theoretical advancements in suicidology (Discussion).
Methods
Data
The collection of the data was conducted with the ethical approval of the Institutional Review Boards of the Hebrew University of Jerusalem and the Technion – Israel Institute of Technology. The complete description of the primary dataset of the current study is available in our previous publication that focused on prediction (rather than on understanding) of suicide (22). Briefly, upon reading and signing a consent form, participants recruited from Amazon’s Mechanical Turk (MTurk) completed common psychiatric and psychological questionnaires and gave a one-off authorization to download their Facebook activity up to 12 months prior to the research date. This activity was extracted to an encrypted data storage through a designated application that was developed for this purpose.
Altogether, the primary dataset consisted of 228,052 Facebook postings that were uploaded by 1,006 active Facebook users (23.26% male). The participants were English speaking residents of the US (mean age = 44.7, SD = 13.9). The quality of the participants’ responses was ensured via subtle measures and attention checks we developed (for further information about this primary dataset, see the Supplementary Material) (23).
Importantly, the collected postings were matched to the users’ responses to the well-researched Columbia Suicide Severity Rating Scale (CSSRS) (24), which was administered in the original study. The CSSRS is considered a ‘diagnostic tool of choice’, both in clinical settings and in empirical research (25, 26). Upon consultation with the principal developer of the CSSRS (Posner, personal written communication), we chose to administer the electronic, screening version of the scale, in light of the fact that the research examined participants from a crowdsourcing platform. This version has been demonstrated to have psychometric validity and prediction accuracies that are comparable to the original clinician version of the scale (27, 28).
The structure of the CSSRS contains two parts. In the first part (items 1-2), participants are asked to respond YES or NO to questions about passive suicide ideation. In the second part (items 3-6, which are only shown to the participants if the first part indicated they had thoughts of killing themselves), the participants are asked about active suicide ideation (i.e., suicidal thoughts with method, intent, or a specific plan) suicide behavior (i.e., real-life activities aimed at ending one’s life, such as collecting pills or obtaining a gun). In this study, the distribution of the participants’ scores was as follows: 64.01% received zero, 10.47% received 1, 12.36% received 2, 6.08% received 3, 2.99% received 4, 3.29% received 5, and 0.8% received 6.
Procedures (a 5-step research pipeline)
Based on this high-quality dataset, we designed a 5-step research pipeline (Figure 1). A complete description of all the methodological details of this pipeline is provided in the Supplementary Material. The description below, in the current section, provides a brief overview of the five consecutive procedures that comprised the research pipeline, including the key computational methods that were implemented in each step and the secondary dataset that was collected in the fifth step.
First, we used a state-of-the-art (yet still “black box”) LLM named SBERT (29) to assign vector representations to the Facebook postings. Second, we applied a clustering algorithm named HDBSCAN (30), with the goal of organizing the different posts (represented numerically) into groups that ideally capture meaningful ‘topics’ (i.e., themes). Third, we conducted a stepwise regression (31) and identified the topics that contributed the most to the prediction of suicide (i.e., the CSSRS scores). Fourth, we analyzed the thematic content of the resulted topics using three methods: manual inspection of representative postings, ‘consultation’ with ChatGPT (32), and content analysis using the well-established method of TF-IDF (Term Frequency – Inverse Document Frequency) (33).
Fifth, to validate and further examine the bottom-up results from the previous steps, which suggested that suicide risk is linked to boredom experiences (Results), a secondary new sample of 1,062 participants was collected (52% male, mean age = 44.7, SD = 13.9). In this secondary, questionnaire-based dataset, participants completed three well-validated psychological measurements: (1) the aforementioned suicide scale (24), (2) the 9-item Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-9) that is commonly used to measure depression (34–36), (considering its potential mediating role that was described in the Introduction), and (3) a well-researched measurement of boredom named the Multidimensional State Boredom Scale (MSBS) (14).
The descriptive information about the CSSRS is presented above. The PHQ-9 is a nine-item scale that targets the nine symptoms of depression that appear in the fifth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) (37). Following the DSM diagnostic criteria, a cut-off point for the presence of depression can be calculated when participants report of five or more symptoms that disturbed them in the past two weeks, for at least “more than half the days”. In addition, at least one of these symptoms should refer to either low interest or depressed mood, which are the core symptoms of the disorder (34). In the current secondary sample, a total of 130 users (12.24%) met this DSM-based cut-off point for depression.
The MSBS consists of 28 items designed to evaluate the state of boredom. An additional scale measuring the more general tendency of the person to feel bored (i.e., the Boredom Proneness Scale) (16) was also implemented in this secondary sample (see the Supplementary Material). However, the following analyses relate to the state boredom scale only to match the two additional scales of the study that evaluated the current/recent (state) depressive and suicidal experiences of the participants (i.e., the PHQ-9 and the CSSRS). The state boredom scale also reflected best the boredom-related expressions that were identified in the primary Facebook data.
The 28 items of the MSBS are classified into five theoretical dimensions: Disengagement, High arousal, Low arousal, Inattention, and Time perception. Of particular interest to our investigation is the central dimension of Disengagement. This dimension, which consists of the largest number of items (N = 10) and has the highest factor loading (0.97) includes items, such as: “Everything seems repetitive and routine to me”, “I feel bored”, and “I want to do something fun, but nothing appeals to me” (14). The mean scores of the participants on this dimension of Disengagement was 2.26 (SD = 1.56, range = 0 to 6).
Notably, this Disengagement dimension was found to be the closest dimension (out of the five potential dimensions of boredom) to the boredom expressions that were common in the primary Facebook data. Various similarity analyses, which are described in the Supplementary Material ensured this similarity formal concept of boredom, as manifested in this Disengagement dimension, and the informal, Facebook-based topic of boredom. Altogether, this boredom measure, alongside the two measurements of suicide and depression, were used to examine the hypothesis that was generated in the previous, bottom-up steps, regarding the direct and indirect links between boredom and suicide.
Results
The analysis of the data was conducted via Pingouin – a commonly used statistics package written in Python (38). The bottom-up analysis of the primary dataset from Facebook and the consecutive top-down analysis of both datasets are presented below.
Bottom-up, topic-based analysis
The stepwise regression model (step 3) of the entire set of topics (step 2) resulted in four topics that were significantly correlated with suicide risk []. Table 1 presents the regression scores for each one of these four topics. The first topic that related to suicide [β=0.144, t(1005)= 3.3, p<0.005] consisted of boredom-related manifestations, as can be seen in the TF-IDF analysis and the response by ChatGPT (Step 4, Figure 2; Supplementary Tables S1, S2).
Figure 2 Word clouds based on the TF-IDF scores of the words in the four suicide-related topics. The word clouds are ordered from left to right according to their prediction strengths (beta scores) of suicide risk. The size of the words in each cloud is proportional to their TF-IDF score. The 5 lemmatized words with the highest TF-IDF scores within each topic are presented beneath the clouds.
The second topic seemed to reflect the user’s wish for something of value to him or her [β=0.142, t(1005)= 3.24, p<0.005]. Notably, most of the posts in this topic (93.5%) included an attached link, photo, or video, describing the specific needs of the participants (e.g., food, clothes, pet animals). The third topic was more difficult to interpret, but it seemed to address the participants’ beliefs, or general view, about life and about their reality [β=0.133, t(1005)= 3.05, p<0.005]. Finally, the fourth topic [β=0.129, t(1005)= 2.97, p<0.005] addressed soft drugs and their legalization.
Top-down hypothesis testing
To validate the purely bottom-up finding regarding boredom, we calculated the correlations between the participants’ scores on the boredom and the suicide scales in the secondary dataset (Step 5). This calculation indicated that the Disengagement factor was moderately correlated with the CSSRS (r = .353, p< 0.001; Figure 3A), thus providing a first confirmation of the hypothesis that boredom is linked to suicide behaviors.
Figure 3 Path analyses and bivariate correlations of Boredom, Depression, and Suicide in the Facebook data and the questionnaire data. The left graph (A) presents bivariate correlations between the three variables of these analyses, while the right graph (B) displays the results of the path analyses. The inner dashed arrows in both graphs represent the complementary data, and the outer continues arrows represent the original Facebook data. The color-coding on the graphs represents the statistical significance of the relationships: green for p-values<.001, yellow for p-values<.05, and red for p-values >.05. A supplemented cosine-similarity analysis that aims to further characterize the differences between the results from the primary Facebook dataset and the secondary questionnaire dataset is provided in the Supplementary Material.
To further characterize this link, we used the secondary dataset to conduct a path analysis (39), which also considered depression, which has been linked before, both to suicide and to boredom (Introduction). As illustrated in Figure 3B, this analysis indicated that the direct path between boredom and suicide was not significant (β = -.08, SE=.048, p = .093), while the indirect path between boredom and suicide through the mediating variable of depression was significant and quite substantial (β = .508, SE = .048, p<.0001).
Finally, we replicated this path analysis using the original Facebook data and replaced the boredom questionnaire with the boredom topic (see also the Supplementary Material). This analysis resulted in two significant paths from boredom to suicide, an indirect path as documented in the previous analysis of the newly collected measures (β = .046, SE = .024, p = .002) as well as a direct path (β = .021, SE = .040, p<.001) that suggests that boredom itself can increase the risk for suicide (Figure 3). Moreover, in this dataset from Facebook, the bivariate correlation between the topic of boredom and suicide risk (r = 0.185, p< 0.001) was stronger than the correlation between boredom and depression (r = 0.078, p< 0.05). A similar pattern of results was evidenced when the proneness boredom scale was used (see the Supplementary Material).
Discussion
This study, which consisted of a designated, 5-step research pipeline, aimed to harness the powerful LLMs for scientific discovery of risk factors for suicide. The first, purely bottom-up steps of the pipeline indicated that the topic that contributed the most to the prediction of suicide risk addressed experiences of boredom. The fifth, top-down step, which involved pre-defined hypotheses, resulted in two potential paths that link boredom to suicide behaviors:
The analysis of the secondary questionnaire-based dataset suggested that the least severe construct of boredom predicts the more severe moderator of depression, which in turn, predicts suicide ideation and behaviors. The analysis of the primary Facebook data indicated that boredom is linked directly to the risk of suicide, thus implying that boredom might serve as a maladaptive ‘active ingredient’ that is capable of triggering suicide ideation regardless of depression. Specifically, this mediation analysis of the Facebook data, as well as the observed superiority of the correlation between boredom and suicide over the correlation between boredom and depression (Figure 3), imply that the role of boredom in the creation and maintenance of suicide behaviors may be more crucial than what we might have thought based on the scarce and mostly outdated literature on this subject.
This last conclusion is noteworthy considering the existing research on boredom. As mentioned in the introduction, following the almost fully automated emergence of the topic of boredom, we used APA PsycInfo to search for the terms boredom and depression in any possible search field. This search yielded 46 results, of which 24 consisted of empirical studies, and only four were relevant to the hypothesized link between boredom and suicide:
A 30-year old research presented two surveys among Canadian and French adolescents, which yielded multiple links between suicide ideation and a range of risk factors, such as drug use, somatic complaints, and other self-perceived health problems, which included also a self-reported item regarding “bored/has a boring life” (18). A second, relatively old study, among 127 students reported a correlation (r = 0.37) between suicide risk and tedium, but the term ‘tedium’ may not serve as a good reflection of boredom as the authors defined it as an “experience of physical, emotional, and mental exhaustion” (20). A third, more recent PhD dissertation among prison inmates reported of multiple risk factors that were associated with suicide risk, including one of the items in the Psychopathy Checklist-Revised scale (PCL-R), which addressed the inmate’s need for stimulation and his proneness to boredom (19). Finally, a fourth study that followed 31 depressed patients for a week, reported that suicide ideation was preceded by feelings of tension, sadness, previous suicide ideation, and boredom (21).
Together, the findings from this literature review, which indicated that boredom did not receive sufficient research attention in the context of suicide, and the findings from the current study emphasize the need to keep investigating the potential harmful role of boredom. It is noted that we do not argue that the current study provides a definite proof for a direct pathway between boredom and suicide. However, we strongly recommend that further research will aim to explore this direction, especially considering the ambiguity of the concept of boredom and its intertwined relationships with depression.
Despite the fact that all humans experience boredom from time to time (40, 41), the term ‘boredom’ and its distinctiveness from similar psychological constructs, such as anhedonia, apathy, and emptiness, are not consistently defined in the literature (42). Correspondingly, a variety of theories were proposed over the years to explain this experience and to track its etiology, ranging from attention-based and arousal paradigms (43) to existential and psychodynamic theories (44). Indeed, some evolutionary-oriented theories emphasized the potentially positive role of boredom, which could encourage people to seek change and engage in creative and meaningful activities (45, 46). However, in many cases, the experience of boredom seems to trigger negative emotions (47) and even psychopathologies (48), including mainly depression.
The developers of the most common boredom proneness scale reported that it demonstrated moderate-size correlations (0.47 ≤ r ≤ 0.52) with three validated measurements of depression (16). These strong correlations were expected, according to the authors, because boredom and depression share “overlapping symptomology” (p. 11). Similarly, the developers of the brief State Boredom Measure (SBM) reported that all of its eight items correlated positively with depression (0.19 ≤ r ≤ 0.59) (17). These psychometric data alongside the aforementioned theoretical statements about “overlapping symptomology” bring forth an unsettling discriminant validity issue whereby boredom is not clearly differentiated from depression (46).
Indeed, some theorists aimed to pinpoint internal psychological aspects that differentiate boredom tendencies from pure depression (49, 50). These aspects might include external ambivalence in which bored persons typically blame others for their failures rather than directing anger towards themselves as is common in depression. They may also be characterized by passive avoidance and tend to avoid work or evade responsibility rather than keep trying while failing to win the love and admiration of others as often is the case with depression (49). However, in practice, the phenomenological overlap between these two concepts of boredom and depression makes the research on the implications of boredom rather difficult. This discriminant validity problem may explain why the results from the path analysis of the questionnaire data were not identical to the results from the Facebook data. In the questionnaire data, it is possible that the concept of boredom was not differentiated sufficiently from the concept of depression, thus preventing us from isolating its direct impact on suicide.
Limitations
This study has several limitations. Firstly, the social media data of this study was extracted from relatively active users of Facebook (22), thus limiting the generalizability of its bottom-up findings. Users who suffer from depression (a close predictor of suicide), for example, might be less active, or hesitant to share their thoughts or emotions online (51), and therefore be less represented in this sample of Facebook users. Secondly, despite the similarity between the Facebook-based topic of boredom and the questionnaire-based concept of boredom (Figure S1), the two psychological constructs may not be identical. This characteristic may explain the different patterns of results, but it also limits our ability to provide a perfect confirmation of the bottom-up findings through the top-down analyses. Lastly, the observational nature of the two datasets used in this study limits our ability to propose causal oriented conclusions, thus emphasizing the importance of conducting further studies on this topic, preferably using longitudinal research designs.
Clinical implications
Considering these limitations, the findings of the current study should be interpreted with caution. Nevertheless, the purely bottom-up nature of the key finding of this study emphasizes the potential hazard in continuous, burdening experiences of boredom. Although boredom is a very common experience, its negative implications may be undervalued, both in the literature and in the clinical field. For some people, boredom may indeed feel like a relatively benign experience, or even a positive experience as it could lead to creative and novel ideas and activities, as well as meaningful positive change (45, 46). However, for many people, boredom is perceived as a fairly aversive experience (41), and our findings suggest that it might even lead a person to engage in dangerous suicidal behaviors.
The mechanism that might explain this hazard has not been the focus of the current study. However, based on the existing literature on boredom, one potential explanation may relate to the increased risk for self-harm behaviors in boredom experiences, which in turn, might evolve into actual suicidal behavior. In the well-cited series of studies on “the challenges of the disengaged mind”, a seemingly harmless and easy task of spending 6-15 minutes doing nothing was experienced by most participants as an inherently unpleasant mission, to the point that many participants (especially male participants) chose to give themselves an electric shock (52). In a way, for these individuals, an adverse stimulation was preferred to no stimulation at all.
Indeed, self-harm behaviors are typically understood as a (non-adaptive) method to regulate unwanted negative emotions (53, 54). However, there seems to be convincing evidence that self-harm behaviors can also result simply from the person’s wish to avoid tedious experiences and disrupt their burdening monotony. Several experiments on this topic, which included benign control groups as well as groups that were induced with negative emotions, found that the ‘active psychological ingredient’ that led participants to self-administer electric shocks was boredom (55–57). This ‘dark side’ of boredom has even led researchers to reconsider its role in the self-harming behaviors that characterize the common Borderline Personality Disorder (58).
A second explanation for the observed link between boredom and suicide may be the enmeshment between boredom and deeper existential experiences, such as emptiness (59) and lack of meaning in life (60), which are recognized as substantial risk factors of suicide (61). To our knowledge, these subjective, amorphic, and even mysterious human experiences typically do not receive sufficient space in the context of evidence-based treatments for suicide, such as interpersonal psychotherapy (IPT) (62) and Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy (CBT) (63). We therefore conclude that boredom should be further considered, both in theory as argued above, and in practice, during suicide prevention programs and therapies.
This conclusion is noteworthy considering the current state of the literature on suicide. Suicide research, as described in the introduction, typically revolves around the same risk factors (5), and this study demonstrated that the integration of AI methods could lead to the exposure of under-recognized risks, such as boredom. Boredom, as documented in our thorough review of the literature above, has rarely been investigated directly within the context of suicide, and the current AI-inspired research allowed us to identify it and further explore it using complementing top-down analysis. We therefore join previous calls to combine AI tools in suicide research (8, 64), in a careful and responsible manner, as these tools can improve our understanding of suicide, and perhaps direct us to craft more clinically tailored treatments to individuals at risk.
Data availability statement
The datasets presented in this article are not readily available because the Facebook postings are identifiable and cannot be shared with the scientific community. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to yaakovophir@gmail.com.
Ethics statement
The studies involving humans were approved by Ethic Committee of the Technion - Israel Institute of Technology. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author contributions
SL: Conceptualization, Investigation, Writing – review & editing, Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Visualization. YO: Conceptualization, Investigation, Funding acquisition, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. RT: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Validation, Writing – review & editing. AK: Conceptualization, Validation, Writing – review & editing. IS: Data curation, Validation, Writing – review & editing. EF: Investigation, Writing – review & editing. RR: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing.
Funding
The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. A grant for data science by VATAT, the Israeli higher education council was used to partially fund the scholarships of Shir Lissak.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Supplementary material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2024.1328122/full#supplementary-material
References
1. Yao H, Chen J-H, Xu Y-F. Patients with mental health disorders in the COVID-19 epidemic. Lancet Psychiatry. (2020) 7:e21. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(20)30090-0
2. Samji H, Wu J, Ladak A, Vossen C, Stewart E, Dove N, et al. Mental health impacts of the COVID-19 pandemic on children and youth–a systematic review. Child Adolesc Ment Health. (2022) 27(2):173–89. doi: 10.1111/camh.12501
3. Bilu Y, Flaks-Manov N, Bivas-Benita M, Akiva P, Kalkstein N, Yehezkelli Y, et al. Data-driven assessment of adolescents’ Mental health during the COVID-19 pandemic. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2023) 62(8):920–37. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2022.12.026
4. Levi-Belz Y, Gvion Y, Apter A. The psychology of suicide: from research understandings to intervention and treatment. Front Psychiatry. (2019) 10:214. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00214
5. Franklin JC, Ribeiro JD, Fox KR, Bentley KH, Kleiman EM, Huang X, et al. Risk factors for suicidal thoughts and behaviors: a meta-analysis of 50 years of research. psychol Bull. (2017) 143(2):187. doi: 10.1037/bul0000084
6. Ophir Y, Tikochinski R, Brunstein Klomek A, Reichart R. The hitchhiker’s guide to computational linguistics in suicide prevention. Clin psychol Sci. (2022) 10:212–35. doi: 10.1177/21677026211022013
7. Brown TB, Mann B, Ryder N, Subbiah M, Kaplan JD, Dhariwal P, et al. Language models are few-shot learners. Adv Neural Inf Process Syst. (2020) 33:1877–901.
8. Resnik P, Foreman A, Kuchuk M, Musacchio Schafer K, Pinkham B. Naturally occurring language as a source of evidence in suicide prevention. Suicide Life-Threatening Behav. (2021) 51(1):88–96. doi: 10.1111/sltb.12674
9. Badian Y, Ophir Y, Tikochinski R, Calderon N, Klomek AB, Fruchter E, et al. Social media images can predict suicide risk using interpretable large language-vision models. J Clin Psychiatry. (2023) 85(1):50516.
10. Bernert RA, Hilberg AM, Melia R, Kim JP, Shah NH, Abnousi F. Artificial intelligence and suicide prevention: a systematic review of machine learning investigations. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2020) 17:5929. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17165929
11. Heckler WF, de Carvalho JV, Barbosa JLV. Machine learning for suicidal ideation identification: A systematic literature review. Comput Hum Behav. (2022) 128:107095. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2021.107095
12. Schafer KM, Kennedy G, Gallyer A, Resnik P. A direct comparison of theory-driven and machine learning prediction of suicide: A meta-analysis. PloS One. (2021) 16:e0249833. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0249833
13. Alishahi A, Chrupała G, Linzen T. Analyzing and interpreting neural networks for NLP: A report on the first BlackboxNLP workshop. Natural Lang Eng. (2019) 25:543–57. doi: 10.1017/S135132491900024X
14. Fahlman SA, Mercer-Lynn KB, Flora DB, Eastwood JD. Development and validation of the multidimensional state boredom scale. Assessment. (2013) 20:68–85. doi: 10.1177/1073191111421303
15. Van Tilburg WAP, Igou ER. Boredom begs to differ: Differentiation from other negative emotions. Emotion. (2017) 17:309. doi: 10.1037/emo0000233
16. Farmer R, Sundberg ND. Boredom proneness–the development and correlates of a new scale. J Pers Assess. (1986) 50:4–17. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa5001_2
17. Todman M. The dimensions of state boredom: Frequency, duration, unpleasantness, consequences and causal attributions. Educ Res Int. (2013) 1:40.
18. Choquet M, Kovess V, Poutignat N. Suicidal thoughts among adolescents: an intercultural approach. Adolescence. (1993) 28:649.
19. Jimenez M. An analysis of neuropsychological functioning and psychopathic traits in correctional patients with histories of suicidal behavior. Palo Alto, California, US: Palo Alto University ProQuest Dissertations Publishing (2012).
20. Lester D. Tedium, depression, and suicidal preoccupation. psychol Rep. (1993) 73:622–2. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1993.73.2.622
21. Ben-Zeev D, Young MA, Depp CA. Real-time predictors of suicidal ideation: Mobile assessment of hospitalized depressed patients. Psychiatry Res. (2012) 197:55–9. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.11.025
22. Ophir Y, Tikochinski R, Asterhan CSC, Sisso I, Reichart R. Deep neural networks detect suicide risk from textual facebook posts. Sci Rep. (2020) 10:16685. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-73917-0
23. Ophir Y, Sisso I, Asterhan CSC, Tikochinski R, Reichart R. The turker blues: Hidden factors behind increased depression rates among Amazon’s Mechanical Turkers. Clin Psychol Sci. (2020) 8:65–83. doi: 10.1177/2167702619865973
24. Posner K, Brown GK, Stanley B, Brent DA, Yershova KV, Oquendo MA, et al. The Columbia–Suicide Severity Rating Scale: initial validity and internal consistency findings from three multisite studies with adolescents and adults. Am J Psychiatry. (2011) 168(12):1266–77. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.10111704
25. Drapeau CW, Nadorff MR, McCall WV, Titus CE, Barclay N, Payne A. Screening for suicide risk in adult sleep patients. Sleep Med Rev. (2019) 46:17–26. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2019.03.009
26. Weber AN, Michail M, Thompson A, Fiedorowicz JG. Psychiatric emergencies: assessing and managing suicidal ideation. Med Clinics. (2017) 101:553–71. doi: 10.1016/j.mcna.2016.12.006
27. Mundt JC, Greist JH, Gelenberg AJ, Katzelnick DJ, Jefferson JW, Modell JG. Feasibility and validation of a computer-automated Columbia-Suicide Severity Rating Scale using interactive voice response technology. J Psychiatr Res. (2010) 44:1224–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.04.025
28. Viguera AC, Milano N, Laurel R, Thompson NR, Griffith SD, Baldessarini RJ, et al. Comparison of electronic screening for suicidal risk with the Patient Health Questionnaire Item 9 and the Columbia Suicide Severity Rating Scale in an outpatient psychiatric clinic. Psychosomatics. (2015) 56(5):460–9. doi: 10.1016/j.psym.2015.04.005
29. Reimers N, Gurevych I. Sentence-bert: Sentence embeddings using siamese bert-networks. arXiv. (2019) arXiv:1908.10084. Available at: https://arxiv.org/abs/1908.10084.
30. McInnes L, Healy J, Melville J. Umap: Uniform manifold approximation and projection for dimension reduction. arXiv. (2018) arXiv:1802.03426. Available at: https://arxiv.org/abs/1802.03426.
31. Hocking RR. A Biometrics invited paper. The analysis and selection of variables in linear regression. Biometrics. (1976) 32(1):1–49. doi: 10.2307/2529336
32. OpenAI. ChatGPT (September 24 version) [Large language model] (2023). Available online at: https://chat.openai.com.
33. Mogotsi IC, Christopher D. Manning, Prabhakar Raghavan, and Hinrich Schütze: Introduction to information retrieval. Inf Retrieval. (2010) 13:192–5. doi: 10.1007/s10791-009-9115-y
34. Spitzer RL, Kroenke K, Williams JBW. Validation and utility of a self-report version of PRIME-MD: the PHQ primary care study. Jama. (1999) 282:1737–44. doi: 10.1001/jama.282.18.1737
35. El-Den S, Chen TF, Gan Y-L, Wong E, O’Reilly CL. The psychometric properties of depression screening tools in primary healthcare settings: A systematic review. J Affect Disord. (2018) 225:503–22. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.08.060
36. Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JBW. The PHQ-9: validity of a brief depression severity measure. J Gen Internal Med. (2001) 16:606–13. doi: 10.1046/j.1525-1497.2001.016009606.x
37. APA. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5®). Washington, D.C., US.: American Psychiatric Association (2013). doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596
38. Vallat R. Pingouin: statistics in python. J Open Source Softw. (2018) 3:1026. doi: 10.21105/joss.01026
40. Finkielsztein M. The significance of boredom: A literature review. J Boredom Stud. (2023) 1:1–33.
41. Westgate EC. Why boredom is interesting. Curr Dir psychol Sci. (2020) 29:33–40. doi: 10.1177/0963721419884309
42. Goldberg YK, Eastwood JD, LaGuardia J, Danckert J. Boredom: An emotional experience distinct from apathy, anhedonia, or depression. J Soc Clin Psychol. (2011) 30:647. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2011.30.6.647
43. Zuckerman M. Beyond the optimal level of arousal. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbraum Associates (1979).
44. Wangh M. Boredom in psychoanalytic perspective. Soc Res. (1975) 1975:538–50. Available at: http://www.jstor.org/stable/41582848.
45. Bench SW, Lench HC. Boredom as a seeking state: Boredom prompts the pursuit of novel (even negative) experiences. Emotio. (2019) 19:242. doi: 10.1037/emo0000433
46. Westgate EC, Steidle B. Lost by definition: Why boredom matters for psychology and society. Soc Pers Psychol Compass. (2020) 14:e12562. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12562
47. Chin A, Markey A, Bhargava S, Kassam KS, Loewenstein G. Bored in the USA: Experience sampling and boredom in everyday life. Emotion. (2017) 17:359. doi: 10.1037/emo0000232
48. Weiss ER, Todman M, Maple E, Bunn RR. Boredom in a time of uncertainty: state and trait boredom’s associations with psychological health during COVID-19. Behav Sci. (2022) 12(8):298. doi: 10.3390/bs12080298
49. Bargdill RW. Habitual boredom and depression: Some qualitative differences. J Humanistic Psychol. (2019) 59:294–312. doi: 10.1177/0022167816637948
50. Bargdill R. The study of life boredom. J Phenomenological Psychol. (2000) 31:188–219. doi: 10.1163/15691620051090979
51. Ophir Y, Asterhan CSC, Schwarz BB. The digital footprints of adolescent depression, social rejection and victimization of bullying on Facebook. Comput Hum Behav. (2019) 91:62–71. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2018.09.025
52. Wilson TD, Reinhard DA, Westgate EC, Gilbert DT, Ellerbeck N, Hahn C, et al. Just think: The challenges of the disengaged mind. Science. (2014) 345(6192):75–7. doi: 10.1126/science.1250830
53. Chapman AL, Gratz KL, Brown MZ. Solving the puzzle of deliberate self-harm: The experiential avoidance model. Behav Res Ther. (2006) 44:371–94. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2005.03.005
54. Klonsky ED. The functions of deliberate self-injury: A review of the evidence. Clin Psychol Rev. (2007) 27:226–39. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.08.002
55. Weingarten N, Römer S, Klein S, Marszalek K, van der Meer I, Nijensteen M, et al. The effect of frustration and boredom on self-harming behaviour. Maastricht Student J Psychol Neurosci. (2016) 5:66–82.
56. Nederkoorn C, Vancleef L, Wilkenhöner A, Claes L, Havermans RC. Self-inflicted pain out of boredom. Psychiatry Res. (2016) 237:127–32. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.01.063
57. Yusoufzai MK, Vancleef L, Lobbestael J, Nederkoorn C. Painfully bored: the role of negative urgency and history of Non-Suicidal Self-Injury in Self-Administering painful stimuli. Motivation Emotion. (2022) 46:689–701. doi: 10.1007/s11031-022-09970-1
58. Masland SR, Shah TV, Choi-Kain LW. Boredom in borderline personality disorder: A lost criterion reconsidered. Psychopathology. (2020) 53:239–53. doi: 10.1159/000511312
59. D’Agostino A, Pepi R, Monti MR, Starcevic V. The feeling of emptiness: a review of a complex subjective experience. Harvard Rev Psychiatry. (2020) 28:287–95. doi: 10.1097/HRP.0000000000000269
60. King LA, Hicks JA. The science of meaning in life. Annu Rev Psychol. (2021) 72:561–84. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-072420-122921
61. Kleiman EM, Beaver JK. A meaningful life is worth living: Meaning in life as a suicide resiliency factor. Psychiatry Res. (2013) 210:934–9. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.08.002
62. Markowitz JC, Weissman MM. Interpersonal psychotherapy: principles and applications. World Psychiatry. (2004) 3:136.
63. Stanley B, Brown G, Brent DA, Wells K, Poling K, Curry J, et al. Cognitive-behavioral therapy for suicide prevention (CBT-SP): treatment model, feasibility, and acceptability. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2009) 48(10):1005–13. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181b5dbfe
Keywords: boredom, social media, large language models, natural language processing, deep learning, risk factors discovery, suicide research, suicide prevention
Citation: Lissak S, Ophir Y, Tikochinski R, Brunstein Klomek A, Sisso I, Fruchter E and Reichart R (2024) Bored to death: Artificial Intelligence research reveals the role of boredom in suicide behavior. Front. Psychiatry 15:1328122. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2024.1328122
Received: 26 October 2023; Accepted: 18 April 2024;
Published: 03 May 2024.
Edited by:
Yossi Levi-Belz, Ruppin Academic Center, IsraelReviewed by:
James Danckert, University of Waterloo, CanadaMariusz Stanisław Wiglusz, Medical University of Gdansk, Poland
Guanyu Cui, Wenzhou University, China
Copyright © 2024 Lissak, Ophir, Tikochinski, Brunstein Klomek, Sisso, Fruchter and Reichart. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Yaakov Ophir, yaakovophir@gmail.com
†These authors have contributed equally to this work