Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Psychol., 14 January 2021
Sec. Personality and Social Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Advances and Obstacles in Contemporary Nonverbal Communication Research View all 18 articles

Expression Authenticity: The Role of Genuine and Deliberate Displays in Emotion Perception

  • 1Department of Criminology and Sociology, Kingston University London, Kingston, United Kingdom
  • 2Department of Psychology, Kingston University London, Kingston, United Kingdom
  • 3Department of Experimental Psychology, University College London, London, United Kingdom

People dedicate significant attention to others’ facial expressions and to deciphering their meaning. Hence, knowing whether such expressions are genuine or deliberate is important. Early research proposed that authenticity could be discerned based on reliable facial muscle activations unique to genuine emotional experiences that are impossible to produce voluntarily. With an increasing body of research, such claims may no longer hold up to empirical scrutiny. In this article, expression authenticity is considered within the context of senders’ ability to produce convincing facial displays that resemble genuine affect and human decoders’ judgments of expression authenticity. This includes a discussion of spontaneous vs. posed expressions, as well as appearance- vs. elicitation-based approaches for defining emotion recognition accuracy. We further expand on the functional role of facial displays as neurophysiological states and communicative signals, thereby drawing upon the encoding-decoding and affect-induction perspectives of emotion expressions. Theoretical and methodological issues are addressed with the aim to instigate greater conceptual and operational clarity in future investigations of expression authenticity.

Introduction

The accurate recognition of emotions plays a crucial role in communication and social interaction. Knowing what another person is feeling is relevant for predicting their psychological state, likely future behavior, and interaction outcome (Hall et al., 2009). However, the advantage of such knowledge hinges on the emotional displays matching the person’s true underlying affect.

Humans have great control over their facial behavior (Zuckerman et al., 1986; Smith, 2004), with the ability to produce complex expressions. This implies that not all displays genuinely reflect a person’s underlying emotional state (Barrett, 2006). Deliberate expressions reflect the strategic intent of the individual in the absence of felt emotions (Ekman and Rosenberg, 2005). During social interaction, individuals may consciously regulate and suppress their emotions and portray expressions of unfelt emotions. This raises the issue of expression authenticity.

While people seem capable of recognizing emotions from specific facial configurations with high accuracy (Calvo and Nummenmaa, 2015),1 the ability to distinguish the authenticity of such expressions is much poorer (Ekman and O’Sullivan, 1991; Hess and Kleck, 1994; McLellan et al., 2010). Interestingly, the reason(s) for this has not been fully determined yet, with difficulties in explanations partly stemming from disagreements about the nature of emotions and the function of facial expressions. Recent propositions have attempted to elucidate some of the inconsistencies of past research, considering facial expressions as both innate cues and communicative signals (Crivelli and Fridlund, 2018; Barrett et al., 2019). Here, we build on this work, thereby focusing on human expression authenticity judgments: assessing whether the emotional expression displayed by another person is a genuine reflection of their underlying affect. This operational definition is representative of the task participants typically perform and the instructions they receive; however, as will be discussed, how one conceptualizes emotions and operationalizes facial expressions will ultimately determine what an authenticity judgment indicates and the inferences that can be drawn from it.2

The Function of Facial Expressions: Encoding-Decoding vs. Affect-Induction

Conceptually, there are two main perspectives regarding the function of facial expressions. These view facial displays either as (a) innate cues reflecting genuine affect or (b) communicative signals of affect and intent.

According to the encoding-decoding perspective (Ekman, 2003), observers (called decoders) “decode” the meaning behind emotional displays of others (called senders). Facial expressions are considered to be innate, neurologically activated, fixed facial muscle patterns that occur in response to emotion-eliciting stimuli (Tomkins, 1962). Their appearance is an evolved response to specific events that are difficult (if not impossible) to suppress (Hurley and Frank, 2011), resulting in facial leakage (i.e., involuntary displays of felt emotions; Ekman, 1997). As such, they are functionally not a source of emotional information but became so as an exaptation (Darwin, 1872). Under this account, voluntary modulations of expressions come in the form of display rules, which are socio-cultural norms regulating the expression of displays (Ekman and Friesen, 1971). For expression authenticity, this perspective places emphasis on presumed reliable muscles, which are facial markers said to activate only during felt affect and being impossible to voluntarily control (Ekman, 2003). Under this view, differences between genuine and deliberate displays exist, and expression authenticity is a function of the decoder’s perceptual ability and knowledge for making accurate inferences. While being popular, this view has been criticized due to its vague conceptualization and lack of empirical support (Barrett et al., 2019).

According to the affect-induction perspective (Crivelli and Fridlund, 2018, 2019),3 facial displays function as a signaling mechanism to communicate one’s emotional states, motivating corresponding states in the observer (called receivers). Evolutionary there is no reason why facial expressions and emotion perception could not have co-evolved as part of a social signaling system (Izard, 1994; Dezecache et al., 2013). Indeed, a growing body of evidence suggests that humans are adept at producing facial expressions for communicative reasons (Smith, 2004). Under this view, the function of emotional displays is to signal emotional information and intent (i.e., they are not cryptic “cues” needed to be decoded; Crivelli and Fridlund, 2019). This perspective is not without its limitations. For instance, there are clear examples of behavior, such as blushing (Crozier, 2010), which can be used to infer the emotional states the sender may wish to suppress but is unable to do so. Also, the perspective does not adequately account for emotional leakage or solitary reactions (e.g., smiling when alone; but see Crivelli and Fridlund, 2018).

When synthesized both perspectives are useful for understanding human expression authenticity judgments. For instance, the encoding-decoding perspective provides the foundation for considering genuine (i.e., innate, involuntary responses) and deliberate (i.e., voluntary, communicative signals) expressions. It is important to note, though, that the argument for clear differences in expression authenticity (Ekman et al., 1988) is neither consistent with empirical investigations (Barrett et al., 2019) nor reflected in human judgments of facial expressions (Zloteanu et al., 2020, 2018). First, senders seem to possess the ability to produce genuine-looking displays of emotion (Surakka and Hietanen, 1998; Gosselin et al., 2010; Gunnery et al., 2013). Second, when considering facial expressions as social signals, as done in the affect-induction perspective, it is possible to understand why expression authenticity judgments are relatively poor. In deceptive scenarios, deliberate emotional cues serve an obvious communicative purpose: they convey an affective state to an observer which strategically benefits the sender (maliciously or otherwise).

Spontaneous vs. Posed: A Sufficiently Nuanced Dichotomy?

Irrespective of the perspective adopted, researchers typically employ an experimental design that separates facial expressions into spontaneous and posed. These are generated in various ways, ranging from emotion-induced exemplars to directed facial muscle activations (Coan and Allen, 2007; Quigley et al., 2013; Siedlecka and Denson, 2019). The conceptualization, however, has been criticized for not reflecting the nuances in expression elicitation (Shackman and Wager, 2019).

Proponents of the encoding-decoding perspective treat spontaneous and posed displays as categorical, with “spontaneous” reflecting felt emotional displays and “posed” reflecting unfelt deliberate displays. The origin of this dichotomy stems from neuroanatomical research alleging separate neural pathways for the production of involuntary and voluntary facial expressions (Rinn, 1984). The two systems are argued to produce visible differences in facial muscle activation, intensity, facial symmetry, and dynamics (Ekman, 2003). Yet, these have been challenged in recent work. For example, research finds no strong support for reliable muscles in either laboratory (Krumhuber and Manstead, 2009) or naturalistic studies (Fernández-Dols and Crivelli, 2013). Also, intensity relates more to the production method than to veracity (Zloteanu et al., 2020, 2018; Miller et al., 2020), and differences in the dynamic components are found to be subtle and varied between emotions (Cohn and Schmidt, 2004; Namba et al., 2016).

Proponents of the affect-induction perspective treat spontaneous and posed as one dimension of emotional displays. The use of actors, for instance, has been proposed to be a valid approach for studying expression authenticity (Gur et al., 2002). Proponents of actor portrayals argue that unmodulated and authentic expressions absent of socio-cultural influence are rare and difficult (if not impossible) to elicit under laboratory conditions (Scherer and Bänziger, 2010). The use of actors permits the creation of well-controlled, reliable, and recognizable displays to investigate the “shared code of emotional signalling” (Scherer and Bänziger, 2010, p. 166); although, the specific acting technique may play a similarly important role (Orlowska et al., 2018; Krumhuber et al., 2020). Nonetheless, such research has often been criticized due to the intentional communicative nature of portrayals, arguing that the reliance on actors for both spontaneous and posed displays invalidates the concept of authenticity (Cowie et al., 2005; Sauter and Fischer, 2018). We conjecture that the use of actors raises an interesting theoretical point. If actors can reproduce elements of genuine, spontaneous, felt displays (e.g., Carroll and Russell, 1997), it calls into question whether authenticity discrimination as a perceptual ability is possible per se.

Ultimately, terms such as “genuine” and “spontaneous” should be treated with caution as—theoretically and methodologically—they are debatable concepts. While some researchers treat them as synonyms, others consider them as different dimensions (i.e., genuine-deceptive and spontaneous-posed). For encoding-decoding scholars, genuine and spontaneous reflect similar concepts, namely, the absence of modulation and intentionality in the emotional display (Ekman, 2003). Yet, for affect-induction scholars the genuine-deceptive dimension reflects the intent of the sender (Crivelli and Fridlund, 2018), while spontaneous/posed are labels given to displays with specific facial characteristics. It is important to note that emotional congruence and sender control are complex issues. For instance, an expression may match the person’s emotional state but be deliberately produced, such as exaggerated displays (e.g., laughing more strongly in the presence of others; Fridlund, 1991). Based on emotional congruence this would be considered as genuine (and potentially even spontaneous); yet, based on control it can be labeled as deceptive (and posed). Careful considerations should also be given to the type of expression as there are many ways of eliciting either spontaneous or posed displays (Zloteanu et al., 2018, 2020; Krumhuber et al., 2019).

How Do We Measure “Accuracy”: Appearance-Based or Elicitation-Based?

Emotional experiences are often difficult to measure, with some scholars even arguing that they are empirically unverifiable (i.e., we can never truly know what someone is experiencing). Most investigations rely on proxies such as self-report or bodily measures (for recent commentaries see Barrett et al., 2019; Crivelli and Fridlund, 2019). This begs the question: what do we mean by “accurate emotion recognition?”

A review of the literature reveals multiple processes with similar yet not equivalent terms and definitions, such as emotion identification, categorization, discrimination, inference, and recognition. These are used interchangeably or separately, and sometimes the same term has different definitions (see Gonçalves et al., 2018), making it difficult to know if two scholars pertain to the same phenomenon. For instance, in our research (Zloteanu et al., 2020, 2018) we define emotion classification accuracy as the ability to infer specific emotions from facial displays, and emotion authenticity discrimination as the ability to differentiate between spontaneous (genuine) and posed (deliberate) displays. By contrast, Buck et al. (2017) use the exact opposite definitions which they label emotion categorization ability and emotion communication accuracy. Such interchangeable use in terminology may lead to confusion or misleading conclusions and interfere with attempts to synthesize research (see Fiske, 2020). This is a symptom of a larger issue within psychology relating to the use of operational definitions to explain phenomena (see Lilienfeld et al., 2015).

Much of the expression authenticity research has employed an appearance-based approach, thereby focusing on stimulus features, such as the Duchenne marker for the distinction between genuine and deliberate smiles. Appearance-based approaches make strong assumptions for the presence/absence of specific facial markers and dynamic features (Ekman, 2003) and impose constraints as to which exemplars are representative of authenticity (thereby excluding facial responses if they fail to meet relevant criteria). Under this approach, judges engage in a categorization task that prompts them to classify facial exemplars based on pre-selected criteria (e.g., Ekman et al., 1983). While such procedure allows for clear and reliable assessments, it may not be sufficient for measuring expression authenticity, as investigations can be conducted with stimuli produced to “look” authentic even though the sender did not experience genuine affect (as often the case with actor portrayals; Scherer and Bänziger, 2010). As such, it only assesses the perceptual or categorization ability of the observer.

The elicitation-based pathway is an alternative approach that places the focus on the methods used to produce facial expressions. Here, expression authenticity is operationalized on the basis of sender veracity (or intent), where genuine expressions reflect responses to an emotion-evoking event and non-genuine expressions are voluntarily produced displays in the absence of such an event. It makes no assumptions as to what constitutes a veridical emotional display and merely refers to the congruence between the eliciting event and the external behavior. Natural variations in displays between senders are considered relevant for the judgment process by decoders. Exemplars are selected based on the elicitation technique, allowing researchers to explore how differently produced displays affect people’s judgments. Under this approach, labels such as “genuine” and “deliberate” apply only to the inferences made by judges. While elicitation-based approaches introduce more variability in judgments, they capture the diversity of facial displays and mirror the emotional inferences made in real life. This is in stark contrast to the appearance-based approach in which facial exemplars must adhere to a strict morphological or dynamic criterion regardless of the production method being used.

Both approaches have merits yet answer different questions. The appearance-based approach permits investigations of universal representations of expressions (i.e., prototypical displays), in decoders’ ability to detect specific facial configurations, and how alterations of such patterns impact perception and judgment. The elicitation-based approach permits investigations of the variability in human responses to emotional events, how such behaviors are affected by context or experimental manipulation, and how people infer meaning from such displays. Noteworthy, measures of accuracy have a different meaning under the two approaches. According to the latter, judgment accuracy is more akin to congruency (as in Dawel et al., 2017), where a judgment is correct if the expression is judged as “genuine” and the sender was experiencing an emotion. By contrast, appearance-based accuracy reflects the correct identification and grouping of expressions with similar facial patterns irrespective of the sender’s affective experience.

Recommendations for Future Research

For future investigations of expression authenticity, we recommend the use of advanced statistical analyses, such as Bayesian mixed-effects models, to account for individual differences in senders and judges as sources for variability (see Sorensen et al., 2016). For example, a study comparing genuine and deliberate expressions may find no overall difference in genuineness ratings, yet inspection of the stimuli reveals that some expressions in the deliberate condition were rated overly genuine, thereby influencing the aggregate score. Omitting those expressions as “bad” exemplars may be unjustifiable as one would need to assume the existence of “good” exemplars; instead, the respective senders may have just been excellent actors who produced convincing portrayals. In a similar vein, some observers may systematically underrate genuine expressions, minimizing potential differences between conditions. Using mixed-effects models such variability can easily be accounted for without the need to remove data or make assumptions regarding its validity, thereby allowing for more robust analyses (Brysbaert and Stevens, 2018).

Separate from the sender-judge variability within and between studies, considerations should be given to biases in authenticity judgments. For sender-specific biases, the demeanor bias – the finding that some senders produce general impressions of (dis)honesty irrespective of their veracity (Zuckerman et al., 1981; Levine et al., 2011) – plays an important role. A person’s demeanor may result in their display being judged as non-genuine, irrespective of appearance, or intent. Merely examining facial features (i.e., Duchenne marker) will not reveal such perceptual biases toward particular senders. For judge-specific biases, response tendencies such as the truth-bias may impact expression judgments. Overestimating others’ truthfulness results in inflated accuracy scores which do not reflect true detection ability but a response preference (see Zuckerman et al., 1981). People may be biased toward disproportionally assuming that facial expressions are genuine (i.e., authenticity bias; Gosselin et al., 1995; Zloteanu, 2020). Hence, it is crucial to separate response biases from signal detection when measuring accuracy (Stanislaw and Todorov, 1999).

Future research should also embrace the wide range in which facial expressions occur. Studies concerned with authenticity typically employ one set of spontaneous and posed stimuli, pre-selected from many exemplars and based on specific criteria (see Krumhuber et al., 2017). Rarely do investigations target multiple types of displays (e.g., Soppe, 1988). Given that “spontaneous” and “posed” serve as umbrella terms (see Sauter and Fischer, 2018; Siedlecka and Denson, 2019), judgments under one operationalization may not generalize to another, and aggregating findings will result in incorrect and misleading inferences. In Zloteanu et al. (2018, 2020), we illustrated how producing deliberate expressions using different methods results in judgment differences for each expression type. Under a classical one genuine vs. one deliberate design, these results would not be easily interpretable as each comparison produces different insights into expression authenticity judgments.

Finally, it would be desirable to aim for greater transparency and consistency in the use of operational definitions, urging researchers to be explicit, comprehensive, and transparent in their methodology. While some scholars may be aware of the nuances and shortcomings of specific terminology (Barrett et al., 2019), over-labeling measures and phenomena increase the risk of confusion within and across a domain (Lilienfeld et al., 2015). Labels should serve as mere conveniences for scientific communication, but do not represent unchallengeable and unfalsifiable constructs. Given that emotion scholars still debate the exact definition of emotions (Ortony and Turner, 1990; Izard, 2007; Kagan, 2007), their taxonomy (Fiske, 2020), and whether they are discrete (Siegel et al., 2018) and universal (Barrett, 2006), it may be premature to taut certainty in a field debating the fundamentals.

Conclusion

Deciphering what another person is feeling is a complex task. Here, we address the role of facial expressions as innate cues and communicative signals, proposing a shift from accuracy measures to judgments in expression authenticity. This includes a comparison of encoding-decoding and affective-induction perspectives to offer insights into the process of emotion expression recognition. We conceive of senders as strategic performers who utilize their full expressive capabilities in social interaction and judges as attempting to infer meaning and intent from emotional displays. To help accelerate progress in the field we encourage researchers to carefully consider theory and methodology in how they operationalize facial expressions.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Author Contributions

MZ: conceptualization. MZ and EK: writing and revision. Both authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

We acknowledge that the publication fees for this article were fully paid by Kingston University London.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Footnotes

  1. ^ High accuracy rates may also result from prototypical and posed expressions typically being used in research (see Krumhuber et al., 2019, 2020; Dupré et al., 2020).
  2. ^ If one conceives affect as a knowable and measurable phenomenon, then expression authenticity judgments reflect the ability to detect the emotion being expressed (i.e., it is an objective task, with a correct answer as defined by the researcher). Such a paradigm measures accuracy, i.e., the proximity of the judgment to the target emotion, and precision, i.e., the variability between and within judges and expressions. Alternatively, if one believes that underlying affect is unverifiable, then expression authenticity judgments reflect the perception of different types of emotional displays (i.e., it is a subjective task, considering judgment formation, and variability). Such a paradigm measures only precision.
  3. ^ Readers familiar with emotion theories may view encoding-decoding as reflecting the Basic Emotion Theory (BET) and affect-induction as reflecting the Behavioral Ecology View (BECV). The present terminology restricts our reliance on these accounts to certain elements concerned with the conceptualization of facial cues.

References

Barrett, L. F. (2006). Are emotions natural kinds? Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 1, 28–58.

Google Scholar

Barrett, L. F., Adolphs, R., Marsella, S., Martinez, A. M., and Pollak, S. D. (2019). Emotional expressions reconsidered: challenges to inferring emotion from human facial movements. Psychol. Sci. Public Interest 20, 1–68. doi: 10.1177/1529100619832930

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brysbaert, M., and Stevens, M. (2018). Power analysis and effect size in mixed effects models: a tutorial. J. Cogn. 1:9.

Google Scholar

Buck, R., Powers, S. R., and Hull, K. S. (2017). Measuring emotional and cognitive empathy using dynamic, naturalistic, and spontaneous emotion displays. Emotion 17, 1120–1136. doi: 10.1037/emo0000285

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Calvo, M. G., and Nummenmaa, L. (2015). Perceptual and affective mechanisms in facial expression recognition: an integrative review. Cogn. Emot. 30, 1081–1106. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2015.1049124

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carroll, J. M., and Russell, J. A. (1997). Facial expressions in Hollywood’s portrayal of emotion. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 72, 164–176.

Google Scholar

Coan, J. A., and Allen, J. J. B. (eds). (2007). Handbook of Emotion Elicitation and Assessment. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Cohn, J. F., and Schmidt, K. L. (2004). The timing of facial motion in posed and spontaneous smiles. Int. J. Wavelets Multiresolution Inf. Process. 02, 121–132. doi: 10.1142/s021969130400041x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cowie, R., Douglas-Cowie, E., and Cox, C. (2005). Beyond emotion archetypes: databases for emotion modelling using neural networks. Neural Netw. 18, 371–388. doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2005.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crivelli, C., and Fridlund, A. J. (2018). Facial displays are tools for social influence. Trends Cogn. Sci. 22, 388–399. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.02.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crivelli, C., and Fridlund, A. J. (2019). Inside-out: from basic emotions theory to the behavioral ecology view. J. Nonverbal Behav. 43, 161–194. doi: 10.1007/s10919-019-00294-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crozier, W. R. (2010). The puzzle of blushing. Psychologist 23, 390–393.

Google Scholar

Darwin, C. (1872). The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals. London: John Murray.

Google Scholar

Dawel, A., Wright, L., Irons, J., Dumbleton, R., Palermo, R., O’Kearney, R., et al. (2017). Perceived emotion genuineness: normative ratings for popular facial expression stimuli and the development of perceived-as-genuine and perceived-as-fake sets. Behav. Res. Methods 49, 1539–1562. doi: 10.3758/s13428-016-0813-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dezecache, G., Mercier, H., and Scott-Phillips, T. C. (2013). An evolutionary approach to emotional communication. J. Pragmat. 59, 221–233. doi: 10.1016/j.pragma.2013.06.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dupré, D., Krumhuber, E. G., Küster, D., and McKeown, G. J. (2020). A performance comparison of eight commercially available automatic classifiers for facial affect recognition. PLoS One 15:e0231968. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0231968

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ekman, P. (1997). “Expression or communication about emotion,” in Uniting Psychology and Biology: Integrative Perspectives on Human Development, eds N. L. Segal, G. E. Weisfeld, and C. C. Weisfeld (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 315–338. doi: 10.1037/10242-008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ekman, P. (2003). Emotions Revealed: Recognizing Faces and Feelings to Improve Communication and Emotional Life. New York, NY: Owl Books.

Google Scholar

Ekman, P., and Friesen, W. V. (1971). Constants across cultures in the face and emotion. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 17, 124–129. doi: 10.1037/h0030377

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., and O’Sullivan, M. (1988). Smiles when lying. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54:414. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.3.414

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ekman, P., Levenson, R. W., and Friesen, W. V. (1983). Autonomic nervous system activity distinguishes among emotions. Science 221, 1208–1210. doi: 10.1126/science.6612338

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ekman, P., and O’Sullivan, M. (1991). Who can catch a liar? Am. Psychol. 46, 913–920. doi: 10.1037/0003-066x.46.9.913

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ekman, P., and Rosenberg, E. L. (2005). What the Face Reveals: Basic and Applied Studies of Spontaneous Expression Using the Facial Action Coding System (FACS). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Fernández-Dols, J.-M., and Crivelli, C. (2013). Emotion and expression: naturalistic studies. Emot. Rev. 5, 24–29. doi: 10.1177/1754073912457229

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fiske, A. P. (2020). The lexical fallacy in emotion research: mistaking vernacular words for psychological entities. Psychol. Rev. 127, 95–113. doi: 10.1037/rev0000174

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fridlund, A. J. (1991). Sociality of solitary smiling: potentiation by an implicit audience. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 60, 229–240. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.60.2.229

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gonçalves, A. R., Fernandes, C., Pasion, R., Ferreira-Santos, F., Barbosa, F., and Marques-Teixeira, J. (2018). Effects of age on the identification of emotions in facial expressions: a meta-analysis. PeerJ 6:e5278. doi: 10.7717/peerj.5278

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gosselin, P., Kirouac, G., and Doré, F. Y. (1995). Components and recognition of facial expression in the communication of emotion by actors. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 68, 83–96. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.68.1.83

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gosselin, P., Perron, M., and Beaupré, M. (2010). The voluntary control of facial action units in adults. Emotion 10, 266–271. doi: 10.1037/a0017748

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gunnery, S. D., Hall, J. A., and Ruben, M. A. (2013). The deliberate duchenne smile: individual differences in expressive control. J. Nonverbal Behav. 37, 29–41. doi: 10.1007/s10919-012-0139-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gur, R. C., Sara, R., Hagendoorn, M., Marom, O., Hughett, P., Macy, L., et al. (2002). A method for obtaining 3-dimensional facial expressions and its standardization for use in neurocognitive studies. J. Neurosci. Methods 115, 137–143. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0270(02)00006-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, J. A., Andrzejewski, S. A., and Yopchick, J. E. (2009). Psychosocial correlates of interpersonal sensitivity: a meta-analysis. J. Nonverbal Behav. 33, 149–180. doi: 10.1007/s10919-009-0070-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hess, U., and Kleck, R. E. (1994). The cues decoders use in attempting to differentiate emotion-elicited and posed facial expressions. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 24, 367–381. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2420240306

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hurley, C. M., and Frank, M. G. (2011). Executing facial control during deception situations. J. Nonverbal Behav. 35, 119–131. doi: 10.1007/s10919-010-0102-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Izard, C. E. (1994). Innate and universal facial expressions: evidence from developmental and cross-cultural research. Psychol. Bull. 115, 288–299. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.115.2.288

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Izard, C. E. (2007). Basic emotions, natural kinds, emotion schemas, and a new paradigm. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2, 260–280. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00044.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kagan, J. (2007). What is Emotion? History, Measures, and Meanings. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

Google Scholar

Krumhuber, E. G., Küster, D., Namba, S., Shah, D., and Calvo, M. (2019). Emotion recognition from posed and spontaneous dynamic expressions: human observers versus machine analysis. Emotion doi: 10.1037/emo0000712 [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Krumhuber, E. G., Küster, D., Namba, S., and Skora, L. (2020). Human and machine validation of 14 databases of dynamic facial expressions. Behav. Res. Methods doi: 10.3758/s13428-020-01443-y [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Krumhuber, E. G., and Manstead, A. S. R. (2009). Can Duchenne smiles be feigned? New evidence on felt and false smiles. Emotion 9, 807–820. doi: 10.1037/a0017844

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krumhuber, E. G., Skora, L., Küster, D., and Fou, L. (2017). A review of dynamic datasets for facial expression research. Emot. Rev. 9, 280–292. doi: 10.1177/1754073916670022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levine, T. R., Serota, K. B., Shulman, H., Clare, D. D., Park, H. S., Shaw, A. S., et al. (2011). Sender demeanor: individual differences in sender believability have a powerful impact on deception detection judgments. Hum. Commun. Res. 37, 377–403. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2958.2011.01407.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lilienfeld, S. O., Sauvigné, K. C., Lynn, S. J., Cautin, R. L., Latzman, R. D., and Waldman, I. D. (2015). Fifty psychological and psychiatric terms to avoid: a list of inaccurate, misleading, misused, ambiguous, and logically confused words and phrases. Front. Psychol. 6:1100. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McLellan, T., Johnston, L., Dalrymple-Alford, J., and Porter, R. J. (2010). Sensitivity to genuine versus posed emotion specified in facial displays. Cogn. Emot. 24, 1277–1292. doi: 10.1080/02699930903306181

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, E. J., Krumhuber, E. G., and Dawel, A. (2020). Observers perceive the Duchenne marker as signaling only intensity for sad expressions, not genuine emotion. Emotion doi: 10.1037/emo0000772 [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Namba, S., Makihara, S., Kabir, R. S., Miyatani, M., and Nakao, T. (2016). Spontaneous facial expressions are different from posed facial expressions: morphological properties and dynamic sequences. Curr. Psychol. 36, 593–605. doi: 10.1007/s12144-016-9448-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Orlowska, A. B., Krumhuber, E. G., Rychlowska, M., and Szarota, P. (2018). Dynamics matter: recognition of reward, affiliative, and dominance smiles from dynamic vs. static displays. Front. Psychol. 9:938. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00938

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ortony, A., and Turner, T. J. (1990). What’s basic about basic emotions? Psychol. Rev. 97:315. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.97.3.315

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Quigley, K. S., Lindquist, K. A., and Barrett, L. F. (2013). “Inducing and measuring emotion and affect: tips, tricks, and secrets,” in Handbook of Research Methods in Social and Personality Psychology, eds H. T. Reis and C. M. Judd (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 220–252. doi: 10.1017/cbo9780511996481.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rinn, W. E. (1984). The neuropsychology of facial expression: a review of the neurological and psychological mechanisms for producing facial expressions. Psychol. Bull. 95, 52–77. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.95.1.52

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sauter, D. A., and Fischer, A. H. (2018). Can perceivers recognise emotions from spontaneous expressions? Cogn. Emot. 32, 504–515. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2017.1320978

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scherer, K. R., and Bänziger, T. (2010). “On the use of actor portrayals in research on emotional expression,” in A Blueprint for Affective Computing: A Sourcebook and Manual, eds K. R. Scherer, T. Bänziger, and E. Roesch (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 166–176.

Google Scholar

Shackman, A. J., and Wager, T. D. (2019). The emotional brain: fundamental questions and strategies for future research. Neurosci. Lett. 693, 68–74. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2018.10.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siedlecka, E., and Denson, T. F. (2019). Experimental methods for inducing basic emotions: a qualitative review. Emot. Rev. 11, 87–97. doi: 10.1177/1754073917749016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegel, E. H., Sands, M. K., Van den Noortgate, W., Condon, P., Chang, Y., Dy, J., et al. (2018). Emotion fingerprints or emotion populations? A meta-analytic investigation of autonomic features of emotion categories. Psychol. Bull. 144, 343–393. doi: 10.1037/bul0000128

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, D. L. (2004). Why We Lie: The Evolutionary Roots of Deception and the Unconscious Mind. New York, NY: St. Martin’s Griffin press.

Google Scholar

Soppe, H. J. G. (1988). Age differences in the decoding of affect authenticity and intensity. J. Nonverbal Behav. 12, 107–119. doi: 10.1007/bf00986929

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sorensen, T., Hohenstein, S., and Vasishth, S. (2016). Bayesian linear mixed models using Stan: a tutorial for psychologists, linguists, and cognitive scientists. Quant. Methods Psychol. 12, 175–200. doi: 10.20982/tqmp.12.3.p175

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stanislaw, H., and Todorov, N. (1999). Calculation of signal detection theory measures. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 31, 137–149. doi: 10.3758/bf03207704

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Surakka, V., and Hietanen, J. K. (1998). Facial and emotional reactions to Duchenne and non-Duchenne smiles. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 29, 23–33. doi: 10.1016/s0167-8760(97)00088-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomkins, S. S. (1962). Affect Imagery Consciousness: Volume I: The Positive Affects. New York, NY: Springer publishing company.

Google Scholar

Zloteanu, M. (2020). “Reconsidering facial expressions and deception detection,” in Handbook of Facial Expression of Emotion, eds A. Freitas-Magalhaes and J. Borod (Porto: FEELab Science Books & Leya), 238–284.

Google Scholar

Zloteanu, M., Krumhuber, E. G., and Richardson, D. C. (2018). Detecting genuine and deliberate displays of surprise in static and dynamic faces. Front. Psychol. 9:1184. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zloteanu, M., Krumhuber, E. G., and Richardson, D. C. (2020). Acting surprised: comparing perceptions of different dynamic deliberate expressions. J. Nonverbal Behav. doi: 10.1007/s10919-020-00349-9 [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zuckerman, M., DePaulo, B. M., and Rosenthal, R. (1986). “Humans as deceivers and lie detectors,” in Nonverbal Communication in the Clinical Context, eds P. D. Blanck, R. Buck, and R. Rosenthal (University Park, TX: Pennsylvania State University Press), 13–35.

Google Scholar

Zuckerman, M., Larrance, D. T., Spiegel, N. H., and Klorman, R. (1981). Controlling nonverbal displays: facial expressions and tone of voice. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 17, 506–524. doi: 10.1016/0022-1031(81)90037-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: emotion, facial expressions, genuine, posed and spontaneous, authenticity discrimination

Citation: Zloteanu M and Krumhuber EG (2021) Expression Authenticity: The Role of Genuine and Deliberate Displays in Emotion Perception. Front. Psychol. 11:611248. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.611248

Received: 28 September 2020; Accepted: 21 December 2020;
Published: 14 January 2021.

Edited by:

Marianne Schmid Mast, University of Lausanne, Switzerland

Reviewed by:

Walter Gerbino, University of Trieste, Italy
Steven Grant Greening, University of Manitoba, Canada

Copyright © 2021 Zloteanu and Krumhuber. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Mircea Zloteanu, m.zloteanu@kingston.ac.uk; m.zloteanu@ucl.ac.uk

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.